Next Article in Journal
Oxytocin Attenuates Yohimbine-Induced Reinstatement of Alcohol-Seeking in Female Rats via the Central Amygdala
Previous Article in Journal
A Follow-Up Study on the Influence of Personal, Family, and School Factors on Learning Outcomes of Students with Disabilities in Senior High School
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Investigating Latent Interactions between Students’ Affective Cognition and Learning Performance: Meta-Analysis of Affective and Cognitive Factors

China Institute of Education Policy, Faculty of Education, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2023, 13(7), 555; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13070555
Submission received: 16 June 2023 / Revised: 29 June 2023 / Accepted: 29 June 2023 / Published: 4 July 2023
(This article belongs to the Section Educational Psychology)

Abstract

:
Affective and cognitive factors play significant roles in influencing students’ learning performance. However, limited studies exist that examine the latent interactions between these factors and students’ learning performance. This study applied a meta-analytic approach to examine the relationships between affective and cognitive factors and students’ learning performance through the selected publications. We identified 18 affective and cognitive influencing factors related to student learning achievement/performance. It was found that academic performance was significantly impacted by learning scores, future aspirations and goals, peer support for learning, and family support for learning. A moderate impact was observed for cognitive benefits, skill development, self-regulation, values, knowledge, character, self-belief, attitudes and beliefs, affective benefits, motivation, optimism, and behavioral engagement. A weak influence was observed for control and relevance of schoolwork and self-efficacy. The discussion and limitations of this study have also been provided in the last sections.

1. Introduction

Affect and cognition are important factors that influence students’ academic performance. According to the theory of aptitude, the measurement of aptitude includes not only cognitive factors (i.e., motivation) but also emotional factors (i.e., emotion) that affect students’ academic performance [1,2,3,4,5,6]. Increasingly, more scholars are paying attention to the influence of students’ emotions on their academic achievement. Cognition and emotion can directly affect students’ academic performance [7,8,9,10]. For example, Bachtiar (2010) integrated emotional factors and cognitive factors in a comprehensive evaluation of cognitive reappraisal ability (CRA). CRA is often used to assess English learning achievement using fuzzy inference. The results suggested that CRA-based FIS is useful for assessing student performance using emotional and cognitive factors to present concrete outcomes. Heikkila et al. (2012) explored the happiness and academic achievement of normal students, focusing on the influence of motivation and emotional factors on students’ learning. The results showed that students with high motivation and high norms (cognitive and emotional factors) had high academic achievement and reported high overall happiness [11,12].
Affective and cognitive factors have been identified as crucial elements that significantly influence students’ learning performance [13,14,15]. However, few studies have examined interactions between affective and cognitive factors and students’ learning performance. This study aims to address the significant problem of exploring the latent interaction between students’ affective cognition and learning performance. The research questions are provided as follows:
Q1: What is the latent interaction between students’ affective cognition and learning performance?
Q2: How can we identify the affective and cognitive influencing factors related to students’ learning achievement/performance?
Therefore, we conducted a meta-analysis to examine relationships between those factors and students’ learning performance. Along with the research questions above, this paper is divided into several sections: Section 1 explores a literature review on the influence of affective and cognitive factors on students’ academic performance and the influence of cognition and emotion on students’ academic performance. The method is provided in Section 2, which includes research methods and data sources, document coding and processing, and a publication bias test. The result sections include overall effect-value analysis, specific effect-value analysis, a heterogeneity test, and publication bias analysis and effect-value analysis. The discussion, limitations, implications, and conclusion are offered in Section 6. The intended audience of this study may focus on exploring the latent interaction between students’ affective cognition and learning performance, as well as identifying the affective and cognitive influencing factors related to students’ learning achievement/performance. Additionally, the intended audience will benefit from examining the latent interactions between these factors and students’ learning performance.

1.1. Literature Review on the Influence of Affective and Cognitive Factors on Students’ Academic Performance

Different scholars offer various approaches to investigating the influence of affective and cognitive factors on students’ academic performance [16]. For example, Kim et al., (2014) used a three-step hierarchical multiple regression method to explore the effects of motivation (i.e., self-efficacy and intrinsic value), math achievement emotion (i.e., anxiety, anger, shame, despair, boredom, enjoyment, and pride), and cognitive processes (i.e., use of cognitive strategies and self-regulation) on students’ math scores. The results showed that motivation accounted for about 13% of the variance in scores. Self-efficacy is an important individual predictor of student achievement. However, when achievement emotion was added to the analysis, self-efficacy did not predict student achievement, and emotion accounted for 37% of the variance in achievement. The use of cognitive strategies and self-regulation did not explain any additional differences in final scores. Eichler and Gradwohl (2021) explored the supportive, emotional, motivational, and cognitive factors that influence the math achievements of the first-year engineering students. The results showed that students’ mathematical ability before the first semester and the tutoring support they received during the first semester were the factors contributing to their success in the final exam of their first year of engineering studies. With the continuous expansion of higher education, the teacher–student ratio is gradually decreasing, and teachers are facing increasing demands for student guidance. This can lead to a weakening of the emotional connections between teachers and students. Loon and Bell (2018) explored the importance of emotions in improving students’ cognitive skills and how emotions interact with knowledge and reflection [17]. Their research results showed that emotions regulated the direct relationship between knowledge and cognitive skills through reflection, as well as the indirect relationship between knowledge and cognitive skills, and that emotions play a crucial role in students’ learning. Sæle et al., (2016) investigated the psychometric properties of a new literacy problem (LP) scale and its impact on grade point average (GPA). Their analysis demonstrated that the scale exhibits good psychometric characteristics. Furthermore, they found that learning trajectory choice, task-solving skills, gender, lack of an education plan, and LP scores predicted GPA in descending order. Using a questionnaire survey, Sayani et al., (2019) investigated students’ perceptions and experiences of emotional, cognitive, and skill development, and assessed whether those perceptions and experiences affected students’ participation in case competitions. The results revealed statistically significant differences in students’ cognitive and emotional gains between perceived learning and actual learning. Moreover, students’ perspectives on learning and real-life experience were significantly and positively correlated with their participation in the case-study competitions. Song and Wang (2021) examined the extent to which disciplinary, cognitive, and affective factors can explain individual differences in students’ interdisciplinary abilities. Multiple linear regression analysis showed that students’ subject knowledge (DK), attitudes toward interdisciplinary approaches, and interdisciplinary learning opportunities were all statistically significant predictors in the added predictive models. The results from semi-structured interviews indicated that students did not demonstrate substantial engineering design knowledge in their responses [4]. Furthermore, the study found that students’ perceptions of interdisciplinary learning influenced their application of subject knowledge (DK) to generate comprehensive insights into various aspects related to possible solutions. When learners have good cognitive and emotional abilities, they will correspondingly achieve good academic performance. However, Suldo et al., (2018) examined the effects of 34 factors on AP and IB student achievement and found that good mental health and academic achievement were associated with higher levels of achievement motivation and cognitive engagement, as well as lower levels of parent–child conflict, stress from major life events, and the use of avoidance coping strategies. Higher levels of emotional engagement, the use of coping methods, and parents who parented with scientific authority were strong predictors of positive mental health outcomes but were not associated with academic outcomes [18,19].

