Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 06 July 2023
Sec. Child Mental Health and Interventions
Volume 2 - 2023 | https://doi.org/10.3389/frcha.2023.1074004

Developmental screening tools for identification of children with developmental difficulties in high-income countries: a systematic review

Sara Cibralic1* Patrick Hawker2 Ferosa Khan2 Abbie Lucien2 Antonio Mendoza Diaz2 Susan Woolfenden1,3 Elisabeth Murphy4 April Deering4 Clare Schnelle4 Sharnee Townsend4 Valsamma Eapen1,2,5
  • 1Ingham Institute, Liverpool, NSW, Australia
  • 2Psychiatry and Mental Health, School of Clinical Medicine, University of New South Wales, Randwick, NSW, Australia
  • 3Sydney Local Health District, Sydney Institute Women, Children and Their Families, Camperdown, NSW, Australia
  • 4New South Wales Ministry of Health, St Leonards, NSW, Australia
  • 5Academic Unit of Infant Child and Adolescent Services (AUCS), SWSLHD, Liverpool, NSW, Australia

Objective: To examine and synthesize the literature on the use of universal developmental screening and surveillance tools in high-income countries in relation to (1) psychometric properties; (2) knowledge, acceptability, and feasibility of tools; and (3) follow-up taken following screening/surveillance.

Method: A PRISMA-compliant systematic review was performed in the PsychInfo, PubMed, and Embase databases. Studies published in the English language were included if they reported results evaluating a universal developmental screening or surveillance measurement tool. Articles on service providers’ and/or parents’ views on developmental screening were also included. Two independent reviewers extracted data and assessed for risk of bias using the Mixed Methods Appraisal Tool and the Quality Assessment of Diagnostic Accuracy Studies Tool. Results were synthesized qualitatively.

Results: Initial searches identified 2,078 articles, of which 52 were included in the final review. Findings showed that several articles assessing the accuracy of screening tools have been published, and together, they suggest that the accuracy of screening tools varies across cultures and countries. Furthermore, available literature indicated that administering universal developmental screening tools was feasible and acceptable, though only a small number of studies are available. Results also showed a limited number of studies looking at actions taken following positive screening results.

Conclusion: As the evidence stands, more research assessing the acceptability, feasibility, and accuracy of developmental screeners, is needed.

Systematic review registration: This review has been registered with the University of York Centre for Reviews and Dissemination (PROSPERO; https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?RecordID=337320, registration number CRD42022337320).

Introduction

Developmental difficulties detected during childhood, such as intellectual disorders, communication disorders, autism spectrum disorder, attention deficit/hyperactive disorder, and specific learning disorders, are prevalent (1, 2). When developmental difficulties persist, especially within environments that do not cater to developmental difficulties (3), they can be associated with a range of negative outcomes, including emotional (4, 5) and behavioural (6) problems. Early detection and intervention for developmental difficulties results in the best outcomes for children (710). It has therefore been recommended by policy bodies that universal developmental surveillance and screening is undertaken with all children under 5 years of age (11).

Developmental screening, which refers to the use of standardised instruments to aid in the identification of developmental difficulties at a specific point in time (12, 13), has been found to increase early identification of developmental difficulties, diagnosis, and access to early intervention (14, 15). Developmental screening forms part of the developmental surveillance process (12), which refers to the ongoing clinical monitoring of children at risk of developmental difficulties (12, 16). Additional components of developmental surveillance include observing children during healthcare appointments, discussing caregivers’ concerns, obtaining a child's developmental history from caregivers, and sharing any concerns with other health professionals (12). Developmental screening does not result in a diagnosis, but it can increase the sensitivity and specificity of surveillance outcomes such as an accurate diagnosis (8, 14, 15). Developmental screening was originally recommended by the American Academy of Paediatrics in 2001 (12). In 2006 The American Academy of Pediatrics developed The Brighter Futures guidelines for health supervision (17), in which the Council on Children with Disabilities (18) recommended developmental surveillance at routine well-child visits, developmental screening at specific ages (e.g., 9, 18, and 30 months) or when surveillance indicates that screening is needed (18). Subsequent research has shown that developmental surveillance together with developmental screening, compared to developmental surveillance alone, results in greater identification of delays, referrals to early intervention, and access to early intervention (14, 19). A randomized control trial in the United States of America (USA) (14), for example, found that when young children (N = 2,104, aged <30 months) were screened with the Ages and Stages Questionnaire-II and Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers with office staff assistance or without office staff assistance were 23.0% and 26.8%, respectively, more likely to be identified with delays compared 13.0% of children who received developmental surveillance alone.

Since the introduction of developmental screening into the developmental surveillance process, advances in our understanding of child development have resulted in the creation of numerous developmental screening and surveillance tools, and several systematic reviews and reports evaluating these tools have been undertaken (2025). The majority of available reviews have however focused on the use of developmental surveillance and screening tools in low-to-middle-income countries (e.g., 21). Therefore, this review aimed to synthesise and evaluate the literature on the use of developmental screening and surveillance tools in high-income countries (see Supplementary Tables S2, S3 for a definition of high-income countries and a list of high-income countries as of 2021).

The review's objectives were:

1. Identify literature on developmental screening and surveillance tools used with children aged 0–5 years in high-income countries.

2. Report on the psychometric properties of identified screening tools.

3. Determine the knowledge, acceptability (parental and service provider) and feasibility of identified developmental screening and surveillance tools.

4. Identify whether referrals were implemented, and follow-up undertaken for children following screening.

Methods

Prior to the commencement of this review, a study protocol was developed and registered with the University of York Centre for Reviews and Dissemination (PROSPERO; registration number: CRD42022337320).

Search strategy

A systematic search of published literature available up to May 2022 was conducted (no limits were placed on the earliest possible starting date). Four search strategies were implemented to identify relevant research studies. First, interdisciplinary research databases PsychInfo, Embase, and PubMed were searched concurrently for entries containing any of the following search terms: “child” OR “infant*” OR “baby” OR “preschool” AND “milestone*” AND “surveillance” OR “screening tool*” OR “screening measure*” OR “screening assessment*”. All searches were limited to entries conducted on “human” subjects published in an “English Language” journal. Second, the reference lists of articles selected for this review were searched manually. Third, internet searches for grey literature were conducted using the above-mentioned search terms alongside focused searches on key websites, including screening tool developer websites. Fourth, the “cited by” option available on some databases was used to manually search articles that had referenced the articles selected for this review. As per Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) guidelines (26), Supplementary Table S1 provides an example of the search strategy approach.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Articles were included for full text review if: (1) they evaluated a universal developmental screening or surveillance tool which included greater than two developmental domains (i.e., studies on screeners focused only on one domain such as gross motor were excluded); (2) the study sample included participants aged 0–5 years (if samples comprised a wide age group they were included if the average child age was below 6 years); (3) the study was undertaken in a high-income country; and (4) the article was published in English. Articles that looked at practitioner and parent acceptability of screening/surveillance tools were also included if they were published in English. Articles were excluded if: (1) they included a screening/surveillance tool to evaluate an intervention outcome only (i.e., there was no evaluation of the screening tool itself); (2) they used a screening/surveillance tool to evaluate development in a specific population only (e.g., children with congenital heart disease) or focused on a specific condition (e.g., autism spectrum disorder only); (3) they were not available in English; (4) they were not data-based (e.g., books, theoretical papers, reviews); or (5) they were unpublished dissertations/theses. Articles focused on First Nations populations were also excluded as a separate review on developmental screening/surveillance tools used with First Nations populations was undertaken (27). Articles that focused on a specific population but had a comparison group drawn from a normative population and segregated data were included, however, only data on the comparison group was interpreted. Furthermore, studies that included results from multiple countries were only included if data was segregated based on country and results from high-income countries were interpretable.

