Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Sleep, 03 April 2023
Sec. Sleep, Behavior and Mental Health
Volume 2 - 2023 | https://doi.org/10.3389/frsle.2023.1145203

Regulation of dendritic spines in the amygdala following sleep deprivation

Lindsay Rexrode1 Matthew Tennin1 Jobin Babu1,2 Caleb Young1 Ratna Bollavarapu1 Lamiorkor Ameley Lawson1 Jake Valeri1,2 Harry Pantazopoulos1,2 Barbara Gisabella1,2*
  • 1Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, University of Mississippi Medical Center, Jackson, MS, United States
  • 2Program in Neuroscience, University of Mississippi Medical Center, Jackson, MS, United States

The amygdala is a hub of emotional circuits involved in the regulation of cognitive and emotional behaviors and its critically involved in emotional reactivity, stress regulation, and fear memory. Growing evidence suggests that the amygdala plays a key role in the consolidation of emotional memories during sleep. Neuroimaging studies demonstrated that the amygdala is selectively and highly activated during rapid eye movement sleep (REM) and sleep deprivation induces emotional instability and dysregulation of the emotional learning process. Regulation of dendritic spines during sleep represents a morphological correlate of memory consolidation. Several studies indicate that dendritic spines are remodeled during sleep, with evidence for broad synaptic downscaling and selective synaptic upscaling in several cortical areas and the hippocampus. Currently, there is a lack of information regarding the regulation of dendritic spines in the amygdala during sleep. In the present work, we investigated the effect of 5 h of sleep deprivation on dendritic spines in the mouse amygdala. Our data demonstrate that sleep deprivation results in differential dendritic spine changes depending on both the amygdala subregions and the morphological subtypes of dendritic spines. We observed decreased density of mushroom spines in the basolateral amygdala of sleep deprived mice, together with increased neck length and decreased surface area and volume. In contrast, we observed greater densities of stubby spines in sleep deprived mice in the central amygdala, indicating that downscaling selectively occurs in this spine type. Greater neck diameters for thin spines in the lateral and basolateral nuclei of sleep deprived mice, and decreases in surface area and volume for mushroom spines in the basolateral amygdala compared to increases in the cental amygdala provide further support for spine type-selective synaptic downscaling in these areas during sleep. Our findings suggest that sleep promotes synaptic upscaling of mushroom spines in the basolateral amygdala, and downscaling of selective spine types in the lateral and central amygdala. In addition, we observed decreased density of phosphorylated cofilin immunoreactive and growth hormone immunoreactive cells in the amygdala of sleep deprived mice, providing further support for upscaling of dendritic spines during sleep. Overall, our findings point to region- and spine type-specific changes in dendritic spines during sleep in the amygdala, which may contribute to consolidation of emotional memories during sleep.

Introduction

Sleep is critical for the regulation of emotional memory consolidation, and disturbances in sleep and memory processing along with alterations in dendric spine numbers are widely reported in people with psychiatric disorders (Ford and Kamerow, 1989; Krakow et al., 2000; Ohayon and Shapiro, 2000; Krystal et al., 2008, 2016; Dorph-Petersen et al., 2009; Sweet et al., 2009; Murray and Harvey, 2010; Gruber et al., 2011; Penzes et al., 2011; Duman and Aghajanian, 2012; Ebdlahad et al., 2013; Germain, 2013; Glausier and Lewis, 2013; Licznerski and Duman, 2013; Shelton et al., 2015; Koffel et al., 2016; Dolsen et al., 2017; MacDonald et al., 2017; Soehner et al., 2017; Forrest et al., 2018; Gisabella et al., 2021). Sleep is critically involved in strengthening memories (for review see Stickgold, 2005). However, the morphological and molecular processes involved in memory consolidation during sleep are still not well understood. Two non-mutually exclusive theories have emerged regarding synaptic regulation underlying memory consolidation during sleep. Studies from Tononi and Cirelli (2006, 2014) have established the synaptic homeostasis hypothesis (Tononi and Cirelli, 2006, 2014) which proposes that neurons form and strengthen synapses during wakefulness as organisms interact with the environment and encode new memories. During sleep, when the encoding process is offline, synapses are downscaled to prevent over-excitation of neurons and improve signal-to-noise ratio and memory performance (Tononi and Cirelli, 2006, 2014). Several studies reporting decreases in dendritic spines, synaptic density, and synaptic markers during sleep in sensory and motor cortical regions provide support for broad synaptic downscaling during sleep (Cirelli, 2005; Maret et al., 2011; de Vivo et al., 2017).

An alternate theory originally proposed by Mueller and Pilzecker and expanded by several groups (McGaugh, 1999) including more recently by Born and colleagues (Rasch and Born, 2013; Dudai et al., 2015), proposes that selective synapses formed during the day are selectively tagged and strengthened during sleep as memory storage is reorganized. Several recent studies support this theory. For example, evidence that sleep deprivation impairs memory strength (Vecsey et al., 2009), and results in decreased dendritic spines (Havekes et al., 2016), supports the hypothesis that selective synapses are strengthened during sleep. In addition, in vivo dendritic spine imaging demonstrated that sleep promotes increases in dendritic spines in selective branches of motor cortex neurons following motor learning (Yang et al., 2014), providing further support that sleep strengthens selective synapses formed during recent learning, even in areas where net synaptic downscaling during sleep was reported (Maret et al., 2011; de Vivo et al., 2017).

Despite the growing evidence for synaptic changes during sleep supporting both downscaling and upscaling theories, very few studies have examined dendritic spine regulation during sleep in subcortical areas involved in emotional memory processing. In our recent study, we observed region- and branch-specific synaptic downscaling in the hippocampus of adult mice, supporting the theory of broad but selective synaptic downscaling in this subcortical region (Gisabella et al., 2020). Furthermore, a recent electron microscopy study in adolescent mice provides additional evidence for broad downscaling in this region (Spano et al., 2019). The amygdala is a key subcortical region involved in emotional memory processing and sleep. The amygdala is highly interconnected with the hipppocampus and these two regions work together during emotional memory processing, with the amygdala representing a key region critically invovled in stress regulation and fear memory (Wilensky et al., 2000; Pare et al., 2004; LeDoux, 2007; Rodrigues et al., 2009; Johansen et al., 2011), while the hippocampus is critical for remembering the spatial context of the fearful stimulus. Recent evidence indicates that the amygdala is part of a network together with the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex involved in fear memory consolidation during sleep (Miyawaki and Mizuseki, 2022). Furthermore, the central nucleus of the amygdala (CEA) projects to brainstem REM regulatory regions (Inagaki et al., 1983; Wellman et al., 2022) and recent evidence demonstrates that the amygdala actively participates in the regulation of sleep architecture (Hasegawa et al., 2022). Specifically, dopamine signaling in the basolateral amygdala (BL) is involved in promoting the transition from NREM to REM sleep (Hasegawa et al., 2022). In comparison, the lateral amygdala (LA) serves as a sensory gateway to the amygdala, integrating sensory inputs from thalamic and cortical areas (LeDoux et al., 1990a; Li et al., 1995). Recent studies suggest that synaptic downscaling occurs specifically during REM sleep (Li et al., 2017; Zhou et al., 2020), thus the amygdala may also regulate synaptic processing during sleep through its role in promoting REM sleep.

The amygdala is also critically involved in the regulation emotional behaviors implicated in psychiatric disorders (Bishop et al., 2004). For example, schizophrenia (SZ) and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) display abnormalities in emotional processing as well sleep disturbances and memory dysfunction (Frodl et al., 2002; Sah et al., 2003; Benarroch, 2015; Krystal et al., 2016; Ferrarelli and Tononi, 2017). The amygdala is hyperactive in PTSD (Bremner et al., 2005) and regulates fear and stress alterations in sleep (Wellman et al., 2008, 2013, 2016; Liu et al., 2009). Furthermore, evidence suggests the amygdala is associated with heightened emotional reactivity in insomnia disorders (Baglioni et al., 2010, 2014). Studies in humans describe amygdala hyperreactivity due to sleep loss (Motomura et al., 2017). Interestingly, altered numbers of dendritic spines, which serve as morphological representations of encoded memory, are also consistently reported across several psychiatric disorders (Dorph-Petersen et al., 2009; Sweet et al., 2009; Penzes et al., 2011; Duman and Aghajanian, 2012; Glausier and Lewis, 2013; Licznerski and Duman, 2013; Shelton et al., 2015; MacDonald et al., 2017).

Currently there is no evidence available regarding the effects of sleep or sleep deprivation on dendritic spines in the amygdala. Chronic stress studies have shown that dendritic spines are differentially affected in the amygdala compared to the hippocampus, with dendritic spine density increasing in the amygdala but decreasing in the hippocampus following chronic stress (Vyas et al., 2002). This suggests that memory consolidation may differentially affect dendritic spine morphology between these two regions. We tested the hypothesis that sleep deprivation prevents broad synaptic downscaling in the amygdala. We used viral vector labeling of dendritic spines combined with confocal imaging and three-dimensional (3D) analysis in sleep-deprived and control mice to determine how sleep deprivation affects dendritic spines on neurons in the subregions of the amygdala. Immunohistochemistry and quantitative microscopy for phosphorylated cofilin (pCoflin), a marker involved in the dendritic spine downscaling (Cichon et al., 2012; Calabrese et al., 2014; Havekes et al., 2016; Noguchi et al., 2016) and for growth hormone (GH), a molecule shown to increase dendritic spines and fear memory in the amygdala (Gisabella et al., 2016), were used as a first step in examining the molecular factors involved in synaptic changes during sleep. Specifically, cofilin is involved in cleaving actin filaments during dendritic spine downscaling, and phosphorylation of cofilin decreases its activity (Havekes et al., 2016). Thus, pCofilin levels are predicted to be inverse to spine density measures in sleep deprived mice, whereas GH measures are predicted to positively correlate with spine density measures.

Methods

Animals

All mice used in experiments were adult male C57BL/6J mice (available from Jackson Laboratories, Bar Harbor, ME). Animals were maintained on a 12:12 h light-dark cycle with ad libitum access to water and food. All animal procedures met National Institutes of Health standards, as outlined in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, and all protocols were approved by the University of Mississippi Medical Center Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC).