1.2. The Influence of Cognition and Emotion on Students’ Academic Performance in Different Internet Learning Modes

In the era of mobile learning and ubiquitous learning, it is of great practical significance to examine the influence of cognition and emotion on students’ academic performance [20,21]. In one such study, Kongcharoen et al. took online social network (OSN) attention as a moderating factor to study the impact of affective and cognitive factors on learning achievement (LA). The results confirmed that student engagement and LA differed significantly across the whole group, while student engagement, emotional scores, and LA were significantly different in the high OSN attention group. No significant differences were observed between the low OSN attention groups. Furthermore, for students with higher rather than lower OSN attention, academic performance can better promote the impact of emotional and cognitive levels. The flipped classroom model has a significant effect on improving learners’ participation and active learning results. Only content/learning-related outcomes, not general ability or satisfaction were influenced by academic ability and epistemological beliefs. Both pre-class and in-class participation levels had an impact on most types of foreign-language achievement, except for general ability, which was only affected by in-class participation level. Learners’ emotional engagement was not affected by epistemological beliefs but had direct and indirect effects on foreign-language learning outcomes through behavioral and cognitive engagement [22,23,24]. The intervention of factors that affect students’ academic performance through technology can also improve students’ academic performance to a certain extent. Hong et al., (2017) designed a prediction-observation-interpretation-based scientific inquiry learning model and implemented it in an electronic application called “Why”. They analyzed the effects of using “Why” on students’ interest in learning science (ILS), cognitive anxiety (CA), and external cognitive load (ECL) through confirmatory factors of structural equation models. The results demonstrated that students with high ILS exhibited low CA and ECL. Following the utilization of “Why” to establish connections, students experienced a significant improvement in self-confidence, which subsequently led to enhanced academic performance [25,26].

1.3. Impacts on Students’ Achievement using Cognition and Affection as the Components of Students’ Engagement

Cognition and emotion, as components of students’ engagement, have been found to impact academic performance [17,27,28,29,30]. Student participation is a significant factor that influences academic performance and serves as the foundation for dropout prevention and high school reform measures. Several scholars have developed student participation scales that assess cognitive, emotional, and other factors, which indirectly reflect students’ academic performance. The Student Engagement Tool (SET), for example, was designed to measure two components of student engagement: cognitive and emotional engagement. Betts et al. verified in their study that the SET can effectively gauge the cognitive and emotional participation of students at the middle and high school levels. However, many existing student engagement scales primarily measure broader school-level participation rather than specific class- or student-level participation [31].
To gain a deeper understanding and measure student participation at the micro level, Whitney et al., (2019) developed a statistical scale that encompasses three aspects: emotional, behavioral, and cognitive characteristics. Additionally, they demonstrated the scale’s practicability in predicting academic performance, presenting specific results. McGeown et al. created a mental resilience scale for adolescents that includes challenges, commitment, confidence (abilities and interpersonal relationships), and control (life and emotions). The study also found that the psychological resilience attributes of adolescents were associated with their learning motivation and engagement, happiness (depression and anxiety), test anxiety, as well as other emotional, cognitive, and behavioral tendencies, thus impacting their academic performance and mental health. Following the SOR theory of behaviorism, Yang and Cheng (2019) investigated factors that affect the persistence of college students’ mobile learning from two aspects: self-determination needs and learning engagement [32,33,34]. The results showed that perceived learning support, learning self-management, and peer influence significantly influenced affective learning participation and cognitive learning participation. As a result, these factors positively impacted sustained intentions to use mobile learning [35,36,37].

2. Methods

2.1. Research Methods and Data Sources

As a “re-statistics” method, meta-analysis provides a statistically effective approach to literature research, distinct from the qualitative descriptions and summaries typically in literature reviews. Following common meta-analytic methods, we conducted a comprehensive examination of different empirical studies, calculated the average effects, integrated the literature, performed secondary statistical analysis on the data, and obtained the real effects of different influencing factors. In addition, we derived extensive and effective conclusions and implications in the end. In accordance with widely accepted meta-analysis methodology, we performed the following steps: (1) literature collection and screening, focusing on quantitative empirical studies in line with the research topic; (2) literature coding and formulation of coding rules to describe the basic information extracted from the relevant literature; and (3) statistical testing and analysis, calculation of effect size, and heterogeneity analysis.
Because the reliability of meta-analysis results depends on the comprehensiveness and validity of the literature collected, we conducted an extensive search of all the literature published in English between 2000 and 2022. In accordance with the theme of this study, we used the search terms, “education”, “affective and cognitive factors”, and “student”. A total of 592 relevant publications were retrieved from the English database Web of Science, which were then narrowed down to education-related subtopics, resulting in 453 eligible publications.
To ensure a scientific and rational literature-screening process, we specified screening rules to obtain relevant studies that were consistent with our theme and to produce data that could be statistically analyzed. The criteria for literature selection were as follows: (1) The topic of the literature research must be related to the impact of emotional and cognitive factors on students’ academic performance, and the dependent variable must be a relevant measurement index of students’ academic success (e.g., academic performance or personal achievement); (2) the literature must be empirical research, excluding qualitative research results; (3) there can be no duplication in the literature, that is, multiple articles with the same research sample by the same author; (4) studies must report statistical measures of relevant influencing factors, including sample size, Pearson correlation coefficient, or other effect sizes that can be translated into the correlation coefficient (e.g., t-value, p-value, or F-value).
The article selection process consisted of several stages. In the initial stage of searching, we used keywords related to affective and cognitive factors in relation to students’ academic performance. During this stage, 592 publications were selected based on their relevance to research themes. In the second stage of keywords and subtopic screening, among the selected 592 publications, we re-selected the key samples and deleted the non-quantitative studies, and at last, 453 publications were selected to meet the method criteria. In the third stage, we excluded the irrelevant publications that could not report the sample size and accurate statistical indicators. In the content extraction stage, 138 selected samples were selected. Among them, 12 (6.42%) were related to the research topic but did not report data. By the end, we eliminated studies with unclear dependent variables or unclear correlations between dependent variables and students’ academic studies, and a total of 98 publications were finally selected for the meta-analysis (See Figure 1).