Screening

Four reviewers independently performed title/abstract screening and full-text screening (two reviewers focused on articles published prior to 2014 (team 1) and two reviewers (team 2) focused on articles published after 2014). In cases where there were disagreements between the reviewers, they resolved these through further discussion. A third reviewer was available to settle conflicts if necessary. The inter-rater reliability for title/abstract screening was 88% and 83% for team 1 and team 2, respectively. The inter-rater reliability for full text screaming was 97% for team 1% and 89% for team 2.

Quality assessment and data analysis

Two reviewers independently assessed the quality of studies using the Mixed Methods Appraisal Tool [MMAT; (28)] or the Quality Assessment of Diagnostic Accuracy Studies Tool [QUADAS-2; (29)]. The MMAT allows for the assessment of the methodological quality of qualitative research, randomized controlled trials, non-randomized studies, quantitative descriptive studies, and mixed methods studies. Using MMAT, the risk of bias is determined based on five sources for each study category. For example, for randomized control trials, study quality is based on (1) randomization; (2) group comparability; (3) complete outcome data; (4) blinding of assessors; and (5) participant intervention adherence. Reviewers first ascertained the study design by evaluating the methodology using the MMAT. They then assigned a “yes”, “no”, or “can't tell” to each outcome. The “can't tell” option was used when not enough information is available for the reviewer to assign a “yes” or “no”. Disagreements were discussed between reviewers, and a third reviewer was available to settle discrepancies, ensuring a consistent and reliable assessment of study quality. The MMAT is however not suitable for use with diagnostic accuracy studies. Therefore, the QUADAS-2 (29) was used to evaluate diagnostic accuracy studies. The risk of bias on the QUADAS-2 is determined based on (1) patient selection; (2) index text; (3) reference standard; and (4) flow and timing. The QUADAS-2 also allows you to assess applicability based on concerns that the study does not match the review questions. For each of the four domains in QUADAS-2 we followed a structured approach that involved assessing signalling questions, making judgments about the risk of bias, and evaluating applicability concerns. Risk of bias was judged as “low,” “high,” or “unclear.” If the answers to all signalling questions for a domain were “yes,” then the risk of bias for that domain was judged as “low”. If any signalling question was answered “no” or “unclear”, potential for bias was denoted as “high”, or “unclear”, respectively. The same criteria were applied to assess applicability concerns, ensuring a comprehensive evaluation of each study's relevance to our review questions. Any discrepancies in the overall risk of bias or applicability judgments (for each of the four domains) were discussed between the reviewers and resolved through agreement. A third reviewer was available to settle disputes if necessary. As both the MMAT and QUADAS-2 discourage the calculation of an overall quality score an overall quality score was not calculated (see Tables 1, 2 for quality assessments of the included studies).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Quality assessment using QUADAS-2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Quality assessment using MMAT.

Results

Figure 1 presents an overview of our search strategy. Initial database searches resulted in a total of 2004 articles. Title and abstract screening resulted in the exclusion of 1,877 articles, resulting in 127 articles. Seventy-four additional articles were identified through reference lists, cited by, and grey literature searches, resulting in a total of 201 articles that underwent full-text screening. Full-text screening resulted in the exclusion of an additional 149 articles. Fifty-two articles met the inclusion criteria and were included in the current review.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram of included studies.

Overview of included studies

Fifty-two studies evaluating universal developmental surveillance or screening tools were identified, Table 3 presents an overview of studies included in this review. Of those, 22 were undertaken in the USA, six in Canada, four in Japan, six in China, four in Australia, two in Norway, two in Singapore, and one each in Chile, Korea, The Netherlands, Portugal, Spain, and Wales. Thirty-two were cross-sectional, nine were longitudinal, six were mixed methods, three were qualitative, one was a randomized control trial, and one was a prospective population-based observational study. Most tools were universal screening tools, apart from Learn the Signs. Act Early which is a surveillance tool.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Overview of included studies table.

Twenty four studies evaluated the Ages and Stages Questionnaire [ASQ; (32, 36, 41, 42, 4451, 53, 54, 57, 6062, 65, 66, 6871)] and one study evaluated the Infant/Child Monitoring Questionnaire [IMQ; (33)] which was later renamed the ASQ; nine studies evaluated the Parents Evaluation of Developmental Status [PEDS; (30, 44, 48, 54, 58, 63, 69, 70, 80)]; five studies evaluated the Learn the Signs. Act Early [LTSAE; (55, 74, 7779)]; three studies evaluated the Survey of Wellbeing of Young Children [SWYC; (48, 56, 73)]; three studies evaluated the Early Symptomatic Syndromes Eliciting Neurodevelopmental Clinical Examinations-Questionnaire [ESSENCE-Q; (37, 38, 81)]; two studies evaluated the Denver Developmental Screening Test [Denver II; (34, 35)]; two studies evaluated the Taipei City Child Development Screening tool, second version [Taipei II; (75, 76)]; and the Bayley Infant Neurodevelopmental Screener—II [BINS; (39)], Child Development Inventory [CDI; (46)], Caregiver Reported Early Development Instruments [CREDI; (43)], Developmental Screening Questionnaire [DSQ; (64)], Early Years Check-In [EYCI; (31)]; Minnesota Child Development Inventory [MCDI; (40)], The Minnesota Preschool Inventory Individual Progress-Shortened Form [MPI; (72)], McCarthy Scale of Children's Abilities—Short From [MSCA; (59)], The Schedule of Growing Skills II [SGS-II; (52)], and The Taiwan Birth Cohort Study [TBCS; (67)] were all evaluated in one study only.

Forty-three studies assessed the psychometric properties of a developmental screening/surveillance tool (3053, 57, 5973, 75, 78, 80, 81), nine evaluated the knowledge, acceptability, and/or feasibility of a developmental screening/surveillance tool (5456, 58, 74, 7679), and nine reported on actions taken following screening (38, 40, 44, 56, 58, 60, 69, 74, 81).