Viral vector injection and sleep deprivation

Adult (3–4 months old) male C57BL/6J mice (6 controls, 6 sleep-deprived mice) received bilateral stereotaxic microinjections of 2 μl of AAV1-CMV-eGFP virus (Vector Biolabs, cat# 7002) in each hemisphere. This number of animals per group was sufficient to detect changes in dendritic spines from sleep deprivation in mice in previous studies from our lab and others (Havekes et al., 2016; Spano et al., 2019; Gisabella et al., 2020). Injections of the AAV-GFP virus under the CMV promoter allowed for the labeling of dendritic branches for spine analysis. The injections targeted the amygdala (BLA) region (stereotaxic coordinates AP:−1.1, ML:3.2, DV:4.1). The virus was infused using a Nanofil 35-gauge stainless steel bevel needle (catalog # NF35BV, World Precision Instruments, Inc., Sarasota, FL) attached to a 10μl Nanofil syringe (Hamilton Company, Reno, NV). Hamilton syringes were mounted in a stereotaxic barrel holder, and the rate of virus delivery was controlled by an automated syringe pump (Harvard Apparatus, Holliston, MA). After 6 weeks, animals were divided randomly into two cohorts of six mice each (6 controls, 6 sleep-deprived mice). The Pinnacle automated sleep deprivation system (Cat# 9000-K5-S) which simulates gentle handling was used for 5 h of sleep deprivation from lights on (6 AM) to 11 AM. This system consists of a cylindrical housing chamber with a bar that continuously rotates at 5 rpm and randomly reversed direction every 10–30 s, which prevents animals from sleeping. All animals were housed in the cylindrical chambers beforehand to adapt to the environment and control animals were housed in the same chambers but without the rotating bar. A researcher visually verified that the bar rotated at all times and that mice did not use alternative strategies to avoid the bar and sleep. This system has been established by prior studies to effectively reduce sleep as measured with EEG recordings in rats and mice (Hines et al., 2013; Burgdorf et al., 2019; Aguilar et al., 2020; Yuan et al., 2021). Both control and sleep-deprived mice were sacrificed at 11 AM (5 h after lights on in the 12:12 light cycle) and processed for quantification of dendritic spines. This time point 5 h into the light cycle is identical to previous studies from our lab and others (Havekes et al., 2016; Gisabella et al., 2020). Brains were then analyzed for viral vector expression and dendritic spines as in our published and preliminary studies (Gisabella et al., 2020).

Tissue processing

Mice were deeply anesthetized with isoflurane and perfused with 0.1M phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) containing 10% formalin. Brains were removed and cryoprotected in 30% sucrose in 0.1M PBS (pH 7.4), then sectioned into coronal 40 μm serial sections using a freezing microtome (American Optical 860, Buffalo, NY) and stored in 0.1 Molar phosphate buffer with 0.1% NaAzide. Sections were mounted on gelatin-coated slides and coverslipped using Dako mounting media (S3023, Dako, North America, Carpinteria, CA) to quantify dendritic spine density from images captured using confocal microscopy.

Immunocytochemistry (mouse samples)

Free-floating mouse brain tissue sections were carried through antigen retrieval in citric acid buffer (0.1 M citric acid, 0.2 M Na2HPO4) heated to 80°C for 30 min, and incubated in the rabbit primary antibody anti-pCofilin (Ser3) (cat#3311, Cell Signaling) (1:1000 μl) or rabbit anti-GH1 (Protein Tech Lab, cat# 55243-1-AP) (1:500 μl) for 48 h at 4°C, and subsequently in biotinylated secondary antibody (goat anti-rabbit IgG; 1:500; Vector Labs, Inc. Burlingame, CA), followed by streptavidin conjugated with horse-radish peroxidase for 2 h (1:5000 μl, Zymed, San Francisco, CA), and, finally, in nickel-enhanced diaminobenzidine/ peroxidase reaction (0.02% diaminobenzidine, Sigma-Aldrich, 0.08% nickel-sulfate, 0.006% hydrogen peroxide in PB). All solutions were made in PBS with 0.2% Triton X (PBS-Tx) unless otherwise specified. Immunostained sections were mounted on gelatin-coated glass slides, dehydrated in a gradient ethanol series, coverslipped, and coded for blinded quantitative analysis. All sections included in the study were processed simultaneously within the same session to avoid procedural differences. Omission of the primary or secondary antibodies did not result in detectable signal.

Confocal imaging

A Zeiss LSM 880 confocal microscope system interfaced with Zen imaging software (ZEN 2.3 SP1) was used to acquire 3D image stacks of dendritic branches from amygdala neurons in sections from control and sleep-deprived mice. All slides used for confocal imaging were coded for blind analysis. Images of 40 μm-thick sections were acquired, with a z-step of 0.5 μm using a 63x oil immersion objective (numerical aperture 1.4 DIC M27; pixel size, 0.10 × 0.10 μm) similar to the method described in our previous study (Gisabella et al., 2016). For dendritic spine quantification, confocal microscopy images were analyzed using Neurolucida 360 software with Autospine to measure spine density in the amygdala cells using an approach previously described (Gisabella et al., 2016). Dendritic spines were sampled in the Lateral (LA), Basolateral (BL), and Central (CEA) areas of the amygdala by an investigator who was blinded to the treatment group. The complex organization of neurons and their dendritic branches in the amygdala, together with the variation in injection sites within each animal made it challenging to follow branches from individual neurons and to establish sampling strategies. Therefore, we collected confocal images of all clearly visible viral vector labeled dendritic branches within the lateral, basolateral, or central nuclei using 25 × 25 μm confocal imaging windows. The minimal viral spread allowed for the feasibility of this approach. Spine density, shape, and volume were analyzed using Neurolucida 360 with semiautomated analysis from 3D confocal image stacks. We quantified the numbers of mushroom, thin, and stubby spines in each branch segment, and the spine head diameter and volume of each spine.

Dendritic spine quantification

For dendritic spine quantification, confocal microscopy images were analyzed using Neurolucida 360 software with Autospine to measure spine density as described in our previous study (Gisabella et al., 2016). Spines were grouped into thin spines, stubby spines, and mushroom spines automatically by the Neurolucida 360 software based on the spine head to neck diameter ratio (1.1), length-to-head ratio (2.5) mushroom head size (0.35 μm), and filopodia length (3 μm) according to previously established criteria (Rodriguez et al., 2008). Spine density, shape, and volume were quantified using Neurolucida 360 with semiautomated analysis from 3D confocal image stacks in an unbiased manner.

Microscopy data collection

In mouse brain samples, serial sections containing the amygdala were quantified using a Leica microscope interfaced with Bioquant Nova Prime v6.0, (R&M Biometrics, Nashville, Tennessee) for pCofilin and using a Zeiss Axioskop 40 interfaced with StereoInvestigator v2021.1.3 for growth hormone. Borders of each region were defined according to the Allen Brain Atlas and traced under 4x magnification. Each traced region was systematically scanned through the full x, y, and z-axes under 40x magnification to count each pCofilin immunoreactive (IR) cell and under 20x magnification to count each GH-IR cell.

Statistical analysis

Densities of dendritic spines were calculated as spines per dendrite segment length in micrometers. Numerical densities of pCofilin cells were calculated as Nd= ∑N / ∑V where N = sum of all pCofilin or GH immunoreactive cells counted in each region for each animal, and V is the volume of each region per animal, calculated as V= ∑a • z, where z is the thickness of each section (30 μm) and a is area in μm2. For all statistical tests, the significance threshold was p ≤ 0.05. Non-paramatric (Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney) tests were used to compare population estimates for control and sleep-deprived groups as data were not normally distributed, and were followed by Bonferroni post hoc correction. Box plots were used to depict the data for each group from n = 6 control and n = 6 sleep-deprived animals.

Results

Lower density of basolateral amygdala dendritic spines in sleep deprived mice

Dendritic spines were quantified from amygdala subregions including the LA, BL, and CEA (Figure 1). We observed significantly decreased dendritic spine density in the BL of sleep deprived mice (p < 0.0005; Figure 1B) compared to the littermates that were permitted to sleep undisturbed (control mean: 1.76, SD mean: 1.59, 10.15 % decrease). In comparison, dendritic spine density was increased in the LA (control mean: 1.39, SD mean: 1.55, 10.88 % increase) and CEA (control mean: 1.35, SD mean: 1.55, 13.8 % increase) of sleep deprived mice (Figure 1B). Decreased density of dendritic spines in the BL was driven by mushroom spines (p < 0.0001; Figure 1F) (control mean: 0.57, SD mean: 0.50, −13.01 % decrease). In contrast, the increase in dendritic spine density in sleep-deprived mice in the LA was observed in stubby spines (LA: p < 0.0004; Figure 1H) (Control mean: 0.17, SD mean: 0.21, 21.05 % increase).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Dendritic spines in the basolateral amygdala (BL) are lower in sleep-deprived mice. AAV viral vector was used to label dendritic processes (A). Decreased density of dendritic spines was observed in the BL amygdala in sleep-deprived mice (nmice = 6; ndendrites = 383) compared to control mice (nmice = 6; ndendrites = 450) (B). In contrast, density of dendritic spines in the LA of sleep-deprived mice (nmice = 5; ndendrites = 167) was increased in comparison to control mice (nmice = 5; ndendrites = 105) (B). A similar increase in density was observed in the CEA of sleep-deprived mice (nmice = 5; ndendrites = 222) compared to control mice (nmice = 5; ndendrites = 239). Representative confocal micrograph of a 10 μm branch segment from the BL of a sleep-deprived mouse (D), with fewer spines than a representative branch segment from a control mouse (C). (E) Representative confocal image depicting examples of thin, mushroom, and stubby spines. Dendritic spine density was decreased in mushroom spines in the BL of sleep-deprived mice (nmice = 6; ndendrites = 383) compared to control mice (nmice = 6; ndendrites = 450) (F). Mushroom spines from the LA of sleep-deprived mice (nmice = 5; ndendrites = 167) were not significantly different from control mice (nmice = 5; ndendrites = 105) (F). Similarly, no differences were observed in CEA mushroom spines from sleep deprived mice (nmice = 5; ndendrites = 222) compared to control mice (nmice = 5; ndendrites = 239). No difference in density of thin spines was observed in the LA of sleep-deprived mice (nmice = 5; ndendrites = 167) compared to control mice (nmice = 5; ndendrites = 105) (G), or in thin spines from the CEA of sleep-deprived mice (nmice = 5; ndendrites = 222) compared to control mice (nmice = 5; ndendrites = 239) (G). Similarly, no difference was observed in the density of thin spines in the BL of sleep deprived mice (nmice = 6; ndendrites = 383) compared to control mice (nmice = 6; ndendrites = 450). Increased density of stubby spines was observed in the LA of sleep-deprived mice (nmice = 5; ndendrites = 167) compared to control mice (nmice = 5; ndendrites = 105) (H). No difference was observed in the CEA of sleep-deprived mice (nmice = 5; ndendrites = 222) compared to control mice (nmice = 5; ndendrites = 239), or in the density of stubby spines in the BL of sleep deprived mice (nmice = 6; ndendrites = 383) compared to control mice (nmice = 6; ndendrites = 450) (G). Box plots depict values for each group, statistical significance was determined using the Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney test with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

Subregion and spine type specific increases in neck length of dendritic spines in sleep deprived mice