2.2. Document Coding and Processing

After selecting eligible publications for the meta-analysis, the collected literature must be coded, which facilitates the identification of subsequent influencing factors, related effect sizes, and sample sizes. In this study, we recorded information including author, title, publication year, sample size, effect size category, effect size value, and influencing factors in each article. Considering that Pearson correlation coefficient has been widely used in the social sciences and is the preferred effect value for meta-analysis in education measurement, we chose the correlation coefficient as the effect size metric for this study. For publications in which no correlation coefficient is reported, but the sample size and t-value, F-value, and χ 2 -value are reported, the correlation coefficient can be uniformly calculated using the following formula, where df is the degree of freedom:
r = t 2 ( t 2 + d f ) = F d f 1 F d f 1 + d f 2 = χ 2 N ( k 1 )
In addition, to distinguish the impact on students’ studies of emotional and cognitive factors, we divided the influencing factors into three categories: (1) emotional, (2) cognitive, and (3) behavioral. Table 1 presents examples of document coding.

2.3. Experimental Design and Data Analysis

In this study, we used R (v4.2.0), a meta-analysis package, to analyze the data. All model parameters were estimated using the restricted maximum likelihood method, and results with a two-tailed p-value less than 0.05 were defined as significant. It was assumed that the studies included in the meta-analysis were independent of each other and that different publications did not influence one another. Among the 98 studies included in the meta-analysis, a total of 93 influencing factors were identified that affected students’ academic performance. Considering that some factors with different names have the same meaning, the variables with similar meanings were uniformly named as the variables that occur most frequently before conducting the actual analysis. After summarizing and combining factors, a total of 42 cognitive and emotional factors that reportedly had an impact on students’ academic performance were retained. Given the need to ensure the reliability of the meta-analysis, this paper provides only the influential factors that were reported in at least in two studies. Table 2 shows the comprehensive effects of some influencing factors on students’ academic performance, as reported in the analyzed literature. The effects reflect the correlation between the factors and students’ academic performance, and whether those effects were positive or negative. Except for anxiety, all factors had a positive effect on students’ academic development. Table 2 also presents the 95% confidence interval of comprehensive correlation coefficients for each influencing factor, along with the significance of the two-tailed test, and Rosenthal’s fail-safe number for publication bias test. Heterogeneity test results for each influencing factor are reported in Table 3.

2.4. Publication Bias Test

Some studies have indicated the presence of publication bias in meta-analyses due to the higher likelihood of publishing studies with statistically significant findings. Publication bias can introduce bias in effect size estimates, ultimately leading to less-accurate results of a meta-analysis. Thus, the true effect of a certain influencing factor cannot be sufficiently estimated. In general, when publication bias is present, the meta-analysis results tend to overestimate the true extent of an impact factor. Qualitative methods, such as funnel plots, can be employed to assess the presence of publication bias. When the funnel plot is basically symmetrical, it indicates that publication bias can be ignored; otherwise, it is necessary to revise the included literature. However, funnel plots are a more subjective judgment method, while Egger’s regression test and Rosenthal’s fail-safe number—used in this study—can be referred to as more objective methods.

3. Results

3.1. Overall Effect-Value Analysis

Figure 2 and Figure 3 show the funnel plots of the effect sizes of the two influencing factors, cognitive dominance, and emotional dominance, respectively. According to the image analysis, the funnel plots were basically symmetrical and, thus, there was no obvious publication bias. However, since the funnel plot is a more subjective judgment method that may not always provide consistent reliability, this study employed a combination of the Egger’s regression test and the assessment of publication bias using Rosenthal’s fail-safe number. The black dots mean the correlated distribution of the selected sample.
Taking studies of the above two influential factors (cognitive and emotional dominance) as an example, under the mixed-effects model, the p-values of Egger’s regression tests were 0.5094 and 0.7392, respectively, indicating that there was insufficient evidence to reject the non-existent publication bias hypothesis. Therefore, it can be concluded that there was no discernible publication bias in studies of those two factors. In studies of most other influential factors, Egger’s test also indicated that no obvious publication bias was present. Egger’s test requires a minimum number of studies that include a certain influential factor, so it is not applicable to a publication bias test that includes only two studies. Therefore, we further investigated the publication bias of influential factors using Rosenthal’s fail-safe number.

3.2. Specific Effect-Value Analysis

Rosenthal’s fail-safe number determines the existence of publication bias by calculating how many missed studies must be added to make statistically significant meta-analysis results statistically insignificant. When the fail-safe number is N < 5k + 10, it indicates that the results of the meta-analysis may be affected by publication bias, and the results of the meta-analysis are therefore unreliable, where k represents the number of studies included in a certain impression factor. Table 2 also lists the publication bias test results for some influencing factors; the test results were calculated using Rosenthal’s fail-safe number. There was no publication bias for most of the influencing factors, and only the fail-safe numbers for Interest, Task Management, and Anxiety were less than 5k + 10, which indicated serious publication bias. The results of the meta-analysis were not convincing and should be rejected in the subsequent analysis.

3.3. Heterogeneity Test and Publication Bias Analysis

Heterogeneity measures the size of variation among studies on the same topic. When the true effects of all selected studies are the same, heterogeneity is 0 and a fixed-effects model should be used. By contrast, if the combined effects of different studies are different, the heterogeneity between studies cannot be ignored, and a random-effects model should be adopted. Most meta-analyses use either a fixed-effects model or a random-effects model. In the case of fixed effects, all studies are assumed to have the same real effect, while random effects allow different studies to have different real effects, and the real effects obey the distribution of some mean. Under different model assumptions, the comprehensive effect estimation methods are different. For fixed-effects models, the weight of each study is the inverse of the variance of each study, and the combined effect is the weighted average of each study.
W i = 1 V Y i
M = i = 1 k W i Y i i = 1 k W i
The random-effects model assumes that the observed effect size consists of the total mean, the deviation between the studied true effect size and the total mean, and the deviation between the observed effect size and the true effect size.
Y i = μ + ζ i + ε i
Thus, for a random-effects model, we need to know not only the variance within the study, but also the variance between the studies. In the present study, the variance between studies was estimated using the moment method. The estimated variance between studies was calculated as:
W * i = 1 V Y i *
V Y i * = V Y i + T 2
M = i = 1 k W i * Y i i = 1 k W i *
To determine which model should be used in our meta-analysis, we first conducted a heterogeneity test. There are many methods for testing heterogeneity, but the most frequently used are Q tests. Using the Q statistic, we calculated the weighted sum of squares of the deviation from the average effect for each study, which followed a chi-square distribution. When the p-value of the Q test was less than 0.05, we determined that there was obvious heterogeneity between studies, and it was more reasonable to adopt the random-effects model; otherwise, the fixed-effects model was adopted. This method was used to judge whether there was heterogeneity in a study that was included because of an influential factor, and how much heterogeneity was needed to be measured statistically.
As illustrated in Table 3, except for the three influential factors, Knowledge, Self-Efficacy, and Interest, the heterogeneity tests of other influential factors all reached the level of significance (p < 0.05), and most of the influential factors exhibiting heterogeneity were greater than 75%. Therefore, we used a random-effects model to estimate the comprehensive effect size of each influential factor, except for Knowledge, Self-Efficacy, and Interest.