Psychometric properties of identified developmental screening tool

Prior to providing an overview of the psychometric properties of identified tools, it is important to note that of the identified studies which looked at the sensitivity and specificity of screeners, most studies operationalised sensitivity and specificity as the correlation between the screener score and the score on a validated measure of developmental status such as Bayley Scale of Infant and Toddler Development [e.g., (32, 36)]. Other identified studies operationalised sensitivity and specificity as the correlation between screener scores, and diagnosis received [e.g., (37, 60)].

ASQ

The ASQ assesses Communication, Gross Motor, Fine Motor, Problem Solving, and Personal-Social skills in children aged 0–5 ½ years (82). Of forty-three studies looking at the psychometric properties of the tool, twenty-three examined the accuracy of the ASQ (32, 36, 41, 42, 4451, 53, 57, 6062, 65, 66, 6871), and one study evaluated the Infant/Child Monitoring Questionnaire, which was surpassed by the ASQ (33). Seven studies were undertaken in the USA, five in Canada, two in China, two in Norway, one in Australia, one in Chile, one in Japan, one in Korea, one in The Netherlands, one in Portugal, and one in Spain. Ten studies recruited participants from health care settings (32, 36, 4448, 51, 66, 70), nine from a community and/or health setting (33, 41, 42, 49, 50, 53, 57, 60, 61), two from preschools (69, 71), one from a child development support program (68), one used data from a national longitudinal survey of children and youth (65), and one used population data (62) (total N = 37,144). Together the results showed that the sensitivity of the tool ranged from 22% (32) to 100% (36, 60) while specificity ranged from 39% (46) to 92% (50). Most studies, however reported that the sensitivity and/or specificity of the ASQ fell within the moderate range (the sensitivity and specificity ranges referenced were: High = >90%, Moderate = 71%–90%, Low = ≤70%) (32, 36, 44, 49, 50, 60). Furthermore, most studies assessing accuracy indicated a low risk of bias on the QUADAS-2 (Table 1).

PEDS

The PEDS is a developmental screener consisting of ten questions that assesses for global/cognitive, expressive language and articulation, receptive language, fine and gross motor, behaviour, self-help, socialisation, and academic concerns in children aged 0–8 years (83). Seven studies evaluated the psychometric properties of the PEDS (30, 44, 48, 63, 69, 70, 80). Four studies (total N = 7,155) were undertaken in the USA, one in Canada, one in Spain, and one in Singapore. Four studies recruited participants from primary care centres (44, 48, 70, 80), one from community and health care settings (30), one from childcare and kindergarten centres (63), and one from pre-primary school (69). Overall results indicated the measure to have moderate sensitivity (74%–83%) (30, 44) and low to moderate specificity (64%–84%) (30, 44). One study also found the measure to have high internal consistency and test-retest reliability, and moderate to high interrater reliability (when completed by parents and professionals) (30).

ESSENCE-Q

The ESSENCE-Q is a developmental screener comprised of twelve items that assess development in children under the age of six (37, 38, 81). All three studies evaluating the ESSENCE-Q were undertaken in Japan by the same team (37, 38, 81). Participants (total N = 381) were recruited from primary (i.e., public health clinic) and secondary (i.e., developmental clinic) health care settings. Together results showed the screener to have moderate to high sensitivity (ranging between 78% and 94%) when completed by mothers, nurses and psychologists, low specificity when completed by mothers (59%), and low to moderate specificity when completed by psychologists and nurses (ranging between 53% and 77%).

SWYC

The SWYC consists of short answer questions relating to child development, child behaviour, and family risk factors (e.g., depression, substance abuse) completed by caregivers of children aged 1–65 months (84). Of the three identified SWYC studies, two evaluated the psychometric properties of the SWYC (48, 73). Both studies evaluating the psychometric properties of the SWYC were undertaken in the USA by the same team, and participants were recruited from primary care settings (total N = 14,571). Results from Sheldrick, Schlichting (73) indicated that a higher percentage of children were reported to pass milestones by the age at which the Centres for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) guidelines indicated that “most children pass” certain milestones and a greater percentage of children were reported to pass milestones by the age at which the CDC stated that parents should “act early”. The second study found that the screener had moderate specificity (70.8%–89%) and moderate sensitivity but only for severe delays (73.3%) (48).

Denver II

The Denver II is a clinician-administered developmental screening test that is comprised of 125 items which assess general areas of development in children aged 0–6 years (85). Two studies assessed the accuracy of the Denver II, both studies were undertaken in the USA by the same team and recruited participants (total N = 193) from childcare centres (34, 35). Together results indicated the measure to have moderate sensitivity (>80%) and low specificity (<50%).

BINS

The BINS is an 11–13 item (depending on age at administration) developmental screener for children aged 3–24 months (86). The overall score is classified as low, moderate or high and reflects a child's risk of developmental delay or neurological impairment (86). The accuracy of the BINS was evaluated in one study undertaken in the USA and recruited participants (N = 106) from hospitals (39). Results showed the measure to have low sensitivity (ranging from 0% to 40%) but high specificity (ranging from 66% to 100%).

CDI

The CDI is a revised version of the MCDI. It is a parent-completed screener that measures eight areas of development (social, self-help, gross motor, fine motor, expressive and receptive language, letters and numbers) in children aged 15 months to 6 years (87). One study was found that evaluated the CDI (46), and one evaluated the MCDI (40). An additional study was found that assessed the MPI—a shortened version of the MCDI used with preschool-aged children (72). Given that the CDI surpasses the MCDI, only the CDI results were interpreted. The CDI study was undertaken in Canada and recruited participants from primary care settings (N = 246). Results indicated that the measure had low sensitivity (50%), low positive predictive validity (50%), moderate specificity (86%), and moderate negative predictive validity (86%).

CREDI

The CREDI is a newly developed parent-completed child development screener for children aged 0–35 months (88). One study evaluated the psychometric properties of the CREDI (43). The study was undertaken in China and recruited participants from urban and rural communities (N = 946). Results indicated that the measure had good internal consistency reliability (Cronbach's alpha = .92–.97) reliability and high concurrent validity with the Bayley Scale of Infant and Toddler Development-III.

DSQ

The DSQ is a parent-completed, computer-based child-screening questionnaire designed to evaluate developmental risk in children aged 1–6 years (64). One study evaluated the accuracy of the DSQ (64). The study was undertaken in Singapore and recruited a community sample (N = 506). Results showed the measure to have high specificity (97%) and no data was provided on sensitivity.

EYCI

The EYCI is a newly developed, parent-completed screener designed to assess developmental concerns in children aged 18 months to 6 years. The measure is comprised of 11 items which examine 10 domains and is available both in paper format and electronically (31). One study assessed the accuracy of the EYCI. The study was undertaken in Canada, recruited participants (N = 246) from childcare settings, and had both parents and educators complete the measure. Results showed the measure to have moderate sensitivity (86%) and specificity (82%) when completed by parents. Agreement between parents and educators was low (Rho > 0.30).

SGS-II

The SGS-II is a developmental screening tool targeted at assessing developmental trajectories in children aged 0–5 years. The tool is an adapted version of the National Childhood Encephalopathy Study tool (89) and is comprised of four subscales (locomotor, language, personal-social, and fine motor) which assesses ten different skill areas (52). One study assessed the accuracy of the SGS-II (52). The study was undertaken in North Wales and families (N = 39) were recruited from nurseries and nursery schools. Results indicated that the measure had high sensitivity (100%; except for the locomotor subscale which had low sensitivity) and specificity (100%) for children aged 0–24 months and moderate to high sensitivity (50%–100%) and moderate to high specificity (85%–91%; except for the locomotor subscale which had low specificity) for children aged 25–52 months.

TBCS

The TBCS is a developmental screener designed to assess gross motor, fine motor, language, and social skills in children at 6, 18, and 36 months (67). A pilot study was undertaken in China to assess the screener (67). The study recruited a community sample of participants (N = 1,783). Results indicated that the measure had good predictive validity with the 6-month assessment results predicting the 18-month results, and the 18-month results predicting the 36-month results.

Taipei II

The Taipei II provides a checklist for thirteen age groups from 4 months to 6 years, which assesses fine motor, gross motor, language/communication, and emotion/social areas (75). One study evaluated the accuracy of the Taipei II (75). The study, which was undertaken in Taiwan (China) and recruited participants (N = 390) from a public health centre, evaluated the psychometric properties of the digital version of the measure compared to the original text version. Results indicated excellent agreement between the two versions of the measure as well as moderate to high reliability.

Knowledge, acceptability, and feasibility of universal developmental screening tool

Nine studies were identified that explored knowledge, acceptability, and/or feasibility of administering universal developmental surveillance and screening tools (5456, 58, 74, 7679). Five studies evaluated the LTSAE (55, 74, 7779), two evaluated the PEDS (54, 58), one the ASQ (54), one the Taipei II (76), and one the SWYC (56). Six were undertaken in the USA, two in Australia, and one in China.

Developmental surveillance tools

LTSAE

The LTSAE program is aimed at improving identification of children with developmental delays (90). The program includes a short developmental checklist targeted at children aged 2 months to 5 years as well as educational materials regarding child development for parents and service providers (90). In 2022, following a review of literature, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention updated the LTSAE (91). Eleven criteria were developed to assess milestones, including that the milestones are indicated at an age that most children (75%) would expect to demonstrate mastery of the milestone and that the milestones are easy for families from different social, cultural and ethnic groups to observe (91). All five studies examining the knowledge, acceptability, and/or feasibility of administering of the LTSAE were undertaken in the USA (55, 74, 7779). Two studies recruited participants from Head Start centres (74, 79), two from child care centres (55, 78), and one from well-child visits (77). Together, results showed that the LTSAE materials increased parental knowledge and awareness of developmental milestones. One study, however, noted that while parents found LTSAE materials appealing, they were unaware of how to act early or why acting early was important. The two studies undertaken at Head Start centres (74, 79) also found that participation in the LTSAE program improved parental engagement with developmental monitoring, improved communication between parents and staff members, and helped build rapport between parents and staff members. Abercrombie, Pann (74) also showed that most Head Start staff in their study found the materials easy to integrate into work with their families and did not experience any barriers to doing so, indicating that it is feasible to incorporate LTSAE materials into the Head Start program. Of the small percentage of Head Start staff (11%) that did identify barriers, the most common barriers included lack of time, conflicting demands, parents being unreceptive to materials, and low parental literacy.

Developmental screening tools

ASQ

Only one study was identified which examined the knowledge and acceptability of administering the ASQ. The study was undertaken by Garg, Ha (54), and looked at 37 Australian health professionals’ [three nurse managers, one Out-of-Home Care coordinator (social worker), two general practitioner practice nurses, five child and family health nurses, six general practitioners, seven paediatricians and one senior child health medical officer] knowledge and acceptability of the ASQ-3 (and the PEDS). Results indicated that most health professionals were aware of the benefits associated with developmental screening tools though several barriers were identified that might prevent them from utilising screening tools. These barriers included the availability of the screening tool in languages other than English, limited knowledge of the screening tool, and concerns regarding the specificity of the screening tool. Some Paediatricians and General Practitioners also noted that they relied on their clinical judgment rather than screening tools to identify developmental delays.

PEDS

Two Australian studies looked at the knowledge and acceptability of the PEDS (54, 58). One study included health professionals recruited from primary care settings (54) and the other included parents (N = 262) recruited from childcare centres and kindergarten (58). As mentioned above (in the ASQ section), most health professionals were aware of the benefits and barriers associated with screening tools. Parents indicated that the measure was easy to complete and felt that the information would be helpful to health professionals.

Taipei II

Cheng, Chang (76) evaluated 118 caregivers’ acceptability and utility of the multimedia version of the Taipei II. Results showed that the measure was easily accessible and that 98% of participants preferred the multimedia version of the measure to the paper version, indicating strong support for the acceptability of the tool.

SWYC

Berger-Jenkins, Monk (56) evaluated the feasibility of administering an adapted SWYC together with a behavioural screener in a busy urban medical practice (N = 349 parents). Results showed that screening rates ranged between 5% and 90%. Screening tables were introduced when barriers, such as running out of photocopies and not knowing which patients to give screeners to, were identified. After the introduction of screening tables, screening rates reached as high as 90%.

Action taken following positive screening

Nine studies provided information on action taken following developmental screening (38, 40, 44, 56, 58, 60, 69, 74, 81). Two were undertaken in Canada, two in Japan, three in the USA, one in Australia, and one in Spain.

Studies either indicated that information regarding test scores was provided to parents who were instructed to seek follow-up assessments or that participants were referred for follow-up assessments. Only two studies, however, conducted follow-ups to determine rates of referral uptake. Berger-Jenkins, Monk (56) noted that following screening using the SWYC (and a behavioural screener) approximately 80% of participants followed up with their primary medical doctor and 50% completed referrals to a clinic social worker. Abercrombie, Pann (74) found that 28% of their participants had child development concerns. Of those, 51% reported that LTSAE materials helped identify concerns. Furthermore, 52% of participants who reported speaking to a professional noted that the materials were very helpful, and 26% found the materials helpful, when speaking with their doctors. The reported outcomes of these visits included 28% of parents receiving additional information about their concerns, 28% being referred to another professional, 25% receiving current help with their concerns, 16% reported that their child received a diagnosis, and 3% noted that their doctor did not identify a delay.

Discussion

This review has described and synthesized the available evidence on the psychometric properties; knowledge, feasibility, and acceptability; and evidence of outcomes of developmental screening and surveillance implementation in high income countries. Fifty-two articles were evaluated to ascertain evidence regarding the psychometric properties of identified tools; knowledge, acceptability, and feasibility of identified tools; and actions taken following positive screens.

The first finding was that a number of studies assessing the psychometric properties, including accuracy, of developmental screening tools have been published, however, the majority of identified literature evaluated the ASQ (n = 23). Overall, for screening tools which had more than one study undertaken (e.g., ASQ, PEDS), findings regarding the accuracy of the tool were highly variable, particularly so when measures were administered in different countries or with different populations. Given these findings it is critical that care is taken to ensure that a measure has been validated for use with a specific population before it is administered with that population to avoid over- or under-recognition of developmental disorders (92). Furthermore, the operationalisation of sensitivity and specificity differed across studies with some studies comparing screener scores to outcomes on standardised developmental measures while others compared screener scores to a diagnosis of a developmental condition. Given that the aim of screeners is to identify developmental conditions; that diagnosis of a developmental conditions requires more than a positive score on a standardised developmental measure; and that most studies looked at the correlation between screener scores and developmental measure outcomes to ascertain sensitivity and specificity, more research needs to be undertaken to determine the sensitivity and specificity of measures in regard to receiving a diagnosis of a developmental condition.

Second, several studies assessing the knowledge relating to, and acceptability and feasibility of, using screening and surveillance tools were also identified, with most identified literature examining the LTSAE (n = 5). Results provided support for each measure's acceptability and/or feasibility. The literature was however limited by only a small number of studies examining each tool. More research examining the acceptability and feasibility of all identified screening tools is recommended. This would increase understanding regarding which measures are well received by target populations and more likely to be completed (93).

Third, a few studies noted that action was taken following positive developmental screens, though, only two studies (56, 74) conducted follow-up assessments to describe these actions. Without assessing outcomes of screening, it is difficult to determine the benefits and risks associated with screening. Commonly cited benefits associated with early screening often include early diagnosis and access to early intervention (94, 95). Early intervention has been found to support the development of core (e.g., communication) and related (e.g., play) areas of child development (96). In contrast, risks frequently associated with screening include receiving a false positive screen and the time, effort, and anxiety associated with further testing (97). More research is therefore needed to determine whether the benefits of screening for developmental disorders outweigh the risks.

Clinical and policy implications

This review identified a variety of available universal developmental screeners. While literature on the acceptability and feasibility of screeners indicated that the administration of most screeners was feasible and acceptable, the accuracy of some screeners varied across studies. Furthermore, with the exception of the ASQ, PEDS, and LTSAE, most screeners were evaluated in three or less studies (with the majority only being evaluated in one study). If developmental screeners are to be implemented universally (e.g., national screening programs), it is necessary to consider the research evidence available on that screener. However, it is also necessary to examine the accuracy, acceptability, and feasibility of the chose screener with the population it is intended to be used with and ensure that they are able to detect developmental delays in target populations (98), before widespread implementation occurs.

Strengths and limitations

Strengths of this review included the use of systematic review strategy with broad inclusion criteria, and that reviewing of all included studies and completion risk of bias assessments was undertaken by two independent reviewers. The review also had several limitations. First, searches were restricted to studies written in the English language, reducing the generalizability of findings. Second, the quality assessment indicated that several of the diagnostic accuracy studies did not report information on whether results of the knowledge standard tests (e.g., secondary, or diagnostic assessment) were interpreted without knowledge of the index test (e.g., screener) results suggesting an increased risk of bias. Third, the psychometric performance of screening tools can be influenced by cultural variations in behavioural expectations and developmental milestones across diverse populations. We did not thoroughly assess cultural variation, as this was beyond the scope of our study. Fourth, given that the focus of the review was on high income countries, the results cannot be generalized to low- and middle-income countries. Fifth, studies that fit the inclusion criteria but did not segregate data based on population or country were excluded. This may have impacted the study outcomes.

Conclusion

To conclude, developmental screeners have been considered useful tools for early identification difficulties. This review found that a variety of developmental screening/surveillance tools are available, however, the majority of identified studies have been undertaken on only a small number of available tools (e.g., ASQ, PEDS, LTSAE), with most screeners only being evaluated in one study. For screeners which have been evaluated in more than one study, the accuracy of the tools was found to vary across countries and populations. It is necessary to ensure that a screener is accurate, feasible, and acceptable to be administered with a certain population before it is used with that population. Without evidence supporting the accuracy of a screener for use with a certain population, the use of the screener may result in false positive screens and the associated negative consequences. Thus, if universal implementation of a developmental screener is to occur, the accuracy, acceptability, and feasibility associated with administrating that screener will need careful consideration before the screener is implemented.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

SC, AM, SW, EM, AD, CS, ST, and VE were involved in the conception and design of the study. SC and PH conducted the analyses and interpreted the data. SC wrote the first draft of the manuscript and PH, FK, AL, AM, SW, EM, AD, CS, ST, and VE contributed to the final draft. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was funded by the New South Wales Ministry of Health, funding number: Ingham Institute for Applied Medical Research MH01.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author VE declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/frcha.2023.1074004/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Francés L, Quintero J, Fernández A, Ruiz A, Caules J, Fillon G, et al. Current state of knowledge on the prevalence of neurodevelopmental disorders in childhood according to the DSM-5: a systematic review in accordance with the PRISMA criteria. Child Adoles Psych Mental Health. (2022) 16(1):27. doi: 10.1186/s13034-022-00462-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Zablotsky B, Black LI, Maenner MJ, Schieve LA, Danielson ML, Bitsko RH, et al. Prevalence and trends of developmental disabilities among children in the United States: 2009–2017. Pediatrics. (2019) 144(4):e20190811. doi: 10.1542/peds.2019-0811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Nicholls G, Hastings RP, Grindle C. Prevalence and correlates of challenging behaviour in children and young people in a special school setting. EurJ Spec Needs Educ. (2020) 35(1):40–54. doi: 10.1080/08856257.2019.1607659

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Cibralic S, Kohlhoff J, Wallace N, McMahon C, Eapen V. A systematic review of emotion regulation in children with autism spectrum disorder. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2019) 68:101422. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2019.101422

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. England-Mason G. Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic feature in children with neurodevelopmental disorders. Curr Dev Disord Rep. (2020) 7(3):130–8. doi: 10.1007/s40474-020-00200-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Donolato E, Cardillo R, Mammarella IC, Melby-Lervåg M. Research review: language and specific learning disorders in children and their co-occurrence with internalizing and externalizing problems: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2022) 63(5):507–18. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Clark MLE, Vinen Z, Barbaro J, Dissanayake C. School age outcomes of children diagnosed early and later with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2018) 48(1):92–102. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3279-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Barger B, Rice C, Wolf R, Roach A. Better together: developmental screening and monitoring best identify children who need early intervention. Disab Health J. (2018) 11(3):420–6. doi: 10.1016/j.dhjo.2018.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Zwicker JG, Lee EJ. Early intervention for children with/at risk of developmental coordination disorder: a scoping review. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2021) 63(6):659–67. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.14804

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Landa RJ. Efficacy of early interventions for infants and young children with, and at risk for, autism spectrum disorders. Int Rev Psychiatry. (2018) 30(1):25–39. doi: 10.1080/09540261.2018.1432574

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Black MM, Walker SP, Fernald LCH, Andersen CT, DiGirolamo AM, Lu C, et al. Early childhood development coming of age: science through the life course. Lancet. (2017) 389(10064):77–90. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31389-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Committee on Children With Disabilities. Developmental surveillance and screening of infants and young children. Pediatrics. (2001) 108(1):192–5. doi: 10.1542/peds.108.1.192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Meisels SJ, Provence S. Screening and assessment: Guidelines for identifying young disabled and developmentally vulnerable children and their families. Washington, DC: ERIC (1989).

14. Guevara JP, Gerdes M, Localio R, Huang YV, Pinto-Martin J, Minkovitz CS, et al. Effectiveness of developmental screening in an urban setting. Pediatrics. (2013) 131(1):30–7. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-0765

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Carroll AE, Bauer NS, Dugan TM, Anand V, Saha C, Downs SM. Use of a computerized decision aid for developmental surveillance and screening: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatr. (2014) 168(9):815–21. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Dworkin PH. Detection of behavioral, developmental, and psychosocial problems in pediatric primary care practice. Curr Opin Pediatr. (1993) 5(5):531–6. doi: 10.1097/00008480-199310000-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Hagan JF, Shaw JS, Duncan PM. Bright futures: Guidelines for health supervision of infants, children, and adolescents. 4th ed. American Academy of Pediatrics (2017).

18. Council on Children With Disabilities, Section on Developmental Behavioral Pediatrics, Bright Futures Steering Committee, Medical Home Initiatives for Children With Special Needs Project Advisory Committee. Identifying infants and young children with developmental disorders in the medical home: an algorithm for developmental surveillance and screening. Pediatrics. (2006) 118(1):405–20. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Thomas SA, Cotton W, Pan X, Ratliff-Schaub K. Comparison of systematic developmental surveillance with standardized developmental screening in primary care. Clin Pediatr. (2011) 51(2):154–9. doi: 10.1177/0009922811420711

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Acar S, Hansen S, Dolata J, Chen CY. An overview of developmental screening: implications for practice. Başkent Univ J Educ. (2017) 1(1):9–18. Available at: http://buje.baskent.edu.tr/index.php/buje/article/view/4

Google Scholar

21. Boggs D, Milner KM, Chandna J, Black M, Cavallera V, Dua T, et al. Rating early child development outcome measurement tools for routine health programme use. Arch Dis Child. (2019) 104(Suppl 1):S22–33.30885963

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

22. Baker J, Kohlhoff J, Onobrakpor S-I, Woolfenden S, Smith R, Knebel C, et al. The acceptability and effectiveness of web-based developmental surveillance programs: rapid review. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth. (2020) 8(4):e16085.32324149

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

23. Gist L, Stein MT. Developmental screening in your community: an integrated approach for connecting children with services. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2014) 35(7). doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000088

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Moodie S, Daneri P, Goldhagen S, Halle T, Green K, LaMonte L. Birth to 5: watch me thrive! A compendium of screening measures for young children (OPRE report 201411). Washington, DC: Office of Planning, Research and Evaluation, Administration for Children and Families, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (2014).

25. Coscini N, Heyes P, Bedford H, Cohen E, D’Aprano A, Goldfeld S, et al. Multicountry review: developmental surveillance, assessment and care by outpatient paediatricians. Arch Dis Child. (2023) 108(3):153–9. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2021-322799

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. (2021) 372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Cibralic S, Hawker P, Khan F, Mendoza Diaz A, Woolfenden S, Murphy E, et al. Developmental screening tools used with first nations populations: a systematic review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2022) 19(23):15627. doi: 10.3390/ijerph192315627

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Hong QN, Pluye P, Fàbregues S, Bartlett G, Boardman F, Cargo M, et al. Mixed methods appraisal tool (MMAT), version 2018 (2018) 1148552.

29. Whiting PF, Rutjes AWS, Westwood ME, Mallett S, Deeks JJ, Reitsma JB, et al. QUADAS-2: a revised tool for the quality assessment of diagnostic accuracy studies. Ann Intern Med. (2011) 155(8):529–36. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-155-8-201110180-00009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Brothers KB, Glascoe FP, Robertshaw NS. PEDS: developmental milestones–an accurate brief tool for surveillance and screening. Clin Pediatr. (2008) 47(3):271–9. doi: 10.1177/0009922807309419

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Clark H, Nair K, Veldhuizen S, Campbell W, Rivard L, Rodriguez MC, et al. Validity of the early years check-in (EYCI) in a cross-sectional sample of families. Front Pediatr. (2020) 8. doi: 10.3389/fped.2020.00157

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Deakin-Bell N, Walker K, Badawi N. The accuracy of parental concern expressed in the ages and stages questionnaire to predict developmental delay. J Paediatr Child Health. (2013) 49(2):E133–E6. doi: 10.1111/jpc.12084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Dixon G, Badawi N, French D, Kurinczuk JJ. Can parents accurately screen children at risk of developmental delay? J Paediatr Child Health. (2009) 45(5):268–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1754.2009.01492.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Glascoe FP, Byrne KE, Ashford LG, Johnson KL, Chang B, Strickland B. Accuracy of the Denver-II in developmental screening. Pediatrics. (1992) 89(6 Pt 2):1221–5. doi: 10.1542/peds.89.6.1221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Glascoe FP, Byrne KE. The accuracy of three developmental screening tests. J Early Interv. (1993) 17(4):368–79. doi: 10.1177/105381519301700403

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Gollenberg AL, Lynch CD, Jackson LW, McGuinness BM, Msall ME. Concurrent validity of the parent-completed ages and stages questionnaires, 2nd ed. with the bayley scales of infant development II in a low-risk sample. Child Care Health Dev. (2010) 36(4):485–90. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2009.01041.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Hatakenaka Y, Fernell E, Sakaguchi M, Ninomiya H, Fukunaga I, Gillberg C. ESSENCE-Q—a first clinical validation study of a new screening questionnaire for young children with suspected neurodevelopmental problems in south Japan. Neuropsychiatr DisTreat. (2016) 12(8). doi: 10.2147/NDT.S108411

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Hatakenaka Y, Ninomiya H, Billstedt E, Fernell E, Gillberg C. ESSENCE-Q—used as a screening tool for neurodevelopmental problems in public health checkups for young children in south Japan. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2017) 13(10). doi: 10.2147/NDT.S132546

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Hess CR, Papas MA, Black MM. Use of the bayley infant neurodevelopmental screener with an environmental risk group. J Pediatr Psychol. (2004) 29(5):321–30. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsh036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Kenny TJ, Hebel JR, Sexton MJ, Fox NL. Developmental screening using parent report. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (1987) 8(1):8–11. doi: 10.1097/00004703-198702000-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. King-Dowling S, Rodriguez MC, Missiuna C, Cairney J. Validity of the ages and stages questionnaire to detect risk of developmental coordination disorder in preschoolers. Child Care Health Dev. (2016) 42(2):188–94. doi: 10.1111/cch.12314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Krijnen LJG, Verhoeven M, van Baar AL. Assessing social-emotional development in infants and toddlers using parent-reports: comparing the ASQ-SE-NL to the social-emotional scale of the Bayley-III-NL. Early Hum Dev. (2021) 161:105439. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2021.105439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Li Y, Tang L, Bai Y, Zhao S, Shi Y. Reliability and validity of the caregiver reported early development instruments (CREDI) in impoverished regions of China. BMC Pediatr. (2020) 20(1):475. doi: 10.1186/s12887-020-02367-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Limbos MM, Joyce DP. Comparison of the ASQ and PEDS in screening for developmental delay in children presenting for primary care. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2011) 32(7):499–511. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31822552e9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Mezawa H, Aoki S, Nakayama SF, Nitta H, Ikeda N, Kato K, et al. Psychometric profile of the ages and stages questionnaires, Japanese translation. Pediatr Int. (2019) 61(11):1086–95. doi: 10.1111/ped.13990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Rydz D, Srour M, Oskoui M, Marget N, Shiller M, Birnbaum R, et al. Screening for developmental delay in the setting of a community pediatric clinic: a prospective assessment of parent-report questionnaires. Pediatrics. (2006) 118(4):e1178–86. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-0466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Schonhaut L, Armijo I, Schonstedt M, Alvarez J, Cordero M. Validity of the ages and stages questionnaires in term and preterm infants. Pediatrics. (2013) 131(5):e1468–74. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-3313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Sheldrick RC, Marakovitz S, Garfinkel D, Carter AS, Perrin EC. Comparative accuracy of developmental screening questionnaires. JAMA Pediatr. (2020) 174(4):366–74. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.6000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Squires JK, Potter L, Bricker DD, Lamorey S. Parent-completed developmental questionnaires: effectiveness with low and middle income parents. Early Child Res Q. (1998) 13(2):345–54. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2006(99)80043-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Squires J, Bricker D, Potter L. Revision of a parent-completed development screening tool: ages and stages questionnaires. J Pediatr Psychol. (1997) 22(3):313–28. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/22.3.313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Tveten KM, Strand LI, Riiser K, Nilsen RM, Dragesund T. The ability of the ages and stages questionnaire (ASQ) to indicate motor difficulties in infants in primary care. Physiother Theory Pract. (2022):1–7. doi: 10.1080/09593985.2022.2056553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Williams ME, Hutchings J, Bywater T, Daley D, Whitaker CJ. Schedule of growing skills II: pilot study of an alternative scoring method. Psychology. (2013) 04(03):143–52. doi: 10.4236/psych.2013.43021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Yue A, Jiang Q, Wang B, Abbey C, Medina A, Shi Y, et al. Concurrent validity of the ages and stages questionnaire and the Bayley scales of infant development III in China. PLoS One. (2019) 14(9):20. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0221675

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Garg P, Ha MT, Eastwood J, Harvey S, Woolfenden S, Murphy E, et al. Health professional perceptions regarding screening tools for developmental surveillance for children in a multicultural part of Sydney, Australia. BMC Fam Pract. (2018) 19(1):42. doi: 10.1186/s12875-018-0728-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Raspa M, Levis DM, Kish-Doto J, Wallace I, Rice C, Barger B, et al. Examining parents’ experiences and information needs regarding early identification of developmental delays: qualitative research to inform a public health campaign. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2015) 36(8):575–85. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Berger-Jenkins E, Monk C, D'Onfro K, Sultana M, Brandt L, Ankam J, et al. Screening for both child behavior and social determinants of health in pediatric primary care. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2019) 40(6):415–24. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Chen C-Y, Xie H, Clifford J, Chen C-I, Squires J. Examining internal structures of a developmental measure using multidimensional item response theory. J Early Interv. (2018) 40(4):287–303. doi: 10.1177/1053815118788063

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Coghlan D, Kiing JS, Wake M. Parents’ evaluation of developmental Status in the Australian day-care setting: developmental concerns of parents and carers. J Paediatr Child Health. (2003) 39(1):49–54. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1754.2003.00084.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Dean RS, Steffen JE. Direct and indirect pediatric screening measures. J Pediatr Psych. (1984) 9:65–75. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/9.1.65

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Elbers J, Macnab A, McLeod E, Gagnon F. The ages and stages questionnaires: feasibility of use as a screening tool for children in Canada. Can J Rural Med. (2008) 13(1):9–14.18208647

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

61. Heo KH, Squires J, Yovanoff P. Cross-cultural adaptation of a pre-school screening instrument: comparison of Korean and US populations. J Intellect Disabil Res. (2008) 52(Pt 3):195–206. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2007.01000.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Janson H, Squires J. Parent-completed developmental screening in a Norwegian population sample: a comparison with US normative data. Acta Paediatr. (2004) 93(11):1525–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2004.tb02641.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Kiing JS, Low PS, Chan YH, Neihart M. Interpreting parents’ concerns about their children’s development with the parents evaluation of developmental status: culture matters. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2012) 33(2):179–83. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31823f686e

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Kwan C, Nam S. Utilizing parental observations and computer technology in developing a child-screening instrument in Singapore. Int J Early Years Educ. (2004) 12(2):117–29. doi: 10.1080/0966976042000225516

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Lamsal R, Dutton DJ, Zwicker JD. Using the ages and stages questionnaire in the general population as a measure for identifying children not at risk of a neurodevelopmental disorder. BMC Pediatr. (2018) 18(1):122. doi: 10.1186/s12887-018-1105-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Lopes S, Graça P, Teixeira S, Serrano AM, Squires J. Psychometric properties and validation of Portuguese version of ages & stages questionnaires (3rd edition): 9, 18 and 30 questionnaires. Early Hum Dev. (2015) 91(9):527–33. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2015.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Lung FW, Chiang TL, Lin SJ, Lee MC, Shu BC. Child developmental screening instrument from six to thirty-six months in Taiwan birth cohort study. Early Hum Dev. (2010) 86(1):17–21. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2009.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Olvera Astivia OL, Forer B, Dueker GL, Cowling C, Guhn M. The ages and stages questionnaire: latent factor structure and growth of latent mean scores over time. Early Hum Dev. (2017) 115:99–109. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2017.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Sarmiento Campos JA, Squires J, Ponte J. Universal developmental screening: preliminary studies in Galicia, Spain. Early Child Dev Care. (2010) 181(4):475–85. doi: 10.1080/03004430903458007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Sices L, Stancin T, Kirchner L, Bauchner H. PEDS And ASQ developmental screening tests may not identify the same children. Pediatrics. (2009) 124(4):e640–7. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-2628

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Tsai H-LA, McClelland MM, Pratt C, Squires J. Adaptation of the 36-month ages and stages questionnaire in Taiwan: results from a preliminary study. J Early Interv. (2006) 28(3):213–25. doi: 10.1177/105381510602800308

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Williams KT, Gridley BE, Treloar JH. Using the parents’ input in preschool screening. J Read Writ Learn Disabil Int. (1989) 5(4):345–52. doi: 10.1080/0748763890050405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Sheldrick RC, Schlichting LE, Berger B, Clyne A, Ni P, Perrin EC, et al. Establishing new norms for developmental milestones. Pediatrics. (2019) 144(6):e20190374. doi: 10.1542/peds.2019-0374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Abercrombie J, Pann J, Shin F, Taylor E, Brisendine AE, Swanson-Holm R, et al. Evaluation of the feasibility and perceived value of integrating learn the signs. Act early. Developmental monitoring resources in early head start. Early Child Educ J. (2021) 50:1–13. doi: 10.1007/s10643-021-01247-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Cheng H-YK, Chen L-Y, Cheng C-H, Ju Y-Y, Chen C-L, Tseng KC. A multimedia child developmental screening checklist: design and validation. J Med Int Res. (2016) 18(10):227–36. doi: 10.2196/jmir.6249

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Cheng H-YK, Chang H-T, Huang P-H, Ju Y-Y, Chen L-Y, Tseng KC. The design and validation of a child developmental e-screening system. J Med Syst. (2017) 41(4):67. doi: 10.1007/s10916-017-0701-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Gadomski AM, Riley MR, Scribani M, Tallman N. Impact of “learn the signs. Act early.” materials on parental engagement and doctor interaction regarding child development. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2018) 39(9):693–700. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Graybill E, Self-brown S, Lai B, Vinoski E, McGill T, Crimmins D. Addressing disparities in parent education: examining the effects of learn the signs/act early parent education materials on parent outcomes. Early Child Educ J. (2016) 44(1):31–8. doi: 10.1007/s10643-014-0680-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Taylor E, Clesi C, Houser J, O’Kelley S, Koehler J, Brisendine A. Using “learn the signs. Act early” to influence the caregiver and educator relationship and increase collaboration of developmental monitoring in early head start settings. Early Child Educ J. (2022):1–12. doi: 10.1007/s10643-021-01292-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Cox JE, Huntington N, Saada A, Epee-Bounya A, Schonwald AD. Developmental screening and parents’ written comments: an added dimension to the parents’ evaluation of developmental status questionnaire. Pediatrics. (2010) 126(Suppl 3):S170–6. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-1466M

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Hatakenaka Y, Maeda M, Ninomiya H, Hachiya K, Fernell E, Gillberg C. ESSENCE-Q obtained in routine Japanese public child health check-ups may be a valuable tool in neurodevelopmental screening. Acta Paediatr. (2020) 109(4):764–73. doi: 10.1111/apa.15029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Paul H. Brookes publishing co. products & pricing (2022). Available at: https://agesandstages.com/products-pricing/.

83. Glascoe F, Robertshaw N. Parents evaluation of developmental status: Developmental milestones (PEDS: DM). Nashville, TN: Ellsworth & Vandermeer Press, LLC (2010).

84. Perrin EC, Sheldrick C, Visco Z, Mattern K. The survey of well-being of young children (SWYC) user’s manual (2022).

85. Frankenburg WK, Dodds J, Archer P, Shapiro H, Bresnick B. The Denver II: a major revision and restandardization of the Denver developmental screening test. Pediatrics. (1992) 89(1):91–7. doi: 10.1542/peds.89.1.91

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Aylward G. Bayley infant neurodevelopmental screener (1995).

87. Ireton H. Child development inventory manual (1992).

88. McCoy D, Fink G, Waldman M. CREDI data management & scoring manual (2018). Available at: https://sites.sph.harvard.edu/credi/

89. Sheridan MD. Children’s developmental progress from birth to five years: The stycar sequences. NFER (1975).

90. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Learn the signs. Act early. About the program (2021). Available at: https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/actearly/about.html.

91. Zubler JM, Wiggins LD, Macias MM, Whitaker TM, Shaw JS, Squires JK, et al. Evidence-informed milestones for developmental surveillance tools. Pediatrics. (2022) 149(3). doi: 10.1542/peds.2021-052138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. D'Aprano AL, Carapetis JR, Andrews R. Trial of a developmental screening tool in remote Australian aboriginal communities: a cautionary tale. J Paediatr Child Health. (2011) 47(1-2):12–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1754.2010.01883.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Bowen DJ, Kreuter M, Spring B, Cofta-Woerpel L, Linnan L, Weiner D, et al. How we design feasibility studies. Am J Prev Med. (2009) 36(5):452–7. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2009.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Novak I, Morgan C, Adde L, Blackman J, Boyd RN, Brunstrom-Hernandez J, et al. Early, accurate diagnosis and early intervention in cerebral palsy: advances in diagnosis and treatment. JAMA Pediatr. (2017) 171(9):897–907. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2017.1689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Lipkin PH, Macias MM, Norwood KW, Brei TJ, Davidson LF, Davis BE, et al. Promoting optimal development: identifying infants and young children with developmental disorders through developmental surveillance and screening. Pediatrics. (2020) 145(1). doi: 10.1542/peds.2019-3449

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Crank JE, Sandbank M, Dunham K, Crowley S, Bottema-Beutel K, Feldman J, et al. Understanding the effects of naturalistic developmental behavioral interventions: a project aim meta-analysis. Autism Res. (2021) 14(4):817–34. doi: 10.1002/aur.2471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Siu AL, US. Preventive Services Task Force. Screening for autism spectrum disorder in young children: US preventive services task force recommendation statement. J Am Med Assoc. (2016) 315(7):691–6. doi: 10.1001/jama.2016.0018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. List J. The voltage effect great britain penguin buisness (2022).

Keywords: developmental screening, developmental surveillance, tools, young children, review, high-income countries

Citation: Cibralic S, Hawker P, Khan F, Lucien A, Mendoza Diaz A, Woolfenden S, Murphy E, Deering A, Schnelle C, Townsend S and Eapen V (2023) Developmental screening tools for identification of children with developmental difficulties in high-income countries: a systematic review. Front. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2:1074004. doi: 10.3389/frcha.2023.1074004

Received: 19 October 2022; Accepted: 21 June 2023;
Published: 6 July 2023.

Edited by:

Elizabeth Halstead, University College London, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Michelle Kuhn, University of Washington, United States
Pierre Ellul, Assistance Publique Hopitaux De Paris, France

© 2023 Cibralic, Hawker, Khan, Lucien, Mendoza Diaz, Woolfenden, Murphy, Deering, Schnelle, Townsend and Eapen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sara Cibralic s.cibralic@unsw.edu.au

Download