The LA and BL displayed significantly greater neck backbone length of mushroom spines in sleep-deprived mice vs. control mice (Figure 2; LA: p < 0.009 and BL: p < 0.0001) (LA: control mean: 0.88, SD mean: 0.95, 7.70% increase; BL: control mean: 0.88, SD mean: 0.95, 7.70% increase). In comparison, thin spines displayed increased neck backbone length in sleep deprived mice in the CEA (Figure 2; p < 0.01) (control mean: 0.78, SD mean: 0.82, 5.00 % increase) and stubby spines displayed greater neck backbone length in sleep deprived mice in the BL (p < 0.01), Figure 2E (control mean: 0.35, SD mean: 0.37, 5.56 % increase). Similar changes were observed regarding head backbone length (Figures 2FH). Mushroom head backbone length was greater in sleep-deprived mice vs. controls in the LA and BL areas (LA: p < 0.001 and BL: p < 0.0001, Figure 2F) (LA: control mean: 1.15, SD mean: 1.21, 5.10% increase; BL: control mean: 1.14, SD mean: 1.20, 5.13% increase). Stubby dendritic spines in the BL amygdala displayed significantly greater head backbone length (p < 0.005, Figure 2H) in sleep-deprived mice vs. controls (control mean: 0.35, SD mean: 0.37, 5.56% increase).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Differential changes in neck backbone and head backbone length dendritic spines in sleep deprived mice. Neurolucida 360 was used to obtain measures of dendritic spine neck backbone length and head backbone length from confocal images. The diagram (A, B) depicts neck backbone length measured as the distance from the insertion point to the center of the spine head minus the head radius and anchor radius (A). Head backbone length is measured as the distance from the insertion point to the center of the spine head minus the anchor radius (B). Neck backbone length was significantly greater in mushroom spines from the LA of sleep-deprived mice (nmice = 5; nspines = 1,339) compared to control mice (nmice = 5; nspines = 1,217) (C). A similar increase was observed in the BL of sleep-deprived mice (nmice = 6; nspines = 3,143) compared to control mice (nmice = 6; nspines = 5,385) for mushroom spine backbone length (C). No significant difference was observed for neck backbone length of mushroom spines in the CEA of sleep deprived mice (nmice = 5; nspines = 1,543) compared to control mice (nmice = 5; nspines = 1,768). In comparison, neck backbone length in thin spines was not altered in the LA of sleep-deprived mice (nmice = 5; nspines = 2,525) compared to control mice (nmice = 6; nspines = 2,046) or in the BL of sleep-deprived mice (nmice = 5; nspines = 5,645) compared to control mice (nmice = 5; nspines = 9,079). Neck backbone length was increased in thin spines from the CEA in sleep-deprived mice (nmice = 5; nspines = 3,207) compared to control mice (nmice = 5; nspines = 3,449) (D). Neck backbone length was greater in stubby spines from the BL of sleep deprived mice (nmice = 6; nspines = 1,343) compared to control mice (nmice = 6; nspines = 2,230). No difference in neck backbone length was observed in the CEA of sleep-deprived mice (nmice = 5; nspines = 878) compared to control mice (nmice = 5; nspines = 871) (E), or in stubby spines in the LA of sleep-deprived mice (nmice = 5; nspines = 618) compared to control mice (nmice = 5; nspines = 469). Similar changes were observed for head backbone length measures, with increased length in mushroom spines from sleep-deprived mice vs. controls in the LA and BL areas and for stubby spines in the CEA (F–H). Box plots depict values for each group, statistical significance was determined using the Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney test with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

Subregion and spine type specific changes in spine head and neck diameter in sleep deprived mice

Decreased spine head diameter was observed in the CEA for thin (p < 0.0002; Figure 3) and stubby (p < 0.009; Figure 3C) spines in sleep deprived mice compared to control mice (thin: control mean: 0.26, SD mean: 0.25, 3.92% decrease; stubby: control mean: 0.61, SD mean: 0.58, 5.04 % decrease). No changes in spine head diameter were detected in thin or stubby spines the LA or BL and no changes were observed in mushroom spine head diameters in any area (Figures 3AC). In comparison, significantly decreased spine neck diameter was observed in the mushroom spines in the BL amygdala (p < 0.0006; Figure 3D) (control mean: 0.14, SD mean: 0.13, 7.41 % decrease), as well as in thin spines in the CEA (thin spines: p < 0.0002; Figure 3E) (control mean: 0.13, SD mean: 0.12, 8.92% decrease). In contrast, we observed increased neck diameter in thin dendritic spines located in the LA (p < 0.0002; Figure 3E) and BL (p < 0.01; Figure 3E) amygdala in sleep deprived mice (LA: Control mean: 0.12, SD mean: 0.14, 15.38% increase; BL: Control mean: 0.12, SD mean: 0.13, 8.00% increase).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Differential changes of head and neck diameter across dendritic spine types during sleep. Mushroom spine head diameter was not significantly different between sleep-deprived and control mice in the LA (Control: nmice = 5; nspines = 1,217; SD: nmice = 5; nspines = 1,339), BL (Control: nmice = 6; nspines = 5,385; SD: nmice = 6; nspines = 3,143) or CEA (Control: nmice = 5; nspines = 1,768; SD: nmice = 5; nspines = 1,543) (A). Thin spine head diameter was selectively decreased in the CEA of sleep-deprived mice (Control: nmice = 5; nspines = 3,449; SD: nmice = 5; nspines = 3,207) (B), with no significant differences in the LA (Control: nmice = 5; nspines = 2,046; SD: nmice = 5; nspines = 2,525) or BL (Control: nmice = 6; nspines = 9,079; SD: nmice = 6; nspines = 5,645) (B). A similar selective decrease was observed for stubby spines in the CEA (Control: nmice = 5; nspines = 871; SD: nmice = 5; nspines = 878), with no significant changes in the LA (Control: nmice = 5; nspines = 469; SD: nmice = 5; nspines = 618) or BL (Control: nmice = 6; nspines = 2,230; SD: nmice = 6; nspines = 1,343) (C). Spine neck diameter was significantly lower in mushroom spines in the BL of sleep deprived mice, with no changes in mushroom spines in the LA or CEA (D). In contrast, neck diameter for thin spines was significantly greater in the LA and BL of sleep-deprived mice and lower in the CEA (E). Neck diameter for stubby spines was not altered (F). Box plots depict values for each group, statistical significance was determined using the Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney test with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

Spine subtype and amygdala region specific changes in surface area and volume in sleep deprived mice

Decreased dendritic spine surface area was observed in the BL in sleep deprived mice compared to control mice for mushroom spines (p < 0.0001; Figure 4) (control mean: 5.39, SD mean: 5.05, −6.51% decrease). In comparison, increased surface area was observed in sleep deprived mice for mushroom spines located in CEA (p < 0.0001; Figure 4A) (control mean: 5.45, SD mean: 6.46, 16.97 % increase). Decreased dendritic spine head volume was observed specifically in mushroom spines in sleep deprived mice in the BL amygdala (p < 0.0001; Figure 4D) (control mean: 0.57, SD mean: 0.53, −7.27 % decrease). In contrast, spine volume was significantly increased in sleep deprived mice in mushroom spines in the CEA (p < 0.0001; Figure 4D) (control mean: 0.62, SD mean: 0.69, 10.69 % increase) and stubby spines in the CEA (p < 0.002; Figure 4F) (control mean: 1.79, SD mean: 2.10, 15.94% increase).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Dendritic spine surface area and volume differences between sleep-deprived and control mice. Dendritic spine surface area was significantly lower in mushroom spines from sleep-deprived mice in the BL compared to control mice (Control: nmice = 6; nspines = 5,385; SD: nmice = 6; nspines = 3,143) (A). Spine surface area was significantly greater in mushroom spines from sleep-deprived mice in the CEA (Control: nmice = 5; nspines = 1,768; SD: nmice = 5; nspines = 1,543), and no differences were observed for mushroom spine surface area in the LA (Control: nmice = 5; nspines = 1,217; SD: nmice = 5; nspines = 1,339) (A). Thin spine surface area was not significantly altered in the LA (Control: nmice = 5; nspines = 2,046; SD: nmice = 5; nspines = 2,525), CEA (Control: nmice = 5; nspines = 3,449; SD: nmice = 5; nspines = 3,207) or BL (Control: nmice = 6; nspines = 9,079; SD: nmice = 6; nspines = 5,645) (B). The surface area of stubby spines was significantly greater in spines from the CEA (Control: nmice = 5; nspines = 871; SD: nmice = 5; nspines = 878) of sleep-deprived mice, with no differences observed from stubby spines in the LA (Control: nmice = 5; nspines = 469; SD: nmice = 5; nspines = 618) or BL (Control: nmice = 6; nspines = 2,230; SD: nmice = 6; nspines = 1,343) (C). Spine volumes displayed largely similar changes, with significantly lower volume in the BL and greater volume in the CEA for mushroom spines in sleep-deprived mice (D), no significant difference in thin (E), and greater volume of stubby spines in the CEA of sleep-deprived mice (F). Box plots depict values for each group, statistical significance was determined using the Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney test with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

Lower densities of pCofilin and growth hormone immunoreactive cells in sleep deprived mice

Sleep-deprived mice displayed significantly lower density of pCofilin immunoreactive cells in the LA and BL amygdala nuclei compared to control mice (LA: p < 0.05 and BL: p < 0.03, Figure 5A). In comparison, no significant changes in density of pCofilin IR cells was detected in the CEA (Figure 5). A similar decrease was observed for densities of GH IR cells in the LA of sleep deprived mice (Figure 5D). No changes were detected for GH densities in the BL or CEA (Figure 5D).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Lower density of pCofilin and GH immunoreactive cells in the amygdala of sleep deprived mice. Sleep-deprived mice displayed significantly reduced numerical density of pCofilin immunoreactive cells in the lateral and basolateral amygdala nuclei, but not in the central nucleus compared to control mice (A). Representative 5x and 40x images of pCofilin labeling in the amygdala from control (B) and SD mice (C). 40x magnification inserts depict pCofilin labeling in the BL. Decreased density of GH immunoreactive cells was also detected in the LA of sleep deprived mice (D), but not in the BL or CEA. Representative 5x and 40x images of GH labeling in the amygdala from control (E) and SD mice (F). 40x magnification inserts depict GH labeling in the LA. Scale bars in yellow = 100 μm for 5x images, 50 μm for 40x inserts. All graphs reflect the mean for each group with n = 6 control and n = 6 sleep-deprived animals. Bar graphs depicting mean and 95% confidence interval of the density of immunoreactive cells. Each dot represents the value for an individual mouse calculated as density of immunoreactive cells per area (cells/mm2).

Discussion

Our results point to region and spine type specific dendritic spine changes during sleep in the amygdala. In summary, we observed evidence for selective strengthening of mushroom spines in the BL amygdala during sleep together with downscaling of thin and stubby spines in the LA and CEA regions. Despite several studies describing the effects of sleep deprivation in cortical areas and in the hippocampus (Yang et al., 2014; Havekes et al., 2016; Spano et al., 2019; Gisabella et al., 2020), our findings represent the first evidence for the effect of sleep deprivation on dendritic spines in the amygdala. Several lines of evidence point to a key role of the amygdala in sleep and emotional memory processing. For example, the amygdala is selectively activated during rapid eye movement sleep (REM) and is involved in consolidation of emotional memories (Goldstein and Walker, 2014; Genzel et al., 2015; Murkar and De Koninck, 2018). Furthermore, studies in human subjects indicate that REM sleep is essential for next-day social and emotional memory functioning (Goldstein and Walker, 2014). In particular, sleep is involved in regulating emotional reactivity, and amygdala function is dysregulated in sleep deprived subjects (Yoo et al., 2007), with sleep loss consistently contributing to emotional instability (Horne, 1985; Dinges et al., 1997). Taken together, our findings suggest that our evidence for upscaling in the BL amygdala may reflect the role of this area in consolidation of emotional memory during sleep through strengthening of mushroom spines typically associated with more stable, long-term memories, which may represent the convergence of sensory inputs with emotional valence in this area.

Differential effects of sleep deprivation on basolateral amygdala vs. lateral and central amygdala spines

We observed differential effects of sleep deprivation between amygdala nuclei. The selective decrease of mushroom spine density in the BL of sleep deprived mice, together with the increased neck length and decreased surface area and volume of these spines all point to upscaling in the BL during sleep. This is in sharp contrast to reports of synaptic downscaling during sleep reported in cortical regions and in the hippocampus (Cirelli, 2005; Maret et al., 2011; de Vivo et al., 2017; Spano et al., 2019; Gisabella et al., 2020). The specificity of these changes for mushroom spines supports strengthening of spines associated with long term memory storage during sleep. Furthermore, shorter spine neck length (as measured by head backbone length and neck backbone length) for mushroom spines in the BL of control mice provides additional support for synaptic upscaling, as shorter neck length is associated with faster synaptic transmission and greater synaptic strength (Araya et al., 2014). Similarly, greater spine surface area and volume in mushroom spines in the BL point to synaptic upscaling during sleep, as greater spine volume and surface area is associated with synaptic strength and density of glutamate receptors (Harris and Stevens, 1989; Noguchi et al., 2005; Kopec et al., 2007; Borczyk et al., 2019). Spine neck diameter is reported to be a key indicator of synaptic transmission as narrow neck diameters impede molecular diffusion from the spine head to the dendrite (Noguchi et al., 2005; Adrian et al., 2014, 2017). Furthermore, spine neck diameter has been reported to be positively correlated with spine head volume (Arellano et al., 2007). Our observed decrease of neck diameter in mushroom spines in the BL amygdala of sleep deprived mice suggests that spine neck diameter increases in these spines during sleep, thus contributing to enhanced diffusion and synaptic strength. Similar changes were observed for neck diameters of thin spines in the CEA, pointing to synaptic strengthening in this area.

In contrast, our findings in the LA and CEA point to synaptic downscaling in these areas. Greater densities of thin and stubby spines in sleep deprived mice in these regions indicate that downscaling selectively occurs in these two spine types. Greater neck diameters for thin spines in the LA and BL of sleep deprived mice suggests decreased diffusion for these spines during sleep, and thus weakened synaptic transmission. What may be behind these region-specific effects of sleep deprivation on amygdalar dendritic spines? Differential connectivity may contribute to the divergent effect of sleep deprivation between the BL vs. the LA and CEA areas. The LA receives sensory inputs from thalamic and cortical areas, serving as the sensory gateway to the amygdala (LeDoux et al., 1990a; Li et al., 1995). In comparison, the BL receives multimodal sensory inputs (LeDoux et al., 1990b; Uwano et al., 1995; Maren et al., 1996; Wilensky et al., 1999; LeDoux, 2000; Lucas et al., 2016; Hintiryan and Dong, 2022) as well as dopaminergic input from the VTA (Lutas et al., 2019; Tang et al., 2020), and noradrenergic input from the locus coeruleus (McCall et al., 2017), as well as input from the LA (Pitkanen et al., 1995). In turn, the BLA sends information to several areas including the CEA (Tovote et al., 2016; Hintiryan and Dong, 2022), the medial prefrontal cortex (McDonald, 1992; Cunningham et al., 2002; Cheriyan et al., 2016), the orbitofrontal cortex (Lichtenberg et al., 2017), the nucleus accumbens (Stuber et al., 2011; Wang et al., 2020). It is also the amygdala region that sends the largest amount of projections to areas CA1 and CA3 of the hippocampus (Benes and Berretta, 2000; Pitkanen et al., 2000). Within this context, our data suggest that synapses in the LA potentially formed from sensory information encoded during wakefulness are broadly downscaled during sleep, whereas synapses in the BL, where multimodal sensory information converges with information from areas involved in contextual representations and reward processing to form long-lasting representations (LeDoux et al., 1990b; Maren et al., 1996; Wilensky et al., 1999; LeDoux, 2000), are upscaled during sleep. These upscaled synapses in the BL may in turn contribute hard-wire contextual and multimodal sensory information with fear or reward response processes, resulting in heightened fear, anxiety or addiction related behaviors. Evidence for increased BL activation during REM sleep supports this hypothesis (Corsi-Cabrera et al., 2016; Hasegawa et al., 2022). Furthermore, the BL amygdala is at the hub of interconnected circuits activated during the replay of emotional memories during sleep (Chen and Wilson, 2017; Girardeau et al., 2017; Miyawaki and Mizuseki, 2022), suggesting that re-activation of BL synapses promotes strengthening of emotionally significant information in this region during sleep while broader sensory inputs in the LA are downscaled to make space for new connections, similar to the downscaling reported in cortical areas and the CA1 hippocampus (Cirelli, 2005; Maret et al., 2011; de Vivo et al., 2017; Spano et al., 2019; Gisabella et al., 2020). Sensory information in the LA that is transferred to long-term storage may occur in selective sets of synapses and/or may be transferred to the BL or other areas during sleep. Sleep deprivation may also allow more thin and stubby spines to form in response to additional environmental stimuli during the extended waking period, particularly in the LA. These excess spines may contribute to increased impulsivity and risk-taking behavior reported in subjects with decreased sleep (Demos et al., 2016; Brunet et al., 2020).

Association of cofilin activity with decreased basolateral amygdala spines in sleep deprived mice

We analyzed neurons expressing pCofilin as a first step in examining molecular pathways that may be involved in dendritic spine changes in sleep deprived mice. Cofilin is modulated by synaptic plasticity (Chen et al., 2007; Pontrello et al., 2012) and is localized in the postsynaptic density where it is believed to function as a key regulator of actin dynamics regulating spine morphology and spine length (Andrianantoandro and Pollard, 2006; Hotulainen et al., 2009). Previous studies suggest that suppression of cofilin activity by phosporylation is important for the stabilization of mature spines, as this prevents cofilin from participating in downregulating these spines by cleaving spine actin filaments (Shi et al., 2009). Furthermore, a recent study demonstrated that 5 h of sleep deprivation results in reduced phosphorylation of cofilin in the hippocampus, which results in greater cofilin activity and in turn enhanced (Havekes et al., 2016). In turn, suppressing cofilin activity prevented the sleep deprivation induced reduction of dendritic spines in the hippocampus (Havekes et al., 2016). Our data showing decreased density of pCofilin neurons in the BL and LA areas of sleep deprived mice indicate increased cofilin activity in sleep deprived mice. Taken together with decreased density of mushroom spines in the BL, this suggests that cofilin activity is enhanced in sleep dperived mice due to reduced phosphorylation of cofilin, and this promotes cofilin cleavage of actin filaments in spines, contributing to synaptic downscaling. Reduced density of pCofilin in the LA area despite decreased spine density in this region may point to subtype specific changes in dendritic spines that are not detected in our broad sampling. Future studies into memory trace specific and neuronal subtype specific dendritic spine changes will provide insight into this process.

Contrasting effects of sleep deprivation on amygdalar vs. hippocampal spines

Our observed effects of sleep deprivation on dendritic spines in the BL are opposite to our previous observations in the hippocampus (Gisabella et al., 2020). Specifically, these results suggest that dendritic spines are broadly downscaled in the hippocampus during sleep but are instead strengthened in the basolateral amygdala. A closer examination points to a complex regulation of dendritic spines in both regions, however. In our previous study focused on sector CA1 of the hippocampus, the effects of sleep deprivation on hippocampal spines were region and branch specific (Gisabella et al., 2020). Studies from other groups also point to hippocampal subregion and branch specific changes, with evidence for upscaling of spines during sleep in the dentate gyrus and in the very distal apical branches of CA1 neurons (Raven et al., 2019; Bolsius et al., 2022). Similarly, our findings in the amygdala point to region and spine type specific changes, with evidence for upscaling of mushroom and stubby spines in the BL and downscaling of thin and stubby spines in the LA and CEA (Figure 6). These region-specific differences between the hippocampus and amygdala may reflect differential connectivity of these areas and their roles in sleep processes as well as memory replay during sleep. Stress is a factor that may contribute to differences between the amygdala and hippocampus. Chronic stress differentially effects on dendritic spines in these regions, with decreased spines in the hippocampus and increased spines in the basolateral amygdala (Vyas et al., 2002). This suggests that there are underlying molecular processes behind dendritic spine regulation between these areas that are involved in sleep as well as stress response.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Summary of dendritic spine changes during sleep in the mouse amygdala. The diagram represents a summary of the working hypothesis of dendritic spine changes during sleep in the mouse amygdala based on our data from sleep deprived mice. Spine densities for each region are indicated by the branch with multiple spines in the upper part of each panel, and spine morphological subtypes are indicated by corresponding shapes (thin, subby, mushroom) in each panel. Mushroom, thin, and stubby spines in the BL are upscaled during sleep through increased spine density and corresponding decreases in neck diameter, neck length, and increased volume. In comparison, thin and stubby dendritic spines in the LA and CEA are largely downscaled during sleep through decreased spine density along with more selective changes in neck and head properties.

In addition, sleep deprivation may serve as a stressor itself. Although our method of sleep deprivation mimicking gentle handling is designed to minimize stress, sleep deprivation itself is a stressor by nature. Thus, the effects of sleep deprivation may reflect, in part, the effect of stress on dendritic spines. However, our observed effects of sleep deprivation on dendritic spines in the amygdala (Figure 6) and hippocampus (Gisabella et al., 2020) overall are opposite to the reported effect of chronic stress on dendritic spines in these regions (Vyas et al., 2002). Similarly, our observed decreased density of GH immunoreactive cells in the LA of sleep deprived mice is in contrast to the increased GH expression reported in the amygdala following chronic stress (Meyer et al., 2014). Specifically, chronic stress was reported to increase dendritic spines in the amygdala, whereas our data shows that sleep deprivation results in downscaling of dendritic spines in this region. This suggests that sleep is involved in synaptic upscaling of mushroom spines in the basolateral amygdala. This synapse promoting effect of sleep in the amygdala may potentially associated with effects of slow wave sleep in this region, as this stage is more predominant in the first half of the inactive period whereas REM sleep is more prevalent in the latter half (Lacroix et al., 2018). Furthermore, SWS and sleep are involved in promoting memory consolidation processes (Stickgold et al., 2000; Miyamoto et al., 2017). Our findings consisting of spine type and region-specific upscaling or downscaling suggests that our results reflect complex effects of sleep that utilize overlapping molecular pathways which may also be utilized for dendritic spine regulation during stress.

Relevance to psychiatric disorders

Several lines of evidence point to the involvement of synaptic alterations in the amygdala in memory consolidation and sleep disturbances in psychiatric disorders including PTSD and mood disorders (Ross et al., 1989; Leibenluft et al., 1996; Colombo et al., 1999; Bremner et al., 2000; Frodl et al., 2002; Drevets, 2003; Jackson et al., 2003; Lange and Irle, 2004; Harvey et al., 2005; Armitage, 2007; Benedetti et al., 2007; Raskind et al., 2007; Thompson et al., 2008; Karolewicz et al., 2009; Tottenham et al., 2010; Harb et al., 2012; McCarthy et al., 2012, 2013, 2016, 2019; Bunney and Bunney, 2013; Burton et al., 2013; Li et al., 2013; McClung, 2013; Palagini et al., 2013; Medina et al., 2014; Brownlow et al., 2015; Bunney et al., 2015; Pagani et al., 2016; Pantazopoulos et al., 2017, 2018; Nudell et al., 2019). Our data suggesting that mushroom spines in the amygdala are upscaled during sleep is in line with preclinical studies suggesting that sleep deprivation early on after a traumatic experience may alleviate the strength or consolidation of traumatic memories that contribute to development of PTSD (Wagner et al., 2006; Kuriyama et al., 2010; Cohen et al., 2012). In our previous studies, we demonstrated that chronic stress increases ghrelin-growth hormone signaling (Meyer et al., 2014) and in turn growth hormone expression increases dendritic spine density in the amygdala and predisposes amygdala neurons to encode fear memories (Gisabella et al., 2016), suggesting that stress may increase dendritic spines in this region in part through elevated growth hormone expression. In turn, chronic stress in rodents results in decreased dendritic spines in the hippocampus in comparison to increased spines in the amygdala, suggesting that growth hormone may be involved in this process (Vyas et al., 2002) and that the approach of sleep deprivation following a traumatic experience may alleviate the strength of the traumatic memory by disrupting the upscaling of dendritic spines in the BL that may be involved in consolidation of the traumatic memory. However, our data also suggests that sleep deprivation would increase dendritic spines in the LA and in the CEA, a key area involved in fear response output signaling. This suggests that sleep, in part, may contribute to resilience to stress by downscaling spines in these areas. Our evidence suggesting that mushroom spines in the BL are upscaled during sleep also suggests that sleep disturbances in schizophrenia may interfere with this process and contribute to synaptic alterations in this disorder. Several studies have reported decreases in sleep spindles together with memory consolidation deficits in patients with schizophrenia (Ferrarelli et al., 2007; Manoach et al., 2010, 2014). Furthermore, recent report points to altered diurnal molecular expression rhythms in the brain of subjects with SZ (Seney et al., 2019). In turn, deficits in the synaptic molecule synaptophysin, and in the synapse stabilizing structures perineuronal nets have been reported in the amygdala of subjects with schizophrenia (Pantazopoulos et al., 2010, 2015; Varea et al., 2012). Sleep and circadian rhythm disturbances in this disorder may thus contribute to impairment in emotional memory consolidation processes in this region differentially from the enhancement of emotional memories in PTSD.

Limitations

The lack of dendritic branch and segment specific analysis in our study represents an important limitation. The complex organization of neurons and their dendritic branches in the amygdala compared to other regions made it challenging to follow branches from individual neurons and to establish sampling strategies. Neurons and their respective dendrites in other regions such as the hippocampus or cortex are organized in layers, resulting in reduced overlapping of dendritic processes that allows for more selective sampling. However, this was not possible in the amygdala. However, obtaining information regarding dendritic spines measures in this region in sleep-deprived animals using viral vector labeling in 3-dimensional quantification was important. Therefore, we analyzed all clearly visible viral vector labeled dendritic branches in our samples. Lack of cell type specificity for the dendritic spines sampled is another potential limiting factor in understanding how dendritic spines may be altered during sleep in specific populations of excitatory and inhibitory neurons in the amygdala. Our current study represents a broad overview of dendritic spine changes in sleep deprived animals in this region, similar to prior studies from several groups that examined spines in broad neuronal populations rather than selective neuronal subtypes (Maret et al., 2011; Raven et al., 2018, 2019; de Vivo et al., 2019; Spano et al., 2019; Gisabella et al., 2020). Future studies will focus on cell type specific and memory trace specific changes in amygdalar dendritic spines in sleep-deprived mice.

Conclusion

Overall, our data represent the first study describing the effect of sleep deprivation on dendritic spines in the amygdala and provide insight into the morphological changes of dendritic spines during sleep in this region. Future studies into the molecular pathways underlying these region and spine type specific changes may provide insight into the consolidation of emotional memories during sleep and how this process may be affected in psychiatric disorders.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by University of Mississippi Medical Center Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.

Author contributions

BG designed the studies, analyzed data, and wrote the manuscript. LR contributed to study design, collected data, analyzed data, and wrote the manuscript. MT data collection and manuscript preparation. JB, CY, RB, LL, and JV contributed to data collection. HP contributed to study design, data analysis, and manuscript preparation. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was funded by the National Institute of Mental Health (MH117460).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Adrian, M., Kusters, R., Storm, C., Hoogenraad, C. C., Kapitein, L. C. (2017). Probing the interplay between dendritic spine morphology and membrane-bound diffusion. Biophys. J. 113, 2261–2270. doi: 10.1016/j.bpj.2017.06.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adrian, M., Kusters, R., Wierenga, C. J., Storm, C., Hoogenraad, C. C., Kapitein, L. C., et al. (2014). Barriers in the brain: resolving dendritic spine morphology and compartmentalization. Front. Neuroanat. 8, 142. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2014.00142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aguilar, D. D., Strecker, R. E., Basheer, R., McNally, J. M. (2020). Alterations in sleep, sleep spindle, and EEG power in mGluR5 knockout mice. J. Neurophysiol. 123, 22–33. doi: 10.1152/jn.00532.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrianantoandro, E., Pollard, T. D. (2006). Mechanism of actin filament turnover by severing and nucleation at different concentrations of ADF/cofilin. Mol. Cell 24, 13–23. doi: 10.1016/j.molcel.2006.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Araya, R., Vogels, T. P., Yuste, R. (2014). Activity-dependent dendritic spine neck changes are correlated with synaptic strength. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 111, E2895–2904. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1321869111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arellano, J. I., Benavides-Piccione, R., Defelipe, J., Yuste, R. (2007). Ultrastructure of dendritic spines: correlation between synaptic and spine morphologies. Front. Neurosci. 1, 131–143. doi: 10.3389/neuro.01.1.1.010.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armitage, R. (2007). Sleep and circadian rhythms in mood disorders. Acta. Psychiatr. Scand. 433, 104–115. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.00968.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baglioni, C., Spiegelhalder, K., Lombardo, C., Riemann, D. (2010). Sleep and emotions: a focus on insomnia. Sleep. Med. Rev. 14, 227–238. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baglioni, C., Spiegelhalder, K., Regen, W., Feige, B., Nissen, C., Lombardo, C., et al. (2014). Insomnia disorder is associated with increased amygdala reactivity to insomnia-related stimuli. Sleep 37, 1907–1917. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benarroch, E. E. (2015). The amygdala: functional organization and involvement in neurologic disorders. Neurology 84, 313–324. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedetti, F., Barbini, B., Colombo, C., Smeraldi, E. (2007). Chronotherapeutics in a psychiatric ward. Sleep Med. Rev. 11, 509–522. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2007.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benes, F. M., Berretta, S. (2000). Amygdalo-entorhinal inputs to the hippocampal formation in relation to schizophrenia. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 911, 293–304. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2000.tb06733.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, S. J., Duncan, J., Lawrence, A. D. (2004). State anxiety modulation of the amygdala response to unattended threat-related stimuli. J. Neurosci. 24, 10364–10368. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2550-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bolsius, Y. G., Meerlo, P., Kas, M. J., Abel, T., Havekes, R. (2022). Sleep deprivation reduces the density of individual spine subtypes in a branch-specific fashion in CA1 neurons. J. Sleep Res. 31, e13438. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13438

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borczyk, M., Sliwinska, M. A., Caly, A., Bernas, T., Radwanska, K. (2019). Neuronal plasticity affects correlation between the size of dendritic spine and its postsynaptic density. Sci. Rep. 9, 1693. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-38412-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bremner, J. D., Narayan, M., Anderson, E. R., Staib, L. H., Miller, H. L., Charney, D. S., et al. (2000). Hippocampal volume reduction in major depression. Am. J. Psychiatr. 157, 115–118. doi: 10.1176/ajp.157.1.115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bremner, J. D., Vermetten, E., Schmahl, C., Vaccarino, V., Vythilingam, M., Afzal, N., et al. (2005). Positron emission tomographic imaging of neural correlates of a fear acquisition and extinction paradigm in women with childhood sexual-abuse-related post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychol. Med. 35, 791–806. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704003290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brownlow, J. A., Harb, G. C., Ross, R. J. (2015). Treatment of sleep disturbances in post-traumatic stress disorder: a review of the literature. Curr. Psychiatr. Rep. 17, 41. doi: 10.1007/s11920-015-0587-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunet, J. F., McNeil, J., Doucet, E., Forest, G. (2020). The association between REM sleep and decision-making: Supporting evidences. Physiol. Behav. 225, 113109. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bunney, B. G., Bunney, W. E. (2013). Mechanisms of rapid antidepressant effects of sleep deprivation therapy: clock genes and circadian rhythms. Biol. Psychiatr. 73, 1164–1171. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.07.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bunney, B. G., Li, J. Z., Walsh, D. M., Stein, R., Vawter, M. P., Cartagena, P., et al. (2015). Circadian dysregulation of clock genes: clues to rapid treatments in major depressive disorder. Mol. Psychiatr. 20, 48–55. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgdorf, J. S., Vitaterna, M. H., Olker, C. J., Song, E. J., Christian, E. P., Sorensen, L., et al. (2019). NMDAR activation regulates the daily rhythms of sleep and mood. Sleep 42, 135. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsz135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burton, C., McKinstry, B., Szentagotai Tatar, A., Serrano-Blanco, A., Pagliari, C., Wolters, M., et al. (2013). Activity monitoring in patients with depression: a systematic review. J. Affect Disord. 145, 21–28. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calabrese, B., Saffin, J. M., Halpain, S. (2014). Activity-dependent dendritic spine shrinkage and growth involve downregulation of cofilin via distinct mechanisms. PLoS ONE 9, e94787. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0094787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, L. Y., Rex, C. S., Casale, M. S., Gall, C. M., Lynch, G. (2007). Changes in synaptic morphology accompany actin signaling during LTP. J. Neurosci. 27, 5363–5372. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0164-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Z., Wilson, M. A. (2017). Deciphering neural codes of memory during sleep. Trends Neurosci. 40, 260–275. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2017.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheriyan, J., Kaushik, M. K., Ferreira, A. N., Sheets, P. L. (2016). Specific targeting of the basolateral amygdala to projectionally defined pyramidal neurons in prelimbic and infralimbic cortex. eNeuro 3, 2. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0002-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cichon, J., Sun, C., Chen, B., Jiang, M., Chen, X. A., Sun, Y., et al. (2012). Cofilin aggregation blocks intracellular trafficking and induces synaptic loss in hippocampal neurons. J. Biol. Chem. 287, 3919–3929. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M111.301911

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cirelli, C. (2005). A molecular window on sleep: changes in gene expression between sleep and wakefulness. Neuroscientist 11, 63–74. doi: 10.1177/1073858404270900

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, S., Kozlovsky, N., Matar, M. A., Kaplan, Z., Zohar, J., Cohen, H., et al. (2012). Post-exposure sleep deprivation facilitates correctly timed interactions between glucocorticoid and adrenergic systems, which attenuate traumatic stress responses. Neuropsychopharmacology 37, 2388–2404. doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.94

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colombo, C., Benedetti, F., Barbini, B., Campori, E., Smeraldi, E. (1999). Rate of switch from depression into mania after therapeutic sleep deprivation in bipolar depression. Psychiatr. Res. 86, 267–270. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(99)00036-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corsi-Cabrera, M., Velasco, F., Del Rio-Portilla, Y., Armony, J. L., Trejo-Martinez, D., Guevara, M. A., et al. (2016). Human amygdala activation during rapid eye movements of rapid eye movement sleep: an intracranial study. J. Sleep Res. 25, 576–582. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cunningham, M. G., Bhattacharyya, S., Benes, F. M. (2002). Amygdalo-cortical sprouting continues into early adulthood: implications for the development of normal and abnormal function during adolescence. J. Comp. Neurol 453, 116–130. doi: 10.1002/cne.10376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Vivo, L., Bellesi, M., Marshall, W., Bushong, E. A., Ellisman, M. H., Tononi, G., et al. (2017). Ultrastructural evidence for synaptic scaling across the wake/sleep cycle. Science 355, 507–510. doi: 10.1126/science.aah5982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Vivo, L., Nagai, H., De Wispelaere, N., Spano, G. M., Marshall, W., Bellesi, M., et al. (2019). Evidence for sleep-dependent synaptic renormalization in mouse pups. Sleep 42, 184. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsz184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demos, K. E., Hart, C. N., Sweet, L. H., Mailloux, K. A., Trautvetter, J., Williams, S. E., et al. (2016). Partial sleep deprivation impacts impulsive action but not impulsive decision-making. Physiol. Behav. 164, 214–219. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinges, D. F., Pack, F., Williams, K., Gillen, K. A., Powell, J. W., Ott, G. E., et al. (1997). Cumulative sleepiness, mood disturbance, and psychomotor vigilance performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4-5 hours per night. Sleep 20, 267–277.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Dolsen, M. R., Soehner, A. M., Harvey, A. G. (2017). Pro-inflammatory cytokines, mood, and sleep in interepisode bipolar disorder and insomnia: a pilot study with implications for psychosocial interventions. Psychosom Med. 80, 87. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dorph-Petersen, K. A., Delevich, K. M., Marcsisin, M. J., Zhang, W., Sampson, A. R., Gundersen, H. J., et al. (2009). Pyramidal neuron number in layer 3 of primary auditory cortex of subjects with schizophrenia. Brain Res. 1285, 42–57. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.06.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drevets, W. C. (2003). Neuroimaging abnormalities in the amygdala in mood disorders. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 985, 420–444. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2003.tb07098.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dudai, Y., Karni, A., Born, J. (2015). The consolidation and transformation of memory. Neuron 88, 20–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duman, R. S., Aghajanian, G. K. (2012). Synaptic dysfunction in depression: potential therapeutic targets. Science 338, 68–72. doi: 10.1126/science.1222939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebdlahad, S., Nofzinger, E. A., James, J. A., Buysse, D. J., Price, J. C., Germain, A., et al. (2013). Comparing neural correlates of REM sleep in posttraumatic stress disorder and depression: a neuroimaging study. Psychiatry Res. 214, 422–428. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrarelli, F., Huber, R., Peterson, M. J., Massimini, M., Murphy, M., Riedner, B. A., et al. (2007). Reduced sleep spindle activity in schizophrenia patients. Am. J. Psychiatr. 164, 483–492. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.3.483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrarelli, F., Tononi, G. (2017). Reduced sleep spindle activity point to a TRN-MD thalamus-PFC circuit dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 180, 36–43. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2016.05.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ford, D. E., Kamerow, D. B. (1989). Epidemiologic study of sleep disturbances and psychiatric disorders. An opportunity for prevention? JAMA 262, 1479–1484. doi: 10.1001/jama.1989.03430110069030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forrest, M. P., Parnell, E., Penzes, P. (2018). Dendritic structural plasticity and neuropsychiatric disease. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 215–234. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2018.16

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frodl, T., Meisenzahl, E., Zetzsche, T., Bottlender, R., Born, C., Groll, C., et al. (2002). Enlargement of the amygdala in patients with a first episode of major depression. Biol. Psychiatr. 51, 708–714. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01359-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Genzel, L., Spoormaker, V. I., Konrad, B. N., Dresler, M. (2015). The role of rapid eye movement sleep for amygdala-related memory processing. Neurobiol. Learn Mem. 122, 110–121. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2015.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Germain, A. (2013). Sleep disturbances as the hallmark of PTSD: where are we now? Am. J. Psychiatr. 170, 372–382. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12040432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Girardeau, G., Inema, I., Buzsaki, G. (2017). Reactivations of emotional memory in the hippocampus-amygdala system during sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 1634–1642. doi: 10.1038/nn.4637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gisabella, B., Babu, J., Valeri, J., Rexrode, L., Pantazopoulos, H. (2021). Sleep and memory consolidation dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: evidence for the involvement of extracellular matrix molecules. Front. Neurosci. 15, 646678. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.646678

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gisabella, B., Farah, S., Peng, X., Burgos-Robles, A., Lim, S. H., Goosens, K. A., et al. (2016). Growth hormone biases amygdala network activation after fear learning. Transl. Psychiatry 6, e960. doi: 10.1038/tp.2016.203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gisabella, B., Scammell, T., Bandaru, S. S., Saper, C. B. (2020). Regulation of hippocampal dendritic spines following sleep deprivation. J. Comp. Neurol. 528, 380–388. doi: 10.1002/cne.24764

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glausier, J. R., Lewis, D. A. (2013). Dendritic spine pathology in schizophrenia. Neuroscience 251, 90–107. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.04.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldstein, A. N., Walker, M. P. (2014). The role of sleep in emotional brain function. Annu Rev. Clin. Psychol. 10, 679–708. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gruber, J., Miklowitz, D. J., Harvey, A. G., Frank, E., Kupfer, D., Thase, M. E., et al. (2011). Sleep matters: sleep functioning and course of illness in bipolar disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 134, 416–420. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harb, G. C., Thompson, R., Ross, R. J., Cook, J. M. (2012). Combat-related PTSD nightmares and imagery rehearsal: nightmare characteristics and relation to treatment outcome. J. Trauma Stress 25, 511–518. doi: 10.1002/jts.21748

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, K. M., Stevens, J. K. (1989). Dendritic spines of CA 1 pyramidal cells in the rat hippocampus: serial electron microscopy with reference to their biophysical characteristics. J. Neurosci. 9, 2982–2997. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-08-02982.1989

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harvey, A. G., Schmidt, D. A., Scarna, A., Semler, C. N., Goodwin, G. M. (2005). Sleep-related functioning in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder, patients with insomnia, and subjects without sleep problems. Am. J. Psychiatr. 162, 50–57. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.1.50

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasegawa, E., Miyasaka, A., Sakurai, K., Cherasse, Y., Li, Y., Sakurai, T., et al. (2022). Rapid eye movement sleep is initiated by basolateral amygdala dopamine signaling in mice. Science 375, 994–1000. doi: 10.1126/science.abl6618

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Havekes, R., Park, A. J., Tudor, J. C., Luczak, V. G., Hansen, R. T., Ferri, S. L., et al. (2016). Sleep deprivation causes memory deficits by negatively impacting neuronal connectivity in hippocampal area CA1. Elife 5, e13424. doi: 10.7554/eLife.13424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hines, D. J., Schmitt, L. I., Hines, R. M., Moss, S. J., Haydon, P. G. (2013). Antidepressant effects of sleep deprivation require astrocyte-dependent adenosine mediated signaling. Transl. Psychiatry 3, e212. doi: 10.1038/tp.2012.136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hintiryan, H., Dong, H. W. (2022). Brain networks of connectionally unique basolateral amygdala cell types. Neurosci Insights 17, 26331055221080175. doi: 10.1177/26331055221080175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horne, J. A. (1985). Sleep function, with particular reference to sleep deprivation. Ann. Clin. Res. 17, 199–208.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hotulainen, P., Llano, O., Smirnov, S., Tanhuanpää, K., Faix, J., Rivera, C., et al. (2009). Defining mechanisms of actin polymerization and depolymerization during dendritic spine morphogenesis. J. Cell Biol. 185, 323–339. doi: 10.1083/jcb.200809046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inagaki, S., Kawai, Y., Matsuzaki, T., Shiosaka, S., Tohyama, M. (1983). Precise terminal fields of the descending somatostatinergic neuron system from the amygdaloid complex of the rat. J. Hirnforsch. 24, 345–356.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Jackson, A., Cavanagh, J., Scott, J. (2003). A systematic review of manic and depressive prodromes. J. Affect. Disord. 74, 209–217. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00266-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johansen, J. P., Cain, C. K., Ostroff, L. E., LeDoux, J. E. (2011). Molecular mechanisms of fear learning and memory. Cell 147, 509–524. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karolewicz, B., Szebeni, K., Gilmore, T., Maciag, D., Stockmeier, C. A., Ordway, G. A., et al. (2009). Elevated levels of NR2A and PSD-95 in the lateral amygdala in depression. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 12, 143–153. doi: 10.1017/S1461145708008985

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koffel, E., Khawaja, I. S., Germain, A. (2016). Sleep disturbances in posttraumatic stress disorder: updated review and implications for treatment. Psychiatr Ann. 46, 173–176. doi: 10.3928/00485713-20160125-01

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kopec, C. D., Real, E., Kessels, H. W., Malinow, R. (2007). GluR1 links structural and functional plasticity at excitatory synapses. J. Neurosci. 27, 13706–13718. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3503-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krakow, B., Artar, A., Warner, T. D., Melendrez, D., Johnston, L., Hollifield, M., et al. (2000). Sleep disorder, depression, and suicidality in female sexual assault survivors. Crisis 21, 163–170. doi: 10.1027//0227-5910.21.4.163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krystal, A. D., Thakur, M., Roth, T. (2008). Sleep disturbance in psychiatric disorders: effects on function and quality of life in mood disorders, alcoholism, and schizophrenia. Ann. Clin. Psychiatry 20, 39–46. doi: 10.1080/10401230701844661

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krystal, J. H., Pietrzak, R. H., Rosenheck, R. A., Cramer, J. A., Vessicchio, J., Jones, K. M., et al. (2016). Sleep disturbance in chronic military-related PTSD: clinical impact and response to adjunctive risperidone in the Veterans Affairs cooperative study #504. J Clin Psychiatry 77, 483–491. doi: 10.4088/JCP.14m09585

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuriyama, K., Soshi, T., Kim, Y. (2010). Sleep deprivation facilitates extinction of implicit fear generalization and physiological response to fear. Biol. Psychiatr. 68, 991–998. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.08.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lacroix, M. M., De Lavilléon, S., Lefort, G., El Kanbi, J., Bagur, K., Laventure, S., et al. (2018). Improved sleep scoring in mice reveals human-like stages. bioRxiv 7, 489005. doi: 10.1101/489005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lange, C., Irle, E. (2004). Enlarged amygdala volume and reduced hippocampal volume in young women with major depression. Psychol. Med. 34, 1059–1064. doi: 10.1017/S0033291703001806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeDoux, J. (2007). The amygdala. Curr. Biol. 17, R868–874. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 155–184. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeDoux, J. E., Cicchetti, P., Xagoraris, A., Romanski, L. M. (1990a). The lateral amygdaloid nucleus: sensory interface of the amygdala in fear conditioning. J. Neurosci. 10, 1062–1069. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-04-01062.1990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeDoux, J. E., Farb, C., Ruggiero, D. A. (1990b). Topographic organization of neurons in the acoustic thalamus that project to the amygdala. J. Neurosci. 10, 1043–1054. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-04-01043.1990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leibenluft, E., Albert, P. S., Rosenthal, N. E., Wehr, T. A. (1996). Relationship between sleep and mood in patients with rapid-cycling bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Res. 63, 161–168. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(96)02854-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, J. Z., Bunney, B. G., Meng, F., Hagenauer, M. H., Walsh, D. M., Vawter, M. P., et al. (2013). Circadian patterns of gene expression in the human brain and disruption in major depressive disorder. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 110, 9950–9955. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1305814110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, W., Ma, L., Yang, G., Gan, W. B. (2017). REM sleep selectively prunes and maintains new synapses in development and learning. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 427–437. doi: 10.1038/nn.4479

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X. F., Phillips, R., LeDoux, J. E. (1995). NMDA and non-NMDA receptors contribute to synaptic transmission between the medial geniculate body and the lateral nucleus of the amygdala. Exp. Brain Res. 105, 87–100. doi: 10.1007/BF00242185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lichtenberg, N. T., Pennington, Z. T., Holley, S. M., Greenfield, V. Y., Cepeda, C., Levine, M. S., et al. (2017). Basolateral amygdala to orbitofrontal cortex projections enable cue-triggered reward expectations. J. Neurosci. 37, 8374–8384. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0486-17.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Licznerski, P., Duman, R. S. (2013). Remodeling of axo-spinous synapses in the pathophysiology and treatment of depression. Neuroscience 251, 33–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.09.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, X., Yang, L., Wellman, L. L., Tang, X., Sanford, L. D. (2009). GABAergic antagonism of the central nucleus of the amygdala attenuates reductions in rapid eye movement sleep after inescapable footshock stress. Sleep 32, 888–896. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.7.888

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lucas, E. K., Jegarl, A. M., Morishita, H., Clem, R. L. (2016). Multimodal and site-specific plasticity of amygdala parvalbumin interneurons after fear learning. Neuron 91, 629–643. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.06.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutas, A., Kucukdereli, H., Alturkistani, O., Carty, C., Sugden, A. U., Fernando, K., et al. (2019). State-specific gating of salient cues by midbrain dopaminergic input to basal amygdala. Nat. Neurosci. 22, 1820–1833. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0506-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDonald, M. L., Alhassan, J., Newman, J. T., Richard, M., Gu, H., Kelly, R. M., et al. (2017). Selective Loss of Smaller Spines in Schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 174, 586–594. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.16070814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manoach, D. S., Demanuele, C., Wamsley, E. J., Vangel, M., Montrose, D. M., Miewald, J., et al. (2014). Sleep spindle deficits in antipsychotic-naive early course schizophrenia and in non-psychotic first-degree relatives. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 762. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00762

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manoach, D. S., Thakkar, K. N., Stroynowski, E., Ely, A., McKinley, S. K., Wamsley, E., et al. (2010). Reduced overnight consolidation of procedural learning in chronic medicated schizophrenia is related to specific sleep stages. J. Psychiatr. Res. 44, 112–120. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maren, S., Aharonov, G., Fanselow, M. S. (1996). Retrograde abolition of conditional fear after excitotoxic lesions in the basolateral amygdala of rats: absence of a temporal gradient. Behav. Neurosci. 110, 718–726. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.110.4.718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maret, S., Faraguna, U., Nelson, A. B., Cirelli, C., Tononi, G. (2011). Sleep and waking modulate spine turnover in the adolescent mouse cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 1418–1420. doi: 10.1038/nn.2934

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCall, J. G., Siuda, E. R., Bhatti, D. L., Lawson, L. A., McElligott, Z. A., Stuber, G. D., et al. (2017). Locus coeruleus to basolateral amygdala noradrenergic projections promote anxiety-like behavior. Elife 6, 14. doi: 10.7554/eLife.18247.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarthy, M. J., Le Roux, M. J., Wei, H., Beesley, S., Kelsoe, J. R., Welsh, D. K., et al. (2016). Calcium channel genes associated with bipolar disorder modulate lithium's amplification of circadian rhythms. Neuropharmacology 101, 439–448. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2015.10.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarthy, M. J., Nievergelt, C. M., Kelsoe, J. R., Welsh, D. K. (2012). A survey of genomic studies supports association of circadian clock genes with bipolar disorder spectrum illnesses and lithium response. PLoS ONE 7, e32091. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0032091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarthy, M. J., Wei, H., Marnoy, Z., Darvish, R. M., McPhie, D. L., Cohen, B. M., et al. (2013). Genetic and clinical factors predict lithium's effects on PER2 gene expression rhythms in cells from bipolar disorder patients. Transl. Psychiatry 3, e318. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.90

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarthy, M. J., Wei, H., Nievergelt, C. M., Stautland, A., Maihofer, A. X., Welsh, D. K., et al. (2019). Chronotype and cellular circadian rhythms predict the clinical response to lithium maintenance treatment in patients with bipolar disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 44, 620–628. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0273-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McClung, C. A. (2013). How might circadian rhythms control mood? Let me count the ways. Biol. Psychiatry. 74, 242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonald, A. J. (1992). Projection neurons of the basolateral amygdala: a correlative Golgi and retrograde tract tracing study. Brain Res. Bull. 28, 179–185. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(92)90177-Y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGaugh, J. L. (1999). The perseveration-consolidation hypothesis: Mueller and Pilzecker, 1900. Brain. Res. Bull. 50, 445–446. doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(99)00126-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Medina, A. B., Lechuga, D. A., Escandon, O. S., Moctezuma, J. V. (2014). Update of sleep alterations in depression. Sleep Sci. 7, 165–169. doi: 10.1016/j.slsci.2014.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, R. M., Burgos-Robles, A., Liu, E., Correia, S. S., Goosens, K. A. (2014). A ghrelin-growth hormone axis drives stress-induced vulnerability to enhanced fear. Mol. Psychiatr. 19, 1284–1294. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyamoto, D., Hirai, D., Murayama, M. (2017). The roles of cortical slow waves in synaptic plasticity and memory consolidation. Front. Neural. Circuits 11, 92. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2017.00092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyawaki, H., Mizuseki, K. (2022). De novo inter-regional coactivations of preconfigured local ensembles support memory. Nat. Commun. 13, 1272. doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-28929-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Motomura, Y., Kitamura, S., Nakazaki, K., Oba, K., Katsunuma, R., Terasawa, Y., et al. (2017). Recovery from unrecognized sleep loss accumulated in daily life improved mood regulation via prefrontal suppression of amygdala activity. Front. Neurol. 8, 306. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murkar, A. L. A., De Koninck, J. (2018). Consolidative mechanisms of emotional processing in REM sleep and PTSD. Sleep Med. Rev. 41, 173–184. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2018.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, G., Harvey, A. (2010). Circadian rhythms and sleep in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 12, 459–472. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2010.00843.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noguchi, J., Hayama, T., Watanabe, S., Ucar, H., Yagishita, S., Takahashi, N., et al. (2016). State-dependent diffusion of actin-depolymerizing factor/cofilin underlies the enlargement and shrinkage of dendritic spines. Sci. Rep. 6, 32897. doi: 10.1038/srep32897

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noguchi, J., Matsuzaki, M., Ellis-Davies, G. C., Kasai, H. (2005). Spine-neck geometry determines NMDA receptor-dependent Ca2+ signaling in dendrites. Neuron 46, 609–622. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.03.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nudell, V., Wei, H., Nievergelt, C., Maihofer, A. X., Shilling, P., Alda, M., et al. (2019). Entrainment of circadian rhythms to temperature reveals amplitude deficits in fibroblasts from patients with bipolar disorder and possible links to calcium channels. Mol. Neuropsychiatr. 5, 115–124. doi: 10.1159/000497354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohayon, M. M., Shapiro, C. M. (2000). Sleep disturbances and psychiatric disorders associated with posttraumatic stress disorder in the general population. Compr. Psychiatr. 41, 469–478. doi: 10.1053/comp.2000.16568

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pagani, L., St Clair, P. A., Teshiba, T. M., Service, S. K., Fears, S. C., Araya, C., et al. (2016). Genetic contributions to circadian activity rhythm and sleep pattern phenotypes in pedigrees segregating for severe bipolar disorder. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 113, E754–761. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1513525113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palagini, L., Baglioni, C., Ciapparelli, A., Gemignani, A., Riemann, D. (2013). REM sleep dysregulation in depression: state of the art. Sleep Med. Rev. 17, 377–390. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2012.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pantazopoulos, H., Gamble, K., Stork, O., Amir, S. (2018). Circadian rhythms in regulation of brain processes and role in psychiatric disorders. Neural. Plast. 2018, 5892657. doi: 10.1155/2018/5892657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pantazopoulos, H., Markota, M., Jaquet, F., Ghosh, D., Wallin, A., Santos, A., et al. (2015). Aggrecan and chondroitin-6-sulfate abnormalities in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a postmortem study on the amygdala. Transl. Psychiatry 5, e496. doi: 10.1038/tp.2014.128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pantazopoulos, H., Wiseman, J. T., Markota, M., Ehrenfeld, L., Berretta, S. (2017). Decreased numbers of somatostatin-expressing neurons in the amygdala of subjects with bipolar disorder or schizophrenia: relationship to circadian rhythms. Biol. Psychiatry 81, 536–547. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pantazopoulos, H., Woo, T. U., Lim, M. P., Lange, N., Berretta, S. (2010). Extracellular matrix-glial abnormalities in the amygdala and entorhinal cortex of subjects diagnosed with schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 67, 155–166. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pare, D., Quirk, G. J., Ledoux, J. E. (2004). New vistas on amygdala networks in conditioned fear. J. Neurophysiol. 92, 1–9. doi: 10.1152/jn.00153.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penzes, P., Cahill, M. E., Jones, K. A., VanLeeuwen, J. E., Woolfrey, K. M. (2011). Dendritic spine pathology in neuropsychiatric disorders. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 285–293. doi: 10.1038/nn.2741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitkanen, A., Pikkarainen, M., Nurminen, N., Ylinen, A. (2000). Reciprocal connections between the amygdala and the hippocampal formation, perirhinal cortex, and postrhinal cortex in rat. A review. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 911, 369–391. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2000.tb06738.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitkanen, A., Stefanacci, L., Farb, C. R., Go, G. G., LeDoux, J. E., Amaral, D. G. (1995). Intrinsic connections of the rat amygdaloid complex: projections originating in the lateral nucleus. J. Comp. Neurol. 356, 288–310. doi: 10.1002/cne.903560211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pontrello, C. G., Sun, M. Y., Lin, A., Fiacco, T. A., DeFea, K. A., Ethell, I. M., et al. (2012). Cofilin under control of β-arrestin-2 in NMDA-dependent dendritic spine plasticity, long-term depression (LTD), and learning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 109, E442–451. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1118803109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rasch, B., Born, J. (2013). About sleep's role in memory. Physiol. Rev. 93, 681–766. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00032.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raskind, M. A., Peskind, E. R., Hoff, D. J., Hart, K. L., Holmes, H. A., Warren, D., et al. (2007). A parallel group placebo controlled study of prazosin for trauma nightmares and sleep disturbance in combat veterans with post-traumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 61, 928–934. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raven, F., Meerlo, P., Van der Zee, E. A., Abel, T., Havekes, R. (2019). A brief period of sleep deprivation causes spine loss in the dentate gyrus of mice. Neurobiol. Learn Mem. 160, 83–90. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2018.03.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raven, F., Van der Zee, E. A., Meerlo, P., Havekes, R. (2018). The role of sleep in regulating structural plasticity and synaptic strength: Implications for memory and cognitive function. Sleep Med. Rev. 39, 3–11. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2017.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodrigues, S. M., LeDoux, J. E., Sapolsky, R. M. (2009). The influence of stress hormones on fear circuitry. Annu Rev. Neurosci. 32, 289–313. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135620

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodriguez, A., Ehlenberger, D. B., Dickstein, D. L., Hof, P. R., Wearne, S. L. (2008). Automated three-dimensional detection and shape classification of dendritic spines from fluorescence microscopy images. PLoS ONE 3, e1997. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, R. J., Ball, W. A., Sullivan, K. A., Caroff, S. N. (1989). Sleep disturbance as the hallmark of posttraumatic stress disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 146, 697–707. doi: 10.1176/ajp.146.1.128-a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sah, P., Faber, E. S., Lopez De Armentia, M., Power, J. (2003). The amygdaloid complex: anatomy and physiology. Physiol. Rev. 83, 803–834. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00002.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seney, M. L., Cahill, K., Enwright, J. F., Logan, R. W., Huo, Z., Zong, W., et al. (2019). Diurnal rhythms in gene expression in the prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. Nat. Commun. 10, 3355. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-11335-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shelton, M. A., Newman, J. T., Gu, H., Sampson, A. R., Fish, K. N., MacDonald, M. L., et al. (2015). Loss of microtubule-associated protein 2 immunoreactivity linked to dendritic spine loss in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatr. 78, 374–385. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.12.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shi, Y., Pontrello, C. G., DeFea, K. A., Reichardt, L. F., Ethell, I. M. (2009). Focal adhesion kinase acts downstream of EphB receptors to maintain mature dendritic spines by regulating cofilin activity. J. Neurosci. 29, 8129–8142. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4681-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soehner, A. M., Kaplan, K. A., Saletin, J. M., Talbot, L. S., Hairston, I. S., Gruber, J., et al. (2017). You'll feel better in the morning: slow wave activity and overnight mood regulation in interepisode bipolar disorder. Psychol. Med. 48, 1–12. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717001581

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spano, G. M., Banningh, S. W., Marshall, W., De Vivo, S.S., Bellesi, L., Loschky, M., et al. (2019). Sleep deprivation by exposure to novel objects increases synapse density and axon-spine interface in the hippocampal CA1 region of adolescent mice. J. Neurosci. 39, 6613–6625. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0380-19.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stickgold, R. (2005). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature 437, 1272–1278. doi: 10.1038/nature04286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stickgold, R., Whidbee, D., Schirmer, B., Patel, V., Hobson, J. A. (2000). Visual discrimination task improvement: a multi-step process occurring during sleep. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 246–254. doi: 10.1162/089892900562075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuber, G. D., Sparta, D. R., Stamatakis, A. M., van Leeuwen, W. A., Hardjoprajitno, J. E., Cho, S., et al. (2011). Excitatory transmission from the amygdala to nucleus accumbens facilitates reward seeking. Nature 475, 377–380. doi: 10.1038/nature10194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sweet, R. A., Henteleff, R. A., Zhang, W., Sampson, A. R., Lewis, D. A. (2009). Reduced dendritic spine density in auditory cortex of subjects with schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 34, 374–389. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.67

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, W., Kochubey, O., Kintscher, M., Schneggenburger, R. (2020). A VTA to basal amygdala dopamine projection contributes to signal salient somatosensory events during fear learning. J. Neurosci. 40, 3969–3980. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1796-19.2020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, C. E., Taylor, F. B., McFall, M. E., Barnes, R. F., Raskind, M. A. (2008). Nonnightmare distressed awakenings in veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder: response to prazosin. J. Trauma. Stress 21, 417–420. doi: 10.1002/jts.20351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tononi, G., Cirelli, C. (2006). Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis. Sleep Med. Rev. 10, 49–62. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2005.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tononi, G., Cirelli, C. (2014). Sleep and the price of plasticity: from synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration. Neuron 81, 12–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tottenham, N., Hare, T. A., Quinn, B. T., McCarry, T. W., Nurse, M., Gilhooly, T., et al. (2010). Prolonged institutional rearing is associated with atypically large amygdala volume and difficulties in emotion regulation. Dev. Sci. 13, 46–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00852.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tovote, P., Esposito, M. S., Botta, P., Chaudun, F., Fadok, J. P., Markovic, M., et al. (2016). Midbrain circuits for defensive behaviour. Nature 534, 206–212. doi: 10.1038/nature17996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uwano, T., Nishijo, H., Ono, T., Tamura, R. (1995). Neuronal responsiveness to various sensory stimuli, and associative learning in the rat amygdala. Neuroscience 68, 339–361. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(95)00125-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varea, E., Guirado, R., Gilabert-Juan, J., Marti, U., Castillo-Gomez, E., Blasco-Ibanez, J. M., et al. (2012). Expression of PSA-NCAM and synaptic proteins in the amygdala of psychiatric disorder patients. J. Psychiatr. Res. 46, 189–197. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vecsey, C. G., Baillie, G. S., Jaganath, D., Havekes, R., Daniels, A., Wimmer, M., et al. (2009). Sleep deprivation impairs cAMP signalling in the hippocampus. Nature 461, 1122–1125. doi: 10.1038/nature08488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vyas, A., Mitra, R., Shankaranarayana Rao, B. S., Chattarji, S. (2002). Chronic stress induces contrasting patterns of dendritic remodeling in hippocampal and amygdaloid neurons. J Neurosci 22, 6810–6818. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-15-06810.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, U., Hallschmid, M., Rasch, B., Born, J. (2006). Brief sleep after learning keeps emotional memories alive for years. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 788–790. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Liu, Z., Cai, L., Guo, R., Dong, Y., Huang, Y. H., et al. (2020). A critical role of basolateral amygdala-to-nucleus accumbens projection in sleep regulation of reward seeking. Biol. Psychiatry 87, 954–966. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.10.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wellman, L. L., Fitzpatrick, M. E., Hallum, O. Y., Sutton, A. M., Williams, B. L., Sanford, L. D., et al. (2016). Individual differences in animal stress models: considering resilience, vulnerability, and the amygdala in mediating the effects of stress and conditioned fear on sleep. Sleep 39, 1293–1303. doi: 10.5665/sleep.5856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wellman, L. L., Lonart, G., Adkins, A. M., Sanford, L. D. (2022). Regulation of dark period sleep by the amygdala: a microinjection and optogenetics study. Brain Res. 1781, 147816. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2022.147816

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wellman, L. L., Yang, L., Ambrozewicz, M. A., Machida, M., Sanford, L. D. (2013). Basolateral amygdala and the regulation of fear-conditioned changes in sleep: role of corticotropin-releasing factor. Sleep 36, 471–480. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2526

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wellman, L. L., Yang, L., Tang, X., Sanford, L. D. (2008). Contextual fear extinction ameliorates sleep disturbances found following fear conditioning in rats. Sleep 31, 1035–1042.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Wilensky, A. E., Schafe, G. E., LeDoux, J. E. (1999). Functional inactivation of the amygdala before but not after auditory fear conditioning prevents memory formation. J. Neurosci. 19, RC48. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-24-j0006.1999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilensky, A. E., Schafe, G. E., LeDoux, J. E. (2000). The amygdala modulates memory consolidation of fear-motivated inhibitory avoidance learning but not classical fear conditioning. J. Neurosci. 20, 7059–7066. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-18-07059.2000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, G., Lai, C. S., Cichon, J., Ma, L., Li, W., Gan, W. B., et al. (2014). Sleep promotes branch-specific formation of dendritic spines after learning. Science 344, 1173–1178. doi: 10.1126/science.1249098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoo, S. S., Gujar, N., Hu, P., Jolesz, F. A., Walker, M. P. (2007). The human emotional brain without sleep–a prefrontal amygdala disconnect. Curr Biol. 17, R877–878. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, R. K., Lopez, M. R., Ramos-Alvarez, M. M., Normandin, M. E., Thomas, A. S., Uygun, D. S., et al. (2021). Differential effect of sleep deprivation on place cell representations, sleep architecture, and memory in young and old mice. Cell Rep. 35, 109234. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2021.109234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, Y., Lai, C. S. W., Bai, Y., Li, W., Zhao, R., Yang, G., et al. (2020). REM sleep promotes experience-dependent dendritic spine elimination in the mouse cortex. Nat. Commun. 11, 4819. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-18592-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: amygdala, sleep, memory consolidation, growth hormone, dendritic spines

Citation: Rexrode L, Tennin M, Babu J, Young C, Bollavarapu R, Lawson LA, Valeri J, Pantazopoulos H and Gisabella B (2023) Regulation of dendritic spines in the amygdala following sleep deprivation. Front. Sleep 2:1145203. doi: 10.3389/frsle.2023.1145203

Received: 15 January 2023; Accepted: 20 March 2023;
Published: 03 April 2023.

Edited by:

Ryan Bottary, Widener University, United States

Reviewed by:

Gina R. Poe, University of California, Los Angeles, United States
Allison Brager, John F. Kennedy Special Warfare Center and School, United States

Copyright © 2023 Rexrode, Tennin, Babu, Young, Bollavarapu, Lawson, Valeri, Pantazopoulos and Gisabella. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Barbara Gisabella, bgisabella@umc.edu

Download