3.4. Effect-Value Analysis

According to the results of the heterogeneity test, a random-effects model was used to combine 18 influencing factors with heterogeneity, while a fixed-effects model was used to combine 3 influencing factors without heterogeneity (Knowledge, Self-Efficacy, and Interest). The combined effect is presented in Table 2. Table 2 also presents the 95% confidence interval of the combined effect and the p-value of the two-tailed test, which not only determined the strength of the combined effect of a certain research factor, but also showed the reliability of the estimated results. To classify the degree of influence of different influencing factors on students’ academic performance, we selected the classification criteria for an effect size proposed by J. Cohen: r < 0.1 means basically no correlation, 0.1 ≤ r < 0.3 means weak correlation, 0.3 ≤ r < 0.5 means medium-strength correlation, and r ≥ 0.5 means strong correlation. According to the double-tail test and publication bias results for each influencing factor, three factors (Interest, Task Management, and Anxiety) were excluded, as they failed the publication bias test. Table 4 lists 18 influencing factors that had varying degrees of influence on students’ academic performance.

4. Discussion

This study applied a meta-analytic approach to investigate the relationships between affective and cognitive factors in student learning performance [38,39,40,41,42,43,44]. Our analysis finally identified 18 influencing factors related to student learning achievement/performance. We found that Learning Scores, Future Aspirations and Goals, Peer Support for Learning, and Family Support for Learning had a significant impact on students’ academic performance. Cognitive Benefits, Skill Development, Self-Regulation, Values, Knowledge, Character, Self-Belief, Attitudes and Beliefs, Affective Benefits, Motivation, Optimism, and Behavioral Engagement had a moderate impact on students’ academic performance. Control and Relevance of School Work and Self-Efficacy had a weak influence on students’ performance. Among the factors that significantly impacted students’ academic performance, the cognitive factors comprised Learning Scores and Future Aspirations and Goals, which is consistent with general research and cognition [45,46,47,48]. Academic achievement and personal goals for the future had a significant positive effect on students’ academic achievement. The emotional factors, Peer Support for Learning and Family Support for Learning, had important supporting effects on students’ academic performance. Creating a good peer learning atmosphere and family education atmosphere is of great emotional significance to students’ individual academic development [49,50,51,52,53]. Among the factors that had a moderate impact on student achievement, the cognitive factors were Cognitive Benefits, Skill Development, Self-Regulation, Values, Knowledge, and Character. These cognitive adjustments indicate that they also have a good intervention effect on students’ academic development. Furthermore, Self-Belief, Attitudes and Beliefs, Affective Benefits, Motivation, and Optimism indicated positive effects on students’ academic performance from the aspects of emotion and attitude. By contrast, the cognitive factor, Control and Relevance of School Work, and the emotional factor, Self-Efficacy, had little importance in students’ academic efficacy and were not considered the primary factors for improving students’ academic ability [54,55,56,57].
Many factors affect students’ academic performance, including internal and external motivation, self-efficacy, and self-initiative, among others [58,59,60,61,62,63]. Scholars have also explored the effects of cognitive and emotional components on these factors. For example, Arpaci and Basol (2020) examined the impact of preservice teachers’ cognitive and technological perceptions of their continuous intention to use the flipped classroom model. They found that both self-regulation and self-efficacy have a positive impact on perceived ease of use. The perceived anxiety has a negative impact on self-efficacy, and self-efficacy mediates the relationship between perceived anxiety. They also suggested that there are significant relationships between cognitive and technological factors and continuous intention to use the flipped classroom model. In addition, Hammoudi (2019) examined the relationship between students’ motivation to succeed in an introductory mathematics course (as the dependent variable of the study) offered by a university in the United Arab Emirates and five other independent variables (i.e., cognitive mathematical self-concept, emotional mathematical self-concept, extrinsic motivation such as expectations of future career and income, the age of the students, and the number of mathematics courses taken by the students). The quantitative correlation observed between student motivation and these five variables showed a theoretically consistent interrelationship, while the five independent variables explained 71.3% of the change in students’ motivation for success. Initiative is the intrinsic ability of students to regulate, control, and supervise their own learning [64,65,66,67,68,69].
The effectiveness of learners at regulating their cognitive, emotional, and behavioral processes as they interact in a learning environment is critical to their academic success. Code proposed a theory of learner agency, or agency for learning (AFL), which is an emergent ability to intentionally self-generate and respond to social factors in a learning environment [70,71,72,73,74].Code further explored the predictive validity and psychometric properties of the agency for learning questionnaire (AFLQ), which covers four dimensions of agentic function, including intentionality (planning, decision-making ability), forethought (intrinsic and extrinsic motivation), self-regulation, and self-efficacy. The research results showed that “the AFLQ provides a reliable, valid, multidimensional measure of AFL based on existing theoretical and empirical findings”. Overall, good cognitive ability is generally positively correlated with students’ academic performance, while most studies report that emotional ability also directly affects students’ academic performance. Additionally, students’ emotional ability often does not have a direct impact on their academic performance, but instead affects it indirectly through emotional factors such as attitude, motivation, and sense of efficacy. In the era of the Internet, furthermore, it is also worth exploring how students’ academic performance is affected by the new learning or teaching modes that have emerged online [75,76,77].
The findings of the present study provide a relatively comprehensive estimate of the reported relationships between affective and cognitive factors and students’ learning performance in international empirical studies. The study offers a systematic exploration of the implicit interactions among affective and cognitive factors in students’ learning performance. Moreover, this meta-analysis elucidates teaching research that is focused on the promotion of affective and cognitive factors to improve students’ learning performance.

5. Limitations

Some limitations of the present study should be noted. The literature examining the relationships between affective and cognitive factors and students’ learning performance should include more relevant research. Furthermore, the research topic could be extended to include additional relevant themes, such as internal or external affective and cognitive actions, and implicit or explicit affective and cognitive actions. In addition, we also suggested that future researchers could add more psychology points of view that deepen the interaction between students’ affective cognition and learning performance. For example, in future research, differences and similarities in the relationships between affective and cognitive factors and students’ learning performance should be studied more extensively, together with moderating variables, to derive more convincing universal conclusions.

6. Implications and Conclusions

This study contributes to research that applies a meta-analysis approach to examining relationships between affective and cognitive factors in student learning performance. Of the 18 influencing factors identified, we found that Learning Scores, Future Aspirations and Goals, Peer Support for Learning, and Family Support for Learning have the strongest impact on students’ academic performance. This study applied a meta-analytic approach to examine the relationships between affective and cognitive factors and students’ learning performance through the selected publications. In total, the 18 affective and cognitive influencing factors related to student learning achievement/performance were selected and we found that academic performance was significantly impacted by Learning Scores, Future Aspirations and Goals, Peer Support for Learning, and Family Support for Learning. A moderate impact was observed for Cognitive Benefits, Skill Development, Self-Regulation, Values, Knowledge, Character, Self-Belief, Attitudes and Beliefs, Affective Benefits, Motivation, Optimism, and Behavioral Engagement.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.L. and E.X.; methodology, J.L.; software, C.L.; validation, J.L., E.X. and C.L; formal analysis, J.L.; investigation, E.X.; resources, E.X.; data curation, C.L.; writing—original draft preparation, J.L.; writing—review and editing, J.L.; visualization, Y.H.; supervision, E.X.; project administration, J.L.; funding acquisition, E.X. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by National Social Science Foundation Youth Project in Education “Study on Process Tracking and Effect Evaluation of Policy Implementation of Excellent Teacher Plan in Ministry-affiliated Normal Universities” (Project No.: CIA220282).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Bachtiar, F.A.; Sulistyo, G.H.; Cooper, E.W.; Kamei, K. Fuzzy Inference System Based on a Model of Affective-Cognitive Criteria. In Proceedings of the 2015 IIAI 4th International Congress on Advanced Applied Informatics, Okayama, Japan, 12–16 July 2015; pp. 343–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Heikkilä, A.; Lonka, K.; Nieminen, J.; Niemivirta, M. Relations between teacher students’ approaches to learning, cognitive and attributional strategies, well-being, and study success. High. Educ. 2012, 64, 455–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Kim, C.; Park, S.W.; Cozart, J. Affective and motivational factors of learning in online mathematics courses. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 2014, 45, 171–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Eichler, A.; Gradwohl, J. Investigating Motivational and Cognitive Factors which Impact the Success of Engineering Students. Int. J. Res. Undergrad. Math. Educ. 2021, 7, 417–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Loon, M.; Bell, R. The moderating effects of emotions on cognitive skills. J. Furth. High. Educ. 2018, 42, 694–707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Sæle, R.G.; Sørlie, T.; Nergård-Nilssen, T.; Ottosen, K.-O.; Goll, C.B.; Friborg, O. Demographic and psychological predictors of grade point average (GPA) in North-Norway: A particular analysis of cognitive/school-related and literacy problems. Educ. Psychol. 2016, 36, 1886–1907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Sayani, H.; Shoaib, M.; Mashood, N.; Kumar, V. Case study competitions and perceptions of learning among undergraduate students in the United Arab Emirates. J. Educ. Bus. 2019, 94, 101–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Song, G.; Wang, Z. Factors influencing middle school students’ interdisciplinary competence in science education. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2021, 58, 1041–1072. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Suldo, S.M.; Shaunessy-Dedrick, E.; Ferron, J.; Dedrick, R.F. Predictors of Success Among High School Students in Advanced Placement and International Baccalaureate Programs. Gift. Child Q. 2018, 62, 350–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Kongcharoen, C.; Ghinea, G. Influence of Students’ Affective and Conative Factors on Laboratory Learning: Moderating Effect of Online Social Network Attention. Eurasia J. Math. Sci. Technol. Educ. 2017, 13, 1013–1024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Lee, J.; Park, T.; Davis, R.O. What affects learner engagement in flipped learning and what predicts its outcomes? Br. J. Educ. Technol. 2022, 53, 211–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Hong, J.-C.; Hwang, M.-Y.; Tai, K.-H.; Tsai, C.-R. An Exploration of Students’ Science Learning Interest Related to Their Cognitive Anxiety, Cognitive Load, Self-Confidence and Learning Progress Using Inquiry-Based Learning with an iPad. Res. Sci. Educ. 2017, 47, 1193–1212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Betts, J.E.; Appleton, J.J.; Reschly, A.L.; Christenson, S.L.; Huebner, E.S. A study of the factorial invariance of the Student Engagement Instrument (SEI): Results from middle and high school students. Sch. Psychol. 2010, 25, 84–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Whitney, B.M.; Cheng, Y.; Brodersen, A.S.; Hong, M.R. The Scale of Student Engagement in Statistics: Development and Initial Validation. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 2019, 37, 553–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. McGeown, S.P.; St Clair-Thompson, H.; Putwain, D.W. The Development and Validation of a Mental Toughness Scale for Adolescents. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 2018, 36, 148–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Yang, S.; Zhou, S.; Cheng, X. Why do college students continue to use mobile learning? Learning involvement and self-determination theory. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 2019, 50, 626–637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Beilock, S.L.; Ramirez, G. On the Interplay of Emotion and Cognitive Control: Implications for Enhancing Academic Achievement. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2011; Volume 55, pp. 137–169. [Google Scholar]
  18. Hammoudi, M.M. Predictive factors of students’ motivation to succeed in introductory mathematics courses: Evidence from higher education in the UAE. Int. J. Math. Educ. Sci. Technol. 2019, 50, 647–664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Code, J. Agency for Learning: Intention, Motivation, Self-Efficacy and Self-Regulation. Front. Educ. 2020, 5, 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Arpaci, I.; Basol, G. The impact of preservice teachers’ cognitive and technological perceptions on their continuous intention to use flipped classroom. Educ. Inf. Technol. 2020, 25, 3503–3514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Abu-Hilal, M.M.; Abdelfattah, F.A.; Alshumrani, S.A.; Abduljabbar, A.S.; Marsh, H.W. Construct validity of self-concept in TIMSS’s student background questionnaire: A test of separation and conflation of cognitive and affective dimensions of self-concept among Saudi eighth graders. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 2013, 28, 1201–1220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Al-Sheeb, B.A.; Hamouda, A.M.; Abdella, G.M. Modeling of student academic achievement in engineering education using cognitive and non-cognitive factors. J. Appl. Res. High. Educ. 2019, 11, 178–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Altun, A.; Cakan, M. Undergraduate students’ academic achievement, field dependent/independent cognitive styles and attitude toward computers. J. Educ. Technol. Soc. 2006, 9, 289–297. [Google Scholar]
  24. Anghelache, V. Relation between Non-Cognitive Factors of Learning and the Students’ Academic Performance. In Non-Cognitive Skills and Factors in Educational Attainment; Brill: Leiden, The Netherlands, 2016; pp. 129–150. [Google Scholar]
  25. An, M.; Ma, X.; Wu, H. Medical students’ academic satisfaction: Social cognitive factors matter. Med. Educ. 2023. ahead of print. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Barbier, K.; Struyf, E.; Verschueren, K.; Donche, V. Fostering cognitive and affective-motivational learning outcomes for high-ability students in mixed-ability elementary classrooms: A systematic review. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 2023, 38, 83–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Bloye, E.J. Vocational Interest and Other Non-Cognitive Factors as Predictors of Academic Performance in High School. Master’s Thesis, University of Johannesburg, Johannesburg, South Africa, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  28. Cargnelutti, E.; Tomasetto, C.; Passolunghi, M.C. The interplay between affective and cognitive factors in shaping early proficiency in mathematics. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2017, 8, 28–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Cassady, J.C.; Johnson, R.E. Cognitive test anxiety and academic performance. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2002, 27, 270–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Chiesi, F.; Primi, C. Cognitive and Non-Cognitive Factors Related to Students’ Statistics Achievement. Stat. Educ. Res. J. 2010, 9, 6–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Chong, W.H.; Liem, G.A.D.; Huan, V.S.; Kit, P.L.; Ang, R.P. Student perceptions of self-efficacy and teacher support for learning in fostering youth competencies: Roles of affective and cognitive engagement. J. Adolesc. 2018, 68, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Danili, E.; Reid, N. Some strategies to improve performance in school chemistry, based on two cognitive factors. Res. Sci. Technol. Educ. 2004, 22, 203–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Danili, E.; Reid, N. Cognitive factors that can potentially affect pupils’ test performance. Chem. Educ. Res. Pract. 2006, 7, 64–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Diaz Mujica, A.; Pérez Villalobos, M.V.; Bernardo Gutiérrez, A.B.; Cervero Fernández-Castañón, A.; González-Pienda García, J.A. Affective and cognitive variables involved in structural prediction of university dropout. Psicothema 2019, 31, 429–436. [Google Scholar]
  35. Dotterer, A.M.; Lowe, K. Classroom context, school engagement, and academic achievement in early adolescence. J. Youth Adolesc. 2011, 40, 1649–1660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Febrilia, I.; Warokka, A.; Abdullah, H.H.; Indonesia, C. University students’ emotional state and academic performance: New insights of managing complex cognitive. J. e-Learn. High. Educ. 2011, 2011, 1–15. [Google Scholar]
  37. Fei, J.; Hu, Y.; Liang, L.; Meng, C.; Mei, S. Exploring the Impact of Emotional and Cognitive Factors on Anxiety Symptoms of Chinese Adolescents: A Serial Mediation Study. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 2023, 20, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Finkelstein, M. The underprepared college student: How non-cognitive factors influence academic preparedness. J. Stud. Educ. 2014, 4, 59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Fonteyne, L.; Duyck, W.; De Fruyt, F. Program-specific prediction of academic achievement on the basis of cognitive and non-cognitive factors. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2017, 56, 34–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Furinghetti, F.; Morselli, F. Every unsuccessful problem solver is unsuccessful in his or her own way: Affective and cognitive factors in proving. Educ. Stud. Math. 2009, 70, 71–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Gray, G.; Mcguinness, C.; Owende, P. Non-Cognitive Factors of Learning as Early Indicators of Students at-Risk of Failing in Tertiary Education. In Non-Cognitive Skills and Factors in Educational Attainment; Brill: Leiden, The Netherlands, 2016; pp. 199–237. [Google Scholar]
  42. Graziano, P.A.; Reavis, R.D.; Keane, S.P.; Calkins, S.D. The role of emotion regulation in children’s early academic success. J. Sch. Psychol. 2007, 45, 3–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Grigorian, C.; Kerfs, M.; Gee, J.P.; Widmann, J.M.; Chen, J.; Self, B.P. Update on the Role of Non-Cognitive and Affective (NCA) Factors in Engineering and Computing Student Academic Performance. In Proceedings of the 2021 ASEE Virtual Annual Conference Content Access, Virtual Conference, 26 July 2021–19 July 2022. [Google Scholar]
  44. Gumora, G.; Arsenio, W.F. Emotionality, emotion regulation, and school performance in middle school children. J. Sch. Psychol. 2002, 40, 395–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Haft, S.L.; Myers, C.A.; Hoeft, F. Socio-emotional and cognitive resilience in children with reading disabilities. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2016, 10, 133–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Hwang, G.J.; Sung, H.Y.; Chang, S.C.; Huang, X.C. A fuzzy expert system-based adaptive learning approach to improving students’ learning performances by considering affective and cognitive factors. Comput. Educ. Artif. Intell. 2020, 1, 100003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Imbrie, P.K.; Lin, J.J.J.; Malyscheff, A. Artificial Intelligence Methods to Forecast Engineering Students’ Retention Based on Cognitive and Non-Cognitive Factors. In Proceedings of the 2008 Annual Conference & Exposition, Pittsburgh, PA, USA, 22–25 June 2008; pp. 13–222. [Google Scholar]
  48. Jou, Y.T.; Mariñas, K.A.; Saflor, C.S. Assessing Cognitive Factors of Modular Distance Learning of K-12 Students Amidst the COVID-19 Pandemic towards Academic Achievements and Satisfaction. Behav. Sci. 2022, 12, 200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Karaman, P. Examining Non-Cognitive Factors Predicting Reading Achievement in Turkey: Evidence from PISA 2018. Int. J. Contemp. Educ. Res. 2022, 9, 450–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Katz, Y.J.; Yablon, Y.B. Online university learning: Cognitive and affective perspectives. Campus-Wide Inf. Syst. 2003, 20, 48–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Khine, M.S. Non-Cognitive Skills and Factors in Educational Success and Academic Achievement. In Non-Cognitive Skills and Factors in Educational Attainment; Brill: Leiden, The Netherlands, 2016; pp. 1–9. [Google Scholar]
  52. Khine, M.S.; Areepattamannil, S. (Eds.) Non-Cognitive Skills and Factors in Educational Attainment; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  53. Komarraju, M.; Ramsey, A.; Rinella, V. Cognitive and non-cognitive predictors of college readiness and performance: Role of academic discipline. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2013, 24, 103–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Kurian, J.; Mekoth, N. Deconstructing coping using cognitive influences on ability groups. Stud. High. Educ. 2021, 46, 177–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Lent, R.W.; Singley, D.; Sheu, H.B.; Schmidt, J.A.; Schmidt, L.C. Relation of social-cognitive factors to academic satisfaction in engineering students. J. Career Assess. 2007, 15, 87–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Lin, J.J.; Malyscheff, A.; Imbrie, P.K. Artificial Intelligence Methods to Forecast Engineering Students’ Retention Based on Cognitive and Non-Cognitive Factors; Purdue University: West Lafayette, IN, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  57. Liu, Y.; Afari, E.; Khine, M.S. Effect of non-cognitive factors on academic achievement among students in Suzhou: Evidence from OECD SSES data. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 2022, 11, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Macklem, G.L.; Macklem, G.L. Boredom and Its Relation to Non-Cognitive Factors: Student Motivation, Self-Regulation, Engagement in Learning, and Related Concepts. In Boredom in the Classroom: Addressing Student Motivation, Self-Regulation, and Engagement in Learning; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2015; pp. 35–43. [Google Scholar]
  59. Mavroveli, S.; Sánchez-Ruiz, M.J. Trait emotional intelligence influences on academic achievement and school behaviour. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2011, 81, 112–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Maw, J.A. The Facilitation of Student Success: Incorporating Affective, Behavioural, and Cognitive Factors into First-Year Experience Programs. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, MB, Canada, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  61. Mohtar, L.E.; Halim, L.; Samsudin, M.A.; Ismail, M.E. Non-cognitive factors influencing science achievement in Malaysia and Japan: An analysis of TIMSS 2015. EURASIA J. Math. Sci. Technol. Educ. 2019, 15, em1697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Ober, T.M.; Brodersen, A.S.; Rebouças-Ju, D.; Hong, M.R.; Carter, M.F.; Liu, C.; Cheng, Y. Math Attitudes, Engagement, and Performance of High School Students on High and Low-stakes Tests of Statistics Knowledge. J. STEM Educ. Res. 2022, 5, 402–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Pagán, J.E. Behavioral, Affective, and Cognitive Engagement of High School Music Students: Relation to Academic Achievement and Ensemble Performance Ratings. Ph.D. Thesis, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  64. Pietarinen, J.; Soini, T.; Pyhältö, K. Students’ emotional and cognitive engagement as the determinants of well-being and achievement in school. Int. J. Educ. Res. 2014, 67, 40–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Qualter, P.; Gardner, K.J.; Pope, D.J.; Hutchinson, J.M.; Whiteley, H.E. Ability emotional intelligence, trait emotional intelligence, and academic success in British secondary schools: A 5-year longitudinal study. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2012, 22, 83–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Rana, R.; Mahmood, N. The relationship between test anxiety and academic achievement. Bull. Educ. Res. 2010, 32, 63–74. [Google Scholar]
  67. Reynolds, K.; Bazemore, C.; Hanebuth, C.; Hendren, S.; Horn, M. The relationship of non-cognitive factors to academic and clinical performance in graduate rehabilitation science students in the United States: A systematic review. J. Educ. Eval. Health Prof. 2021, 18, 31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Richardson, M.; Abraham, C.; Bond, R. Psychological correlates of university students’ academic performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2012, 138, 353–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Sanchez-Ruiz, M.J.; El Khoury, J.; Saadé, G.; Salkhanian, M. Non-Cognitive Variables and Academic Achievement: The Role of General and Academic Self-Efficacy and Trait Emotional Intelligence. In Non-Cognitive Skills and Factors in Educational Attainment; Brill: Leiden, The Netherlands, 2016; pp. 65–85. [Google Scholar]
  70. Scheidt, M.; Godwin, A.; Senkpeil, R.R.; Ge, J.S.; Chen, J.; Self, B.P.; Widmann, J.M.; Berger, E.J. Validity Evidence for the SUCCESS Survey: Measuring Non-Cognitive and Affective Traits of Engineering and Computing Students. In Proceedings of the 2018 ASEE Annual Conference & Exposition, Salt Lake City, UT, USA, 23 June–27 July 2018. [Google Scholar]
  71. Scheidt, M.; Senkpeil, R.; Chen, J.; Godwin, A.; Berger, E. SAT Does not Spell Success: How Non-Cognitive Factors can Explain Variance in the GPA of Undergraduate Engineering and Computer Science Students. In Proceedings of the 2018 IEEE Frontiers in Education Conference (FIE), San Jose, CA, USA, 3–6 October 2018; pp. 1–5. [Google Scholar]
  72. Semeraro, C.; Giofrè, D.; Coppola, G.; Lucangeli, D.; Cassibba, R. The role of cognitive and non-cognitive factors in mathematics achievement: The importance of the quality of the student-teacher relationship in middle school. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0231381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  73. Sommerfeld, A. Recasting Non-Cognitive Factors in College Readiness as What They Truly Are: Non-Academic Factors. J. Coll. Admiss. 2011, 213, 18–22. [Google Scholar]
  74. Stavredes, T. Effective Online Teaching: Foundations and Strategies for Student Success; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  75. Stephanou, G.; Kyridis, A. University Students’ Perceptions of Teacher Effectiveness and Emotions in Lectures: The Role of Socio-Cognitive Factors, and Academic Performance. Int. Educ. Stud. 2012, 5, 58–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  76. Wang, M.T.; Eccles, J.S. Adolescent behavioral, emotional, and cognitive engagement trajectories in school and their differential relations to educational success. J. Res. Adolesc. 2012, 22, 31–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  77. Zhang, L.; Wang, L.; Treagust, D.F. Discipline-specific cognitive factors that influence grade 9 students’ performance in chemistry. Chem. Educ. Res. Pract. 2021, 22, 813–841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Publication screening process.
Figure 1. Publication screening process.
Behavsci 13 00555 g001
Figure 2. The funnel plot of the effect size of cognitive dominance.
Figure 2. The funnel plot of the effect size of cognitive dominance.
Behavsci 13 00555 g002
Figure 3. The funnel plot of the effect size of emotional dominance.
Figure 3. The funnel plot of the effect size of emotional dominance.
Behavsci 13 00555 g003
Table 1. Affective and cognitive factors affecting students’ academic performance: examples from the literature.
Table 1. Affective and cognitive factors affecting students’ academic performance: examples from the literature.
No.AuthorsYearSample SizeEffect Size ClassInfluencing Factor
1Joseph E. Betts20102416correlation coefficient(1) Teacher–student relationships (0.74)
(2) Control and relevance of school work (0.7)
(1) Peer support for learning (0.76)
(2) Future aspirations and goals (0.80)
(1) Family support for learning (0.79)
2Fitra A. Bachtiar2015188correlation coefficient(1) Motivation (0.526)
(1) Introversion (−0.145)
(1) Extroversion (0.677)
(1) Anxiety (−0.304)
(2) Learning scores (0.603)
3Hameedah Sayani2018204t-value(2) Cognitive benefits (7.62)
(2) Skill development (2.36)
(1) Affective benefits (4.95)
4Brendan M. Whitney2018195correlation coefficient(2) Cognitive benefits (0.74)
(1) Affective benefits (0.73)
(3) Behavioral benefits (0.76)
5Sarah McGeown2016439correlation coefficient(2) Challenge (0.59)
(1) Interpersonal confidence (0.21)
(1) Confidence in abilities (0.46)
(1) Control of emotions (0.24)
(2) Control of life (0.43)
(1) Commitment (0.31)
(1) Self-belief (0.74)
(1) Persistence (0.6)
(2) Valuing (0.79)
(2) Task management (0.55)
(2) Planning (0.57)
(1) Anxiety (0.27)
Table 2. Results of the meta-analysis based on comprehensive correlation coefficients.
Table 2. Results of the meta-analysis based on comprehensive correlation coefficients.
Influencing FactorKNR95% Confidence IntervalZ Test
p-Value
Fail-Safe Number
Lower Limit of IntervalUpper Limit of Interval
Learning Scores27100.65910.56530.75300.0001317
Future Aspirations and Goals445960.55610.14980.26250.00023086
Peer Support for Learning351040.55290.27840.82740.00012019
Family Support for Learning247950.51030.05950.96110.02651361
Self-Belief311550.46880.1130.82460.0098452
Cognitive Benefits874950.45960.29370.62550.00013139
Skill Development27100.4354−0.09080.96160.1049174
Attitudes and Beliefs415400.43050.1670.6940.0014628
Affective Benefits768340.40660.24270.57040.00011581
Motivation225670.3980.15720.63890.0012177
Self-Regulation314370.37060.06420.6770.0178149
Behavioral Engagement462250.3560.10130.61070.0062652
Values37210.35230.06510.63950.0162126
Optimism Emotion525220.35220.24030.46410.0001451
Knowledge25460.34230.25770.42690.000148
Character212440.3212−0.07570.7180.112751
Control and Relevance of School Work364550.2069−0.44220.85590.5322696
Self-Efficacy414900.13540.08560.18530.000136
Interest23620.12420.02250.22580.01673
Task Management220990.0947−0.87551.06490.84822
Anxiety4851−0.1249−0.40430.15450.38091
Table 3. Heterogeneity test results of the meta-analysis.
Table 3. Heterogeneity test results of the meta-analysis.
Influencing FactorKNHeterogeneity (Q Test)
Q-Valuep-Value I 2 τ 2 τ SE
Learning Scores27103.74850.052973.32%0.00340.05870.1425
Future Aspirations and Goals44596415.82260.000199.64%0.08920.29860.1498
Peer Support for Learning35104478.95950.000199.42%0.05830.24150.1401
Family Support for Learning24795478.82380.000199.79%0.10560.32490.23
Self-Belief3115570.55840.000198.77%0.09680.31120.1815
Cognitive Benefits87495856.64470.000198.75%0.05490.23430.0846
Skill Development271055.63880.000198.20%0.14160.37630.2685
Attitudes and Beliefs41540111.02990.000198.15%0.07030.26510.1345
Affective Benefits76834323.74590.000198.03%0.04640.21540.0836
Motivation2256719.18090.000194.79%0.02870.16940.1229
Self-Regulation31437130.44810.000197.45%0.06930.26320.1563
Behavioral Engagement46225224.06730.000199.06%0.06620.25720.1299
Values372144.20210.000193.68%0.05920.24320.1465
Optimism Emotion525221299.85170.000199.12%0.16530.40650.0571
Knowledge25461.2390.265719.29%0.00080.02790.0432
Character2124444.11450.000197.73%0.08020.28310.2025
Control and Relevance of School Work364552719.050.000199.92%0.32870.57330.3311
Self-Efficacy414904.76460.18990.12%00.00220.0254
Interest23620.24590.620.00%000.0519
Task Management22099702.29070.000199.86%0.48940.69950.495
Anxiety485128.94960.000191.14%0.03460.18590.0993
Table 4. Influencing factors related to students’ academic performances.
Table 4. Influencing factors related to students’ academic performances.
Correlation Coefficient Influencing Factors
Strong correlation (r ≥ 0.5)Cognitive FactorLearning Scores (0.6591), Future Aspirations, and Goals (0.5561)
Affective FactorPeer Support for Learning (0.5529), Family Support for Learning (0.5103)
Medium correlation (0.3 ≤ r ≤ 0.5)Cognitive FactorCognitive Benefits (0.4596), Skill Development (0.4354), Self-Regulation (0.3706), Values (0.3523), Knowledge (0.3423), Character (0.3212)
Affective FactorSelf-Belief (0.4688), Attitudes and Beliefs (0.4305), Affective Benefits (0.4066), Motivation (0.398), Optimism Emotion (0.3522)
Behavioral FactorBehavioral Engagement (0.356)
Weak correlation (0.1 ≤ r ≤ 0.3)Cognitive FactorControl and Relevance of School Work (0.2069)
Affective FactorSelf-Efficacy (0.1354)
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Li, J.; Xue, E.; Li, C.; He, Y. Investigating Latent Interactions between Students’ Affective Cognition and Learning Performance: Meta-Analysis of Affective and Cognitive Factors. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 555. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13070555

AMA Style

Li J, Xue E, Li C, He Y. Investigating Latent Interactions between Students’ Affective Cognition and Learning Performance: Meta-Analysis of Affective and Cognitive Factors. Behavioral Sciences. 2023; 13(7):555. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13070555

Chicago/Turabian Style

Li, Jian, Eryong Xue, Chenchang Li, and Yunshu He. 2023. "Investigating Latent Interactions between Students’ Affective Cognition and Learning Performance: Meta-Analysis of Affective and Cognitive Factors" Behavioral Sciences 13, no. 7: 555. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13070555

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop