Next Article in Journal
Vascular Endothelial Growth Factor as a Putative Biomarker of Depression in Asthmatics with Reversible Airway Narrowing
Next Article in Special Issue
Overview of the Development and Use of Akt Inhibitors in Prostate Cancer
Previous Article in Journal
Is Chronic Kidney Disease Affecting the Postoperative Complications of Vitrectomy for Proliferative Diabetic Retinopathy?
Previous Article in Special Issue
Potential Targets Other Than PSMA for Prostate Cancer Theranostics: A Systematic Review
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Prostate-Specific Antigen and Testosterone Levels as Biochemical Indicators of Cognitive Function in Prostate Cancer Survivors and the Role of Diabetes

by
Alicja Popiołek
1,2,*,
Bartosz Brzoszczyk
3,
Piotr Jarzemski
3,
Aleksandra Chyrek-Tomaszewska
1,2,
Radosław Wieczór
2,
Alina Borkowska
1 and
Maciej Bieliński
1
1
Department of Clinical Neuropsychology, Collegium Medicum in Bydgoszcz, Nicolaus Copernicus University in Toruń, 85-067 Bydgoszcz, Poland
2
Department of Internal Diseases, Jan Biziel University Hospital No. 2 in Bydgoszcz, 85-163 Bydgoszcz, Poland
3
Department of Laparoscopic, General, and Oncological Urology, Jan Biziel University Hospital No. 2 in Bydgoszcz, 85-067 Bydgoszcz, Poland
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Clin. Med. 2021, 10(22), 5307; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10225307
Submission received: 17 September 2021 / Revised: 10 November 2021 / Accepted: 12 November 2021 / Published: 15 November 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Novel Therapeutic Targets of Prostate Cancer)

Abstract

:
Prostate cancer (PC) is one of the most common malignancies in men. The increase in the number of PC survivors is associated with many problems including cognitive impairment. Early detection of such problems facilitates timely protective intervention. This study examined the association between prostate-specific antigen (PSA) or testosterone (T) levels and cognitive function in patients undergoing radical prostatectomy. Such a correlation could help identify patient groups at risk of cognitive impairment. Participants underwent clinical (demographic data, medical history, physical examination, and blood analyses) and neuropsychological assessment (cognitive test battery). Preoperative PSA or T levels were not associated with cognitive function. However, long-term follow-up after prostatectomy showed a strong correlation between PSA levels and the results of verbal memory and executive function tests. A trend toward significance was also observed for visuospatial memory. The levels of free T and total T were not correlated with cognitive function. Only the levels of free T after hormonal treatment were significantly correlated with executive functions. Comorbid diabetes affected these correlations. In conclusion, PSA levels at a distant postoperative time and free T level after hormonal treatment may be biomarkers of cognitive function.

1. Introduction

Prostate cancer (PC) is one of the most common malignancies in men [1]. Despite considerable progress in the detection and treatment of PC, it remains the fifth leading cause of death worldwide, with approximately 375,000 deaths per year. PC patients die more often from any cause than the general population [2,3]. Advances in medicine, particularly in screening and in the design of effective treatments, have led to an increase in the population of patients with PC, which requires specific care tailored to each problem. One of the issues that PC survivors have to face is cancer-related cognitive impairment. Deterioration of mental function occurs in patients with breast cancer, ovarian cancer, colorectal cancer, and lymphoma [4], as well as in PC [5,6,7]. It occurs in up to 75% of patients during cancer treatment and in up to 30% of patients prior to any treatment [8]. However, the pathogenetic mechanisms underlying these dysfunctions remain unclear. They seem to be multifactorial and related to both the cancer itself and to treatments such as radiochemotherapy, hormone therapy, and surgery [4,5,9,10]. The probable link of PC and cognitive dysfunction is supported by some biological processes including through abnormal accumulation of proteins, neuroinflammation, oxidative stress, and neuronal cell death. These changes can lead to the breakdown of the blood-brain barrier and promote key pathways for the development of cognitive disorders (such as brain insulin resistance, mitochondrial dysfunction, and deposits of neurotoxic beta-amyloid oligomers, synaptic loss, neuronal dysfunction, and cell death) [11,12,13].
Regardless of the mechanisms underlying their development, cognitive disorders affect the daily life of patients, decreasing quality of life, hindering cooperation, and interfering with the patient’s ability to live independently [4,8,14,15,16,17]. The comprehensive care of cancer survivors should include early assessment of cognitive function because early detection or even prediction of cognitive impairment enables timely protective intervention [16,18]. This may reduce the degree of cognitive loss and promote the maintenance of cognitive performance, which improves quality of life [19].
The development of cognitive disorders in cancer survivors is associated with age (the prevalence increases with age), genetic polymorphisms, psycho-social components, stress, depression, anxiety, or other mood disorders related to the disease [4,5,9,10].
This study examined the potential association of biochemical parameters that are routinely measured in PC, such as prostate-specific antigen (PSA) or testosterone, with cognitive function. The existence of such a dependency could help identify patients at risk of cognitive impairment in PC.
PSA is the biomarker recommended by clinical practice guidelines for the early detection of PC to rule-in patients for prostate biopsy referral, for PC surveillance, and for therapeutic monitoring [20]. Preliminary studies show an association between PSA level and cognitive decline [13,21]. Sternberg et al. suggested the possibility of using PSA as a serum biomarker of cognitive function [21].
Testosterone is a hormone that is routinely measured in PC. Its deficiency plays an important role in cognitive function impairment [22]. Androgens (testosterone among them) are closely related to beta amyloid. Their reduced levels induce beta amyloid accumulation in a brain and impair hippocampal neurons, causing cognitive difficulties [12].
Here, we evaluated the correlation of PSA and testosterone levels with cognitive function in PC survivors treated with radical prostatectomy and identified factors that could influence this correlation.

2. Materials and Methods

The study was carried out in The Department of Laparoscopic, General, and Oncological Urology of Jan Biziel University Hospital No. 2 in Bydgoszcz in Poland between July 2017 and June 2018. The study group comprised 118 men with PC who underwent laparoscopic radical prostatectomy. All patients were Polish and Caucasian. The mean age of the cohort was 65 years (range, 48–77 years). Patients were evaluated once postoperatively after an average of 26 months (range, 3–102 months). The inclusion criteria were as follows: histopathologically confirmed diagnosis of prostate adenocarcinoma and subsequent treatment with radical prostatectomy, the ability to understand the purpose of the study, and the willingness to participate in the study. The exclusion criteria were severe somatic, psychiatric, or neurological disorders.
All respondents underwent clinical and neuropsychological assessment.
Clinical assessment included personal data (age, ethnicity, education, physical activity, body mass index, and smoking status) and medical history (data about the course of the disease, PC Grade Group (GG) according to the International Society of Urological Pathology classification [23], date of prostatectomy, adjuvant therapy including radiotherapy and hormone therapy called androgen deprivation therapy (ADT), and comorbidities such as diabetes, hypertension, myocardial infarction, and stroke). Peripheral blood tests were also performed. The following parameters were determined: free and total testosterone levels and total PSA measured by enzyme immunoassay (the PSA score before prostatectomy, at week 6 after surgery, and at the time of the study, called current PSA, were used for data analysis), leukocyte levels, and C-reactive protein.
Neuropsychological assessment included assessment of cognitive function using the computer-based test Neurotest (Table 1) [24].

Statistical Analysis

The distribution of the parameters of the study group was examined using the Shapiro–Wilk test and a significant deviation from the normal distribution was found. Statistical significance of differences between the groups was tested using the Mann–Whitney U test. The analysis of the significance of the correlation was tested using the R-Spearman correlation test. Analysis of covariance was used to investigate the effects of interactions (ANCOVA). The size of the effect is determined with Cohen’s d. A multiple regression model was used to perform multivariate analyses. Statistica 13.0 was used for statistical analysis.

3. Results

In the first stage of the analysis, the study group was classified according to a biochemical indicator of the effectiveness of the surgical procedure, namely a PSA level of <0.1 ng/mL. The study group was divided into those who achieved and those who did not achieve this result. Comparison of the clinicopathological and demographic characteristics of the two groups (Table 2) showed that the group with PSA > 0.1 ng/mL at 6 weeks postoperatively had a lower incidence of arterial hypertension and higher rates of disease severity before surgery according to GG classification. Analyses of biochemical parameters (Table S1) showed that the group with a persistent increase in PSA levels had higher baseline PSA levels before therapy, as well as higher levels of PSA at the time of the study. Analysis of cognitive parameters (Table S2) showed that this group also had a significantly lower number of remembered words in further attempts of the Verbal Memory (VM) test. There was also a trend toward longer response times in the Visuospatial Working Memory Task (VWMT).
The PSA levels before the procedure, at 6 weeks after the procedure, and the current levels were analyzed (Table 3). Preoperative PSA levels did not correlate with the results of cognitive tests. Postoperative PSA levels were correlated only with the lower score in the first attempt of the VM test. The current PSA level was positively correlated with a longer response time in the simple reaction test and with a lower number of remembered words in all VM tests and deferred memory trials. Longer response times in the GoNoGo test and VWMT were associated with higher PSA levels at the time of the study. There were no significant correlations between the level of total and free testosterone and the results of cognitive tests in the entire cohort (Table S3).
The correlation between biochemical parameters and the results of cognitive tests was analyzed in subgroups of patients with arterial hypertension, stroke, myocardial infarction, and diabetes. Correlations were only detected in the subgroup of diabetic patients (n = 19), in which numerous and strong correlations between the current PSA level and the results of cognitive tests were observed (Table 4). After excluding diabetic patients, there was no correlation between the current PSA level and the results of cognitive tests in the remaining patients.
In the subgroup of diabetic patients, those with higher levels of free testosterone had a higher number of correct GoNoGo test responses and fewer incorrect Go responses in this test (Table S4).
Because part of the study population underwent hormone therapy, we analyzed the correlation between biochemical parameters and cognitive function in subgroups of patients with and without hormone therapy. In patients who received hormone therapy (Table 5), the level of free testosterone was significantly associated with a greater number of remembered words in the VM deferred test, a greater number of correct answers in the GoNoGo test, and a lower number of incorrect Go and incorrect NoGo responses. Analysis of the correlation between current PSA levels and cognitive outcomes in the same groups showed that in both groups, a higher level of current PSA was associated with some cognitive parameters (Table 5).
To improve the accuracy of the analyses, we assessed whether the group of patients with diabetes differed from other patients (Table S5). Patients with diabetes were characterized by significantly lower levels of physical activity and significantly lower levels of total testosterone. In the cognitive tests, diabetics had longer reaction times in the simple reaction test and made significantly more incorrect NoGo responses in the GoNoGo test (Table S6).
A summary analysis of the covariance of factors determining the parameters of neuropsychological tests was also performed. This confirmed the significance of the studied biochemical and clinical factors, such as diabetes, in a cognitive context (Table 6). To confirm the obtained results, a multiple regression model was used, which showed a significant share of the current PSA level in almost all the results of cognitive tests. Other important factors were age, diabetes, and one-time post-surgery PSA (Table S7).

4. Discussion

4.1. Prostate-Specific Antigen

PSA belongs to the serin protease family and together with other clinical tools (digital rectal examination, family history of PC, age, and magnetic resonance imaging) is recommended by clinical practice guidelines for the early detection of PC and for therapeutic monitoring [25]. In this study, PSA levels before prostatectomy did not correlate with cognitive function (Table 3). Postoperative PSA levels correlated only with the lower score in the first attempt of the VM test. However, when PSA levels were measured at longer times after prostatectomy, there was a significant relationship between PSA and cognitive functions including reaction time and VM (Table 3). This is consistent with the literature, and suggests that PSA could be used as a marker of cognitive function [21]. Sternberg et al. compared PSA levels between patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD), mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and a control group. The results showed a significant association between cognitive status and PSA levels [21]. A similar study was performed by Lin et al. in a large cohort; these authors observed that patients with AD not only had increased PSA levels, but also an increased risk of PC compared with controls [13]. In studies that did not include PSA level, but only the diagnosis of PC and its treatment, neither PC itself nor the use of hormonal treatment was associated with MCI [26].
The above studies were limited to the overall assessment of cognitive function or the overall diagnosis of cognitive disorders, and did not assess individual domains. In the present study, PSA levels were associated with almost all cognitive domains, especially reaction time and VM. In addition, visuospatial memory was associated with PSA levels in the present cohort (Table 3).
The mechanisms underlying these associations are not clear. Given that PSA is present in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), it can be hypothesized that PSA has a direct effect on brain tissue and brain function [27]. Significant differences in CSF PSA levels were observed between cancer and other prostate diseases [28]. However, in patients with PC, high CSF PSA levels are commonly associated with central nervous system cancer involvement/metastasis [29]. The clinical usefulness of these studies, however, is limited because CSF PSA values are usually too low [28].
The relationship between cognitive function and PSA could be related to the treatment; some studies have reported that cognitive function in PC depends on the type of treatment [30,31]. The role of ADT, which is often used as an adjuvant treatment, is emphasized. Consistently, the cognitive domains that correlate with PSA are the most frequently affected in PC patients receiving ADT [6]. Testosterone deprivation (under the influence of hormonal treatment) could affect both cognitive function and PSA levels [21,32]. However, patients with increased PSA levels after local treatment are at a high risk of subsequent progression and often undergo ADT, which causes a decrease in cognitive disorders [5,33,34]. In this study, the group of patients with persistently elevated PSA after surgery had worse VM than those with normal postoperative PSA levels (<0.1 ng/mL) (Table S2). To exclude the effect of hormonal treatment on the relationship between cognitive presentation and PSA, we performed subgroup analyses. These analyses showed that strong correlations between these variables also occurred in patients who did not receive hormonal treatment, but they concerned other cognitive domains (Table 5).
This study also examined various factors that could modulate PSA levels and affect their correlation with cognitive function. Among the factors that could affect PSA or cognitive function, we analyzed arterial hypertension and history of stroke, myocardial infarction, or diabetes [35,36]. Most of these factors were not correlated with cognitive function except diabetes mellitus. The results are presented in Table 4. The differences concerned all the studied cognitive domains.
Diabetes is an important independent risk factor for cognitive impairment [37]. Elabbady et al. assessed the role of diabetes in prostate-related parameters and observed significant differences in the levels of PSA between patients with and without diabetes [38]. Similarly, Kobayashi et al. reported that, in men with higher blood glucose levels, PSA values were significantly reduced [36]. Diabetes mellitus is thus a factor that influences cognition and can affect PSA.

4.2. Testosterone

Testosterone level is an important factor that affects many aspects of PC. In this study, we investigated the association of serum total testosterone and free testosterone levels with cognitive function in PC survivors and did not find any significant relationships (Table S3). However, literature data on the association of testosterone and its supplementation with cognitive function are not consistent. Some studies have reported significant dependencies [12,39,40], whereas other reports have not [41,42]. A review by Cai and Li demonstrated that low levels of free testosterone and total testosterone are significantly correlated with cognitive decline, as well as with an increased risk of AD in older men [12]. Pintana et al., on the other hand, have remarked that new reports highlight non-significant findings regarding the relationship between testosterone and cognitive function [41]. These incompatibilities can be explained by methodological differences as well as differences in age or ethnicity [41]. Placebo-controlled randomized clinical trials also showed different effects of testosterone supplementation on cognitive function [43].
The treatment of PC also affects testosterone levels. To examine the potential influence of hormonal treatment, we performed separate analyses of cognitive function in men who received ADT and those who did not. In patients receiving hormone therapy, we observed a significant correlation between free testosterone levels and cognitive function, and high testosterone levels were associated with better cognitive performance. These correlations were not observed in the group of people who did not receive ADT (Table 5). Data on the role of ADT in cognitive function are also inconsistent. A beneficial effect of ADT on cognitive status has been reported [44], whereas other studies do not support these observations or report subtle but significant adverse effects of ADT [45]. Andella et al. reviewed publications assessing the cognitive functioning of PC patients treated with ADT. Of the 31 studies included in the analysis, 16 studies found that this therapy had no adverse effects on cognitive function, while another 11 studies found that it negatively affected cognition. In four studies, the results were inconclusive. The authors also noted that future research should focus on further exploring brain features with functional magnetic resonance imaging because this technique may be more sensitive in detecting brain abnormalities in patients treated with ADT [46]. Neuroimaging studies indicate that ADT may cause changes in regional brain metabolism associated with alterations in spatial performance and VM. Brain areas that are affected by androgen deprivation are consistent with those affected in diabetes and in AD, suggesting possible common mechanisms [47]. These changes are located only in brain regions that control specific functions. This may explain why these abnormalities are often elusive in general cognitive function tests, as their detection requires methods aimed at assessing specific cognitive domains [48]. In studies of specific domains, patients treated with ADT perform worse than controls mostly in visuomotor tasks and spatial and VM [43,49,50]. In this study, we observed cognitive decline mostly in executive functions and deferred memory (Table 5).
As described for PSA, we performed additional analyses to identify factors affecting free testosterone levels and cognitive parameters. In patients with diabetes, the level of free testosterone strongly correlated with cognitive control and cognitive inhibition, and a correlation was also observed in the deferred memory test (Table S4). Both diabetes and low testosterone levels are risk factors for cognitive dysfunction [51]. Bertram et al. reported that low testosterone level is a risk factor for both cognitive impairment and the development of diabetes. These findings suggest that the dependencies are complex. German scientists proposed a molecular framework that links diabetes, testosterone, and cognitive impairment [11]. They suggested that the blood-brain barrier can break down because of the inflammatory, oxidative, and metabolic changes in diabetes, which promote the pathological features of dementia. The relationship between testosterone levels, insulin-resistant obesity, and cognitive function was also observed by Pintana et al. [41]. This author proposed a model in which cognitive decline results from either the direct effect of lack of testosterone on the brain or an association between testosterone deficiency and obesity leading to fat tissue accumulation, insulin resistance, and finally cognitive impairment [41].
In conclusion, the present findings suggest that diabetes is associated with the effect of PSA or testosterone levels on cognitive function, supporting that good quality of care in diabetes patients with PC is important. However, the present study was limited by the small number of participants. Additional studies, including molecular studies, are needed to confirm and better understand the observed phenomena and their causes.

5. Conclusions

Persistently elevated PSA levels following prostatectomy and free testosterone levels after hormonal treatment are potential biomarkers of cognitive function that are related mainly to VM and executive function. Concomitant diabetes affects the correlation between PSA levels and cognitive abilities, as well as that between testosterone level and cognitive function.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/jcm10225307/s1. Table S1: Biochemical parameters in the study group and in subgroups with and without persistently elevated PSA levels. Table S2: Cognitive parameters in the study group and in subgroups with and without persistently elevated PSA levels. Table S3: R-Spearman correlations of cognitive test results and free/total testosterone level. Table S4: R-Spearman correlations of cognitive test results and free testosterone level in subgroups with and without diabetes. Table S5: Demographic and clinical data in non-diabetes and diabetes groups. Table S6: Cognitive parameters in non-diabetes and diabetes groups. Table S7: Multiple regression model coefficients on cognitive test results.

Author Contributions

Author Contributions: Conceptualization—P.J., B.B., A.P., A.C.-T., M.B., and A.B.; methodology, A.P., P.J., B.B., and M.B.; formal analysis, A.P., A.C.-T., and M.B.; investigation, P.J. and B.B.; data curation, A.P., B.B., and A.C.-T.; writing—original draft preparation, A.P., B.B., A.C.-T., and R.W.; writing—review and editing—R.W., P.J., M.B., and A.B.; Visualization, M.B.; supervision, M.B. and A.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Bioethics Committee of the Nicolaus Copernicus University, Collegium Medicum in Bydgoszcz (Approval No. 476/2017).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data available on request due to ethical policy.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Rawla, P. Epidemiology of Prostate Cancer. World J. Oncol. 2019, 10, 63–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Sung, H.; Ferlay, J.; Siegel, R.L.; Laversanne, M.; Soerjomataram, I.; Jemal, A.; Bray, F. Global Cancer Statistics 2020: Globocan estimates of incidence and mortality worldwide for 36 cancers in 185 countries. CA Cancer J. Clin. 2021, 71, 209–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Patasius, A.; Smailyte, G. All-Cause Mortality Risk in National Prostate Cancer Cohort: An Impact of Population-Based Prostate Cancer Screening. J. Clin. Med. 2021, 10, 2459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Lange, M.; Joly, F.; Vardy, J.; Ahles, T.; Dubois, M.; Tron, L.; Winocur, G.; De Ruiter, M.; Castel, H. Cancer-related cognitive impairment: An update on state of the art, detection, and management strategies in cancer survivors. Ann. Oncol. 2019, 30, 1925–1940. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Ryan, C.; Wefel, J.S.; Morgans, A.K. A review of prostate cancer treatment impact on the CNS and cognitive function. Prostate Cancer Prostatic Dis. 2019, 23, 207–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Treanor, C.; Li, J.; Donnelly, M. Cognitive impairment among prostate cancer patients: An overview of reviews. Eur. J. Cancer Care 2017, 26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Jarzemski, P.; Brzoszczyk, B.; Popiołek, A.; Stachowicz-Karpińska, A.; Gołota, S.; Bieliński, M.; Borkowska, A. Cognitive function, depression, and anxiety in patients undergoing radical prostatectomy with and without adjuvant treatment. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2019, 15, 819–829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Hardy, S.J.; Krull, K.R.; Wefel, J.S.; Janelsins, M. Cognitive Changes in Cancer Survivors. Am. Soc. Clin. Oncol. Educ. Book 2018, 23, 795–806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Cascella, M.; Di Napoli, R.; Carbone, D.; Cuomo, G.F.; Bimonte, S.; Mbolla, B.F.E.; Muzio, M.R. Chemotherapy-related cognitive impairment: Mechanisms, clinical features and research perspectives. Recent. Prog. Med. 2018, 109, 523–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Toh, Y.L.; Mujtaba, J.S.; Bansal, S.; Yeo, A.; Shwe, M.; Lau, A.J.; Chan, A. Prechemotherapy Levels of Plasma Dehydroepiandrosterone and Its Sulfated Form as Predictors of Cancer-Related Cognitive Impairment in Patients with Breast Cancer Receiving Chemotherapy. Pharmacother. J. Hum. Pharmacol. Drug Ther. 2019, 39, 553–563. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Bertram, S.; Brixius, K.; Brinkmann, C. Exercise for the diabetic brain: How physical training may help prevent dementia and Alzheimer’s disease in T2DM patients. Endocrine 2016, 53, 350–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Cai, Z.; Li, H. An Updated Review: Androgens and Cognitive Impairment in Older Men. Front. Endocrinol. 2020, 11, 586909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Lin, H.-C.; Kao, L.-T.; Chung, S.-D.; Huang, C.-C.; Shia, B.-C.; Huang, C.-Y. Alzheimer’s disease is associated with prostate cancer: A population-based study. Oncotarget 2018, 9, 7616–7622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Spratt, D.E.; Shore, N.; Sartor, O.; Rathkopf, D.; Olivier, K. Treating the patient and not just the cancer: Therapeutic burden in prostate cancer. Prostate Cancer Prostatic Dis. 2021, 24, 647–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Burke, M.A.; Lowrance, W.; Perczek, R. Emotional and cognitive burden of prostate cancer. Urol. Clin. 2003, 30, 295–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Chung, N.-C.; Walker, A.K.; Dhillon, H.M.; Vardy, J.L. Mechanisms and Treatment for Cancer-and Chemotherapy-Related Cog-nitive Impairment in Survivors of Non-CNS Malignancies. Oncology 2018, 32, 591–598. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  17. Janelsins, M.C.; Kesler, S.R.; Ahles, T.A.; Morrow, G.R. Prevalence, mechanisms, and management of cancer-related cognitive impairment. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 2014, 26, 102–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Cui, M.Y.; Lin, Y.; Sheng, J.Y.; Zhang, X.; Cui, R.J. Exercise Intervention Associated with Cognitive Improvement in Alzheimer’s Disease. Neural Plast. 2018, 2018, 9234105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Leidi-Maimone, B.; Notter-Bielser, M.-L.; Laouadi, M.-H.; Perrin, S.; Métraux, H.; Damian, D.; Chavan, C.F.; Nsir, M.; Cibelli, G.; Tâche, M.-J.; et al. How non-drug interventions affect the quality of life of patients suffering from progressive cognitive decline and their main caregiver. Aging 2020, 12, 10754–10771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Ferraro, S.; Bussetti, M.; Bassani, N.; Rossi, R.; Incarbone, G.; Bianchi, F.; Maggioni, M.; Runza, L.; Ceriotti, F.; Panteghini, M. Definition of Outcome-Based Prostate-Specific Antigen (PSA) Thresholds for Advanced Prostate Cancer Risk Prediction. Cancers 2021, 13, 3381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Sternberg, Z.; Podolsky, R.; Nir, A.; Yu, J.; Nir, R.; Halvorsen, S.W.; Chadha, K.; Quinn, J.F.; Kaye, J.; Kolb, C. Increased free prostate specific antigen serum levels in Alzheimer’s disease, correlation with Cognitive Decline. J. Neurol. Sci. 2019, 400, 188–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Giagulli, V.A.; Guastamacchia, E.; Licchelli, B.; Triggiani, V. Serum Testosterone and Cognitive Function in Ageing Male: Updating the Evidence. Recent Pat. Endocr. Metab. Immune Drug Discov. 2016, 10, 22–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. van Leenders, G.J.; van der Kwast, T.H.; Grignon, D.J.; Evans, A.J.; Kristiansen, G.; Kweldam, C.F.; Litjens, G.; McKenney, J.K.; Melamed, J.; Mottet, N.; et al. The 2019 International Society of Urological Pathology (ISUP) Consensus Conference on Grading of Prostatic Carcinoma. Am. J. Surg. Pathol. 2020, 44, e87–e99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Guenter, W.; Bieliński, M.; Bonek, R.; Borkowska, A. Neurochemical Changes in the Brain and Neuropsychiatric Symptoms in Clinically Isolated Syndrome. J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 3909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Ferraro, S.; Bussetti, M.; Panteghini, M. Serum Prostate-Specific Antigen Testing for Early Detection of Prostate Cancer: Managing the Gap between Clinical and Laboratory Practice. Clin. Chem. 2021, 67, 602–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Quiñones, H.J.A.; Stish, B.J.; Hagen, C.; Petersen, R.C.; Mielke, M.M. Prostate Cancer, Use of Androgen Deprivation Therapy, and Cognitive Impairment: A Population-Based Study. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 2020, 34, 118–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Orphanos, G.; Ioannidis, G.; Michael, M.; Kitrou, G. Prostate-specific antigen in the cerebrospinal fluid: A marker of local disease. Med. Oncol. 2008, 26, 143–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Sahin, H.; Aflay, U.; Batun, S.; Bircan, M.K. Cerebrospinal fluid prostate specific antigen (CSF PSA) in prostate cancer patients with lower spine metastasis. Int. Urol. Nephrol. 2005, 37, 727–730. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Schaller, B.; Merlo, A.; Kirsch, E.; Lehmann, K.; Huber, P.R.; Lyrer, P.; Steck, A.J.; Gratzl, O. Prostate-specific antigen in the cerebrospinal fluid leads to diagnosis of solitary cauda equina metastasis: A unique case report and review of the literature. Br. J. Cancer 1998, 77, 2386–2389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Jim, H.S.L.; Small, B.J.; Patterson, S.; Salup, R.; Jacobsen, P.B. Cognitive impairment in men treated with luteinizing hormone–releasing hormone agonists for prostate cancer: A controlled comparison. Support. Care Cancer 2009, 18, 21–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Yiannopoulou, K.G.; Anastasiou, A.I.; Kontoangelos, K.; Papageorgiou, C.; Anastasiou, I.P. Cognitive and Psychological Impacts of Different Treatment Options for Prostate Cancer: A Critical Analysis. Curr. Urol. 2020, 14, 169–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Zarghami, N.; Grass, L.; Diamandis, E. Steroid hormone regulation of prostate-specific antigen gene expression in breast cancer. Br. J. Cancer 1997, 75, 579–588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Soo, A.; O’Callaghan, M.E.; Kopsaftis, T.; Vatandoust, S.; Moretti, K.; Kichenadasse, G. PSA response to antiandrogen withdrawal: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Prostate Cancer Prostatic Dis. 2021, 24, 826–836. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Saad, F.; Bögemann, M.; Suzuki, K.; Shore, N. Treatment of nonmetastatic castration-resistant prostate cancer: Focus on second-generation androgen receptor inhibitors. Prostate Cancer Prostatic Dis. 2021, 24, 323–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Tarantino, G.; Crocetto, F.; Di Vito, C.; Martino, R.; Pandolfo, S.D.; Creta, M.; Aveta, A.; Buonerba, C.; Imbimbo, C. Clinical factors affecting prostate-specific antigen levels in prostate cancer patients undergoing radical prostatectomy: A retrospective study. Future Sci. OA 2021, 7, FSO643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Kobayashi, M.; Mizuno, T.; Yuki, H.; Kambara, T.; Betsunoh, H.; Nukui, A.; Abe, H.; Fukabori, Y.; Yashi, M.; Kamai, T. Association between serum prostate-specific antigen level and diabetes, obesity, hypertension, and the laboratory parameters related to glucose tolerance, hepatic function, and lipid profile: Implications for modification of prostate-specific antigen threshold. Int. J. Clin. Oncol. 2020, 25, 472–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Zilliox, L.A.; Chadrasekaran, K.; Kwan, J.Y.; Russell, J.W. Diabetes and Cognitive Impairment. Curr. Diabetes Rep. 2016, 16, 87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Elabbady, A.; Hashad, M.M.; Kotb, A.; Ghanem, A.E. Studying the effect of type 2 diabetes mellitus on prostate-related parameters: A prospective single institutional study. Prostate Int. 2016, 4, 156–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Cherrier, M.M.; Matsumoto, A.M.; Amory, J.K.; Asthana, S.; Bremner, W.; Peskind, E.R.; Raskind, M.A.; Craft, S. Testosterone improves spatial memory in men with Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 2005, 64, 2063–2068. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Okamoto, K.; Sekine, Y.; Nomura, M.; Koike, H.; Matsui, H.; Shibata, Y.; Ito, K.; Suzuki, K. Effects of a luteinizing hormone-releasing hormone agonist on cognitive, sexual, and hormonal functions in patients with prostate cancer: Relationship with testicular and adrenal androgen levels. Basic Clin. Androl. 2015, 25, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Pintana, H.; Chattipakorn, N.; Chattipakorn, S. Testosterone deficiency, insulin-resistant obesity and cognitive function. Metab. Brain Dis. 2015, 30, 853–876. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Matsumoto, A.M. Testosterone Replacement in Men with Age-Related Low Testosterone: What Did We Learn from the Testosterone Trials? Curr. Opin. Endocr. Metab. Res. 2019, 6, 34–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Jamadar, R.J.; Winters, M.J.; Maki, P.M. Cognitive changes associated with ADT: A review of the literature. Asian J. Androl. 2012, 14, 232–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Cherrier, M.; Rose, A.; Higano, C. The effects of combined androgen blockade on cognitive function during the first cycle of intermittent androgen suppression in patients with prostate cancer. J. Urol. 2003, 170, 1808–1811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Nelson, C.J.; Bs, J.S.L.; Ba, M.C.G.; Roth, A.J. Cognitive effects of hormone therapy in men with prostate cancer. Cancer 2008, 113, 1097–1106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Andela, C.D.; Matte, R.; Jazet, I.M.; Zonneveld, W.C.; Schoones, J.W.; Meinders, A.E. Effect of androgen deprivation therapy on cognitive functioning in men with prostate cancer: A systematic review. Int. J. Urol. 2021, 28, 786–798. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Cherrier, M.M.; Cross, D.J.; Higano, C.S.; Minoshima, S. Changes in cerebral metabolic activity in men undergoing androgen deprivation therapy for non-metastatic prostate cancer. Prostate Cancer Prostatic Dis. 2018, 21, 394–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Chao, H.H.; Uchio, E.; Zhang, S.; Hu, S.; Bednarski, S.R.; Luo, X.; Rose, M.; Concato, J.; Li, C.-S.R. Effects of androgen deprivation on brain function in prostate cancer patients–a prospective observational cohort analysis. BMC Cancer 2012, 12, 371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  49. McGinty, H.L.; Phillips, K.M.; Jim, H.S.L.; Cessna, J.M.; Asvat, Y.; Cases, M.; Small, B.; Jacobsen, P.B. Cognitive functioning in men receiving androgen deprivation therapy for prostate cancer: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Support. Care Cancer 2014, 22, 2271–2280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Salminen, E.K.; Portin, R.I.; Koskinen, A.; Helenius, H.; Nurmi, M. Associations between serum testosterone fall and cognitive function in prostate cancer patients. Clin. Cancer Res. 2004, 10, 7575–7582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Etgen, T.; Sander, D.; Bickel, H.; Förstl, H. Mild cognitive impairment and dementia. Dtsch. Aerztebl. Int. 2011, 108, 743–750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. The computer-based test battery (Neurotest) components.
Table 1. The computer-based test battery (Neurotest) components.
TestPatient’s TaskEvaluated Parameters
Simple reaction time test (SRT)to press the button after seeing a green circle appearing on the computer screen; the stimulus is presented five times, and the number of correct answers and the average response time (ms) are measuredspeed and correctness of reactions to stimuli; general vigilance and psychomotor speed
Verbal memory test (VM test)to remember as many words as possible from the list of 10 words read by a researcher five times; the patient has to recall words in any order after each reading and 20 min later; the number of correctly repeated words, the number of words outside the list and the number of repetitions are counted (for each attempt)efficiency of the working memory (VM1), short-term memory (VM2, VM3, VM4, and VM5, VM1–VM5 are successive attempts when patients recall the memorized words during the test), and deferred memory (verbal memory deferred test; VMDT), immediate auditory memory (number of words saved), learning (improvement of results in subsequent repetitions), and deferred memory (remembering repeating words)
GoNoGo testto press a key when a green square appears on the computer screen (“Go” part) and to refrain from pressing a key when a blue square appears on the screen (“NoGo” part); stimuli are presented in a random manner; the time (ms) of correct “Go” reactions and the number and percentage of correct and incorrect “Go” and “NoGo” reactions are listedresponse time under the conditions of the need to control reactions–actions and inhibitions (cognitive control and cognitive inhibition); executive functions
Visuospatial working memory task (VWMT)to remember the layout of the seven playing cards that were previously presented in different places on the monitor screenvisuospatial memory—correct and incorrect answer and time of reaction
Table 2. Demographic and clinical data in all group and in subgroups with and without persistently elevated PSA levels.
Table 2. Demographic and clinical data in all group and in subgroups with and without persistently elevated PSA levels.
ParameterAll
n = 118
Postoperative PSA < 0.1 ng/mL
(n = 97)
Postoperative PSA > 0.1 ng/mL
(n = 21)
d-Cohenp
Age (y)66.0
(60–70)
66.0
(60.0–70.0)
67.0
(65.0–71.0)
0.820.29
BMI (kg/m2)27.2
(25.6–29.7)
26.8
(25.4–29.7)
27.5
(26.0–29.0)
0.160.54
Months from surgery (m)19.0
(13.0–33.0)
19.0
(14.0–33.0)
23.0
(12.0–33.0)
0.030.70
Diabetes (n, %)19 (16%)14 (14.5%)5 (24%) 0.50
Hypertension (n, %)65 (55%)59 (61%)6 (28.5%)0.02
MI (n, %)10 (8.5%)8 (8%)2 (9.5%)0.92
Stroke (n, %)7 (6%)7 (7%)0 (0%)0.60
EducationBasic (n, %)6 (5%)5 (5%)1 (5%)0.31
Vocational (n, %)33 (28%)27 (28%)6 (28.5%)
Secondary (n, %)36 (30.5%)26 (27%)10 (47.5%)
Higher (n, %)43 (36.5%)39 (40%)4 (19%)
Physical activityNone (n, %)40 (34%)37 (38%)3 (14%)0.09
<1x/week (n, %)24 (20.5%)17 (17.5%)7 (33.5%)
<3x/week54 (45.5%)43 (44.5%)11 (52.5%)
GRADE1 (n, %)70 (59.5%)63 (65%)7 (33.5%)0.005
2 (n, %)35 (29.5%)27 (28%)8 (38%)
3 (n, %)5 (4%)5 (5%)0 (0.0%)
4 (n, %)4 (3.5%)1 (1%)3 (14.25%)
5 (n, %)4 (3.5%)1 (1%)3 (14.25%)
Nicotinism (n, %)53 (45%)44 (45%)9 (43%)0.86
Data are shown as the median (Q25–Q75) or number (%). Inter-group differences were assessed using the Mann–Whitney U test. Effect size was measured using the Cohen d method. BMI—body mass index; MI—myocardial infarction; GRADE: group of grading system classification; y—years; m—months. Significant p-values shown in bold.
Table 3. R-Spearman correlations of cognitive test results and PSA pre, post, and current.
Table 3. R-Spearman correlations of cognitive test results and PSA pre, post, and current.
ParameterPreoperative PSApPost-Surgery PSApCurrent PSAp
SRT_C0.062ns.0.019ns.0.016ns.
SRT_RT−0.118ns.0.043ns.0.2490.007
VM_10.003ns.−0.2140.017−0.1930.036
VM_20.114ns.−0.049ns.−0.2350.01
VM_30.055ns.−0.017ns.−0.2180.017
VM_4−0.092ns.0.080ns.−0.2660.003
VM_5−0.098ns.0.078ns.−0.2670.003
VMDT_C−0.123ns.−0.084ns.−0.1870.047
GoNoGo_C0.116ns.0.017ns.−0.156ns.
GoNoGo_RT0.057ns.−0.048ns.−0.1940.035
GoNoGo IncGO−0.132ns.−0.069ns.0.131ns.
GoNoGo IncNoGo0.087ns.−0.016ns.0.108ns.
VWMT_C−0.061ns.−0.039ns.0.131ns.
VWMT_CRT0.023ns.−0.029ns.0.2240.014
VWMT_IRT0.060ns.−0.039ns.0.2440.007
SRT_C—simple reaction time test (number of correct answers); SRT_RT—simple reaction time test (average reaction time); VM_1–VM_5—verbal memory (number of words remembered in each of the five attempts); VMDT—verbal memory deferred test (number of words remembered); GoNoGo_C—GoNoGo test (number of correct answers); RT—reaction time; IncGo—number of incorrect Go answers; IncNoGo—incorrect NoGo answers; VWMT_C—visuospatial working memory task (number of correct answers); CRT—average response time for correct answers; IRT—average response time for incorrect answers. PSA—prostate-specific antigen. Significant p-values shown in bold.
Table 4. R-Spearman correlations of cognitive test results and current PSA level in subgroups with and without diabetes.
Table 4. R-Spearman correlations of cognitive test results and current PSA level in subgroups with and without diabetes.
ParameterCurrent PSA in NONdiabetes GrouppCurrent PSA in Diabetes Groupp
SRT_C0.044846ns.0.003714ns.
SRT_RT−0.119912ns.0.7034960.0007
VM_10.048692ns.−0.5107030.025
VM_20.089762ns.−0.6160010.005
VM_3−0.030882ns.−0.5584310.012
VM_4−0.066766ns.−0.6151730.005
VM_5−0.009472ns.−0.6253630.004
VMDT_C0.175397ns.−0.4749870.03
GoNoGo_C0.069103ns.−0.231986ns.
GoNoGo_RT0.117830ns.−0.4867850.03
GoNoGo IncGO−0.054729ns.0.311612ns.
GoNoGo IncNoGo−0.048036ns.0.211985ns.
VWMT_C0.175397ns.−0.4589710.048
VWMT_CRT−0.113569ns.0.7898220.00005
VWMT_IRT−0.083593ns.0.7395360.0002
SRT_C—simple reaction time test (number of correct answers); SRT_RT—simple reaction time test (average reaction time); VM_1–VM_5—verbal memory (number of words remembered in each of the five attempts); VMDT—verbal memory deferred test (number of words remembered); GoNoGo_C—GoNoGo test (number of correct answers); RT—reaction time; IncGo—number of incorrect Go answers; IncNoGo—incorrect NoGo answers; VWMT_C—visuospatial working memory task (number of correct answers); CRT—average response time for correct answers; IRT—average response time for incorrect answers; PSA—prostate-specific antigen. Significant p-values shown in bold.
Table 5. R-Spearman correlations of cognitive test and free testosterone in group treated and untreated by hormone therapy.
Table 5. R-Spearman correlations of cognitive test and free testosterone in group treated and untreated by hormone therapy.
ParameterFree TestosteroneCurrent PSA
No Hormone Therapy Group
(n = 104)
pHormone Therapy Group
(n = 14)
pNo Hormone Therapy Group
(n = 14)
pHormone Therapy Group
(n = 104)
p
SRT_C0.020066ns.0.012728ns.0.0184537ns.−0.030253ns.
SRT_RT−0.1836240.060.167365ns.0.261116ns.0.001209ns.
VM_1−0.2162140.020.377769ns.−0.236645ns.0.080332ns.
VM_2−0.166072ns.0.311832ns.−0.5425440.04−0.073243ns.
VM_3−0.001777ns.0.148310ns.−0.445146ns.−0.126351ns.
VM_40.022967ns.0.395777ns.−0.336402ns.−0.2184890.02
VM_50.031529ns.0.4936760.07−0.191995ns.−0.2196040.02
VMDT_C0.004462ns.0.6673090.010.077626ns.−0.163046ns.
GoNoGo_C0.066962ns.0.7795790.001−0.090094ns.0.022998ns.
GoNoGo_RT−0.082913ns.0.4853600.07−0.417786ns.0.047521ns.
GoNoGo IncGO−0.065987ns.−0.7118900.0040.090094ns.−0.023911ns.
GoNoGo IncNoGo−0.043903ns.−0.7656540.0010.418701ns.−0.029561ns.
VWMT_C−0.093605ns.0.4873080.070.066111ns.−0.163046ns.
VWMT_CRT0.044880ns.−0.393309ns.0.6816560.0070.2634240.007
VWMT_IRT0.078058ns.−0.178738ns.0.7398300.0020.075388ns.
SRT_C—simple reaction time test (number of correct answers); SRT_RT—simple reaction time test (average reaction time); VM_1–VM_5—verbal memory (number of words remembered in each of the five attempts); VMDT—verbal memory deferred test (number of words remembered); GoNoGo_C—GoNoGo test (number of correct answers); RT—reaction time; IncGo—number of incorrect Go answers; IncNoGo—incorrect NoGo answers; VWMT_C—visuospatial working memory task (number of correct answers); CRT—average response time for correct answers; IRT—average response time for incorrect answers. Significant p-values shown in bold.
Table 6. Analysis of the covariance (ANCOVA) of the factors determining the parameters of neuropsychological tests.
Table 6. Analysis of the covariance (ANCOVA) of the factors determining the parameters of neuropsychological tests.
AgeDuration from SurgeryPre-Treatment PSAPost-Surgery PSACurrent PSAFree TestosteroneTotal TestosteroneDiabetesGRADEHormone Therapy
WaldpWaldpWaldpWaldpWaldpWaldpWaldpWaldpWaldpWaldp
SRT_C0.80.3426.0<0.00117.2<0.0012.70.0916.2<0.0011.50.2221.8<0.0012.90.0814.50.0023.50.06
SRT_RT0.20.610.010.910.0020.950.50.474.50.030.360.549.10.0010.030.841.270.730.0050.94
VM_13.00.080.050.801.40.231.640.192.080.142.90.080.340.590.560.450.840.830.950.32
VM_20.40.520.640.420.050.810.020.870.89s0.343.410.060.030.845.70.010.950.810.220.64
VM_30.40.500.030.860.570.440.910.331.10.290.010.933.220.078.80.0028.20.040.280.59
VM_42.90.085.80.013.40.060.490.480.930.332.20.130.560.451.350.244.10.250.930.33
VM_52.90.085.70.013.40.060.50.480.90.332.240.130.560.451.30.244.10.250.930.33
VMDT_C19.4<0.00116.5<0.00118.3<0.0010.280.590.120.7215.8<0.0010.560.451.010.3123.3<0.0017.40.006
GoNoGo_C1.220.261.40.222.50.113.80.056.40.010.140.7012.3<0.0010.70.3811.40.0090.50.46
GoNoGo_RT0.020.861.640.22.80.090.20.660.240.620.0060.9310.2<0.0010.540.452.20.520.670.41
GoNoGo IncNoGo4.80.028.50.0035.80.010.20.60.020.8819.9<0.0011.80.17<0.0010.954.70.1911.1<0.001
VWMT_C15.2<0.00112.3<0.0010.980.320.530.460.440.500.280.590.970.323.150.076.780.074.20.04
VWMT_CRT2.640.100.370.541.60.200.140.700.230.625.20.0225.0<0.0011.80.188.10.040.60.41
SRT_C—simple reaction time test (number of correct answers); SRT_RT—simple reaction time test (average reaction time); VM_1–VM_5—verbal memory (number of words remembered in each of the five attempts); VMDT—verbal memory deferred test (number of words remembered); GoNoGo_C—GoNoGo test (number of correct answers); RT—reaction time; IncGo—number of incorrect Go answers; IncNoGo—incorrect NoGo answers; VWMT_C—visuospatial working memory task (number of correct answers); CRT—average response time for correct answers. Significant p-values shown in bold.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Popiołek, A.; Brzoszczyk, B.; Jarzemski, P.; Chyrek-Tomaszewska, A.; Wieczór, R.; Borkowska, A.; Bieliński, M. Prostate-Specific Antigen and Testosterone Levels as Biochemical Indicators of Cognitive Function in Prostate Cancer Survivors and the Role of Diabetes. J. Clin. Med. 2021, 10, 5307. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10225307

AMA Style

Popiołek A, Brzoszczyk B, Jarzemski P, Chyrek-Tomaszewska A, Wieczór R, Borkowska A, Bieliński M. Prostate-Specific Antigen and Testosterone Levels as Biochemical Indicators of Cognitive Function in Prostate Cancer Survivors and the Role of Diabetes. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2021; 10(22):5307. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10225307

Chicago/Turabian Style

Popiołek, Alicja, Bartosz Brzoszczyk, Piotr Jarzemski, Aleksandra Chyrek-Tomaszewska, Radosław Wieczór, Alina Borkowska, and Maciej Bieliński. 2021. "Prostate-Specific Antigen and Testosterone Levels as Biochemical Indicators of Cognitive Function in Prostate Cancer Survivors and the Role of Diabetes" Journal of Clinical Medicine 10, no. 22: 5307. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10225307

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop