Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Public Health, 10 June 2022
Sec. Public Mental Health

Can Self-Regulatory Strength Training Counter Prior Mental Exertion? A Systematic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials

  • 1Department of Sport Studies, Faculty of Education Studies, Universiti Putra Malaysia, Selangor, Malaysia
  • 2School of Journalism and Communication, Zhengzhou University, Zhengzhou, China
  • 3School of Physical Education and Sports, SooChow University, Suzhou, China

Background: Prior mental exertion consumes self-regulation and influences any subsequent physical or cognitive performance according to the strength model of self-regulation. However, the counteractive effect of self-regulatory strength training remains unclear.

Objective: This study aims to report a comprehensive systematic review investigating self-regulatory strength training programmes on physical or cognitive performance.

Methods: To select relevant studies from the available literature, a thorough search was conducted on PubMed, Web of Science, EBSCOhost (CENTRAL, Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Collection, SPORTDicus), Scopus, and Google Scholar, as well as the sources of reference for gray literature. Only randomized controlled trials involving healthy humans, strength-based self-regulation training programmes with comparable protocols, and a physical or cognitive task associated with the study were selected for the current review. The Grading of Recommendations Assessment Development and Evaluation (GRADE) framework was used to develop the summary of findings.

Results: Twelve articles were included based on the selection criteria. Evidence certainty for outcomes was graded as either low or very low level. The majority of the studies reported that self-regulatory strength training programmes can significantly counter prior mental exertion and decrement of performance, while only one study did not find such improvement. According to the strength model, a period of training increased the ‘self-regulatory muscle.'

Conclusion: Strength is an important ingredient in the resource model of self-regulation and can be trained to counter prior mental exertion and improve subsequent physical and cognitive performance. The training effects are cross-domain (e.g., emotional and cognitive domains; higher and lower levels of executive functions). However, motivation plays a key role to mobilize this resource. Future studies should examine the mechanism that underlies the strength.

Systematic Review Registration: https://inplasy.com/inplasy-2022-1-0060/, identifier: INPLASY202210060.

Introduction

Self-regulation is the ability to adjust one's mental and physiological state adaptively to a given context, and it includes emotional, cognitive, behavioral, and physiological adaptation (1, 2). Humans must regularly exercise mental exertion and seize control over themselves to achieve the best performance. For example, to attain exemplary scores in school, a student must concentrate in class and exercise mental exertion to combat any internal (task-induced boredom) or external detractors of accomplishing goal attainment. Similarly, a cyclist must perform mental exertion to resist the urge to slow down despite suffering from body ache, or a soccer player must extract and interpret useful information while blocking out distractions from the complex competitive environment they are in for a prolonged period. Over the last two decades, there has been a growing body of evidence indicating that mental exertion has a long-term effect on one's physical and cognitive performance (36). Specifically, Englert and Wolff (5) demonstrated that depleted participants with low self-regulatory strength invested less effort to do the cycling test compared with those having increased self-regulatory strength, which is measured as a lower heart rate. Besides the physical performance, Furley et al. (6) found that depleted participants could not focus their attention on the task to make good decisions and block out additional irrelevant stimuli from the audition.

In recent years, several efforts have been made to synthesize the literature both narratively (7, 8) and quantitatively (9, 10), whereby performing mental exertion has led to a subsequent decrease in physical and cognitive performance across a wide range of tasks. However, these existing reviews mainly focus on the carryover effects, without exploring potential counteractive strategies. Finding effective interventions should be the next goal of studies in this field, which means that they should not be limited to simply demonstrating these negative effects. Therefore, a comprehensive synthesis and analysis of intervention methods are necessary.

The strength model of self-regulation (11, 12) has been utilized to describe performance decrements caused by previous mental exertion in the last two decades. Strength, also called energy, is required and can be depleted temporarily when individuals regulate the self (13, 14). The ability to conduct mental exertion is based on strength or this depletable resource as per the paradigm (9, 15). This depletable state is known as “ego depletion,” and it is thought to impair physical and cognitive performance. The “global” nature of such resources refers to the fact that all of the self-regulatory activities consume the same resource pool; for example, regulating an emotional or physiological response will affect performance in completely unrelated self-regulation demanding handgrip tasks (12). The model was well tested in a meta-analysis conducted by Hagger et al. (16) in 83 experiments with 10, 500 participants. Notably, the model indicates that the strength of self-regulation is similar to a “muscle” and can be exercised (17, 18), providing a way to minimize the negative effect of prior mental exertion and improve the subsequent performance, including physical or cognitive aspects. Muraven et al. (19) provided the first evidence that 2 weeks of training in self-regulatory strength (e.g., posture and mood regulation) can significantly reduce the susceptibility of fatigue induced by prior mental exertion and improve handgrip.

Fatigue is a sign of a decrease in available energy usage for future self-regulation, resulting in an inability to maintain current effort (20, 21). Thus, it is not surprising that the condition of mental fatigue is also induced by prior mental exertion, which has been reported several years ago [e.g., (22, 23)]. Many studies have shown that mental fatigue negatively influences a variety of physical and cognitive performance, such as cycling performance (24, 25), goal-directed attention (26), and inhibition (27). Furthermore, most current literature on mental fatigue focuses on sports performance. The negative effect has been corroborated in intermittent endurance (2830), technical performance (3133), and decision-making skills (34, 35).

On the other hand, individuals with a better capacity for self-regulation can be less vulnerable to mental fatigue and perform better in subsequent endurance tests (36). Additionally, Martin and colleagues (37) found professional cyclists showed greater resistance to mental fatigue because they must do routine training and follow a certain programme, and even restrict their diet, which could significantly strengthen their self-regulatory capacity. However, several studies indicated that the intervention to counter mental fatigue and improve the subsequent performance is still misty (35, 3841). Also, because of the similarities in potential mechanism (decreased activation in areas that include the anterior cingulate cortex and prefrontal cortex) and methodology (e.g., dual-task paradigm) of investigations in two academic areas (ego depletion and mental fatigue), mounting studies have been merging them theoretically (42) and practically (10, 43). Thus, this review summarizes interventions in two study areas together, which provides deeper insights and suggests available interventions for prior mental exertion.

As a result, the review develops a comprehensive evaluation of the intervention's enhanced self-regulation strength, providing evidence for future research to explore particular techniques to counteract earlier mental exertion. Particularly, only the studies that investigated the outcome of physical (the measurement of the capacity to carry out any tasks related to the action) and/or cognitive performance (the measurement of cognitive abilities such as inhibition, decision-making, problem-solving, etc.) are selected.

Methodology

This review's reporting adheres to the preferred reporting items checklist used in the systematic (PRISMA) protocol (44). A systematic literature search was carried out using four main databases, namely, PubMed, Web of Science, EBSCOhost (CENTRAL, Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Collection, and SPORTDicus), and Scopus, for published works from 1999 onwards, which is the first publication year of self-regulatory strength training study (19), to January 2022 (Supplementary Table S1). EBSCOhost comprises numerous sub-databases; however, only Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL), Psychology and Behavioral Science Collection, and SPORTDicus were selected, due to the relevance of their content. In addition, citations and reference lists were searched to identify any additional studies. The details of the search results are presented in Figure 1. Data searching was assisted by experienced librarians, who ensured the reliability of the searching method.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA summary of the selection procedure.

Eligibility Criteria

The PICOS method was utilized to identify the literature (Table 1), implying that studies were eligible for the review if they conformed to the following criteria: (a) evaluated self-regulation training in healthy humans; (b) included physical or cognitive performance outcomes after participating in a mental exertion task; (c) reported a randomized controlled trial; (d) peer-reviewed literature published in English; and (e) included records published during the period from 1999 to 2022.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. PICOS (participation, intervention, comparison, outcome, and study design).

Strength is defined as the energy that is required and can be depleted temporarily when individuals regulate the self (13). The training programmes that were developed and aimed to increase the energy of regulating self were included. Moreover, only the studies that investigated the outcome of physical (the measurement of the capacity to carry out any tasks related to the action) and/or cognitive performance (the measurement of cognitive abilities such as inhibition, decision-making, problem-solving, etc.) were selected.

When searching the databases, the following keywords, truncation, and Boolean operators were employed separately and in combination (Supplementary Table S1). The search was centered on the sources of reference in the identified reviews for additional literature, which would not have surfaced in the search results if only the primary databases were utilized.

Two reviewers separately examined the abstracts and titles of studies from other sources, and the results of publications acquired using the search strategy to identify research that may fit the inclusion criteria mentioned earlier. After screening, 197 papers were selected for a full-text review. Two reviewers independently reviewed these papers for inclusion. A third reviewer was consulted to resolve any discrepancies.

Protocol and Registration

The protocol of methods and planned analyses applied in this systematic review was registered in INPLASY (ref. INPLASY202210060). There are protocols in INPLASY that examine the counteractive effects intervention has on prior mental exertion (e.g., mental fatigue), such as supplements, but none of the studies focus on self-regulatory strength training toward physical and/or cognitive performance concurrently. As such, the novelty of the proposed protocol was assured.

Risk of Bias and the Certainty of Evidence Assessment

The Revised Cochrane Risk of Bias tool for randomized trials (RoB 2.0) was used to assess the risk of bias in the individual studies. Each of these five categories earned a rating of “low risk of bias,” “high risk of bias,” or “some concerns of bias” according to the signaling questions specified in the RoB 2.0 tool. Lastly, the overall risk of biased judgement was formed for each study. The guidelines provided by the Cochrane community were followed by reviewers (S.H. and X.T.).

Given the heterogeneity across measurement and training programmes, the pooling of data for a meta-analysis was not done. Therefore, only narrative synthesis of findings with outcomes was developed and presented in the summary of findings table (Table 2). Grading of Recommendations, Assessment, Development, and Evaluations (GRADE) with the “GRADEpro” online tool was recruited to facilitate the synthesis and check the certainty (quality) of evidence regarding limitations of studies (e.g., risk of bias, inconsistency of training programme effects, indirectness, imprecision, or other factors) (5557). The certainty of evidence assessment was done by two reviewers (S.H. and X.T.). The results for the certainty of evidence assessment and risk of bias were verified by the review team, which has systematic review methodology experts (S. K. G. and Z. Z). Any disagreements were resolved by further discussion in the team.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Overview of the included publications details.

Results

Study Selection

The study search yielded 1,906 unique publications. After screening, 12 studies met all of the eligibility criteria. A forward search (assessing the citations of the included publications) and backward search (assessing the reference lists of the included publications), and searching in Google Scholar provided no additional studies. Two independent reviewers showed agreement about the result. Figure 1 illustrates the study selection procedure.

Risk of Bias

The risk bias of assessment for the 12 included studies with the RoB 2-tool showed that six studies had a high level of risk of bias, while the other studies scored low or unclear risk (Figures 2, 3). According to the signaling questions of RoB 2, the main reason for a high risk of bias due to deviations from intended interventions in the five studies (1, 5052, 54) was no blindness information for either experimenter or participants. Moreover, they did not require participants to have a diary and ensure adherence.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Risk of bias for all included studies.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Risk of bias summary for each included study.

Additionally, one study (49) had a high-risk bias arising from the randomization, because the study did not report allocation concealment and had the baseline imbalance issue. Other 11 studies were rated as “unclear risk of bias” in this item, for there was no information about allocation concealment. Finally, only Filipas et al. (54) and Oaten and Cheng (47) showed outcome assessors were not aware of the training programme received. Others had some concerns about bias in the measurement of the outcome.

Population Characteristics

Table 2 contains all information regarding relevant study characteristics. The total number of participants was 951. The male: female participant ratio was 56:125 (285 males and 666 females). The average age of the participants ranged from 18 (45) to 33.8 years (54). University students constituted the main population of the selected studies.

Prior Mental Exertion Task

To examine the counteractive effect on mental fatigue, experimentally inducing mental exertion is necessary (58). However, different types and durations of mental exertion tasks were detected in the current study.

Specifically, two types of tasks were recruited by the previous studies to perform the mental exertion: emotional [e.g., anger-induced (48)] and cognitive tasks (other 11 studies presented in Table 2). Regarding the duration, the majority of the studies utilized 5 min to perform the mental exertion (19, 4547, 5052), while the longest duration was 90 min (54).

Training Programme Characteristics

The majority of studies trained self-regulatory strength from cognitive domains. Mainly, they are posture regulation (19), studying programme (45), verbal mannerism modification (1), non-dominant hand use (1, 53), financial monitoring (47), Stroop and stop-signal task (49, 53), regular logical reasoning (50), and food-specific inhibition (52). Among all these training programmes related to cognitive domains, the longest training length is 4 months for the financial monitoring (47) and the shortest length is 1 week for regular logical reasoning (50). The majority of the included studies did not report the frequency of training, probably participants were expected to maintain the training at all times in their daily life. In contrast, studies that recruited some cognitive tasks showed this duration and frequency of training. They are Stroop task (3 times/day and 10 min/time) [(49), study 1], complex counting task (3 times/day and 10 min/time) [(49), study 2], and stop-single task (1 time/day) (52).

Moreover, some studies recruited physical training programmes as the intervention, such as cardiovascular exercise (46), isometric handgrip exercise (51), and the incremental maximal ramp with a cycle ergometer (54). Specifically, Bray et al. (51) used a 4-week and 3–4 times/week programme for cardiovascular exercise; Oaten and Cheng (46) recruited isometric handgrip exercise with 2 weeks and 2 times/day; and finally, Filipas et al. (54) used a 4-week incremental maximal ramp on cycle ergometers with 3 times/week and 60 min/time.

Performance Outcomes

Outcomes including physical and cognitive performance are presented in Table 3. To operationally determine the effects that manipulation of self-regulatory strength has in improving physical and/or cognitive performance, the significant value in the experimental group compared with the control group was recruited.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Summary of findings table for self-regulatory strength training programmes.

Physical Performance Outcome

Four studies, including five investigations, showed the results of physical performance related to endurance performance in handgrip tasks (19, 49, 53) and ergometer cycling (54).

Specifically, according to the strength model of self-regulation of muscle analogy, Muraven et al. (19) first proposed that longitudinally repeated exercises of self-regulation could strengthen the resource (muscle). Muraven et al. (19) measured handgrip after 2-week posture and mood regulation, which required participants to maintain good posture (e.g., sit up straight and walk erectly) and mood all the time. The improvement index (19) (Table 3) showed a smaller decrease in persistent time of handgrip test after mental exertion in the posture-regulation group (+7.0) when compared to the mood-regulation group (−8.6). To find a more effective and accurate way to maintain compliance, Cranwell et al. (49) utilized a smartphone application to implement the training programme. Four weeks of Stroop (color identification) and counting (standing with one leg while counting backward from 1,000 in multiples of seven) task significantly improved persistence in handgrip compared to the control groups (study 1 Stroop training programme: F(1, 25)= 6.11, p < 0.02, ηp2 = 0.196; study 2 counting training programme: F(1, 30) = 15.09, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.335). Thus, the muscle analogy was corroborated by the strength model of self-regulation. Based on these results, Filipas et al. (54) found that a 4-week cycle ergometer with incremental maximal ramp training could increase tolerance for mental exertion and improve total distance in endurance tests compared to the non-intervention group (F(1, 19) = 5.66; p = 0.03).

To investigate more comprehensively from lab-based (handgrip) to real-life outcome (well-being), Miles et al. (53) recruited a 6-week training programme, including behavioral (non-dominant hand using for all daily activities) and cognitive (Stroop and stop-signal tasks) domains. More closely related to the current review concerning the outcome, the result inconsistently showed no significant difference in the persistent duration of the handgrip test between the training and control groups [F(1, 171)= 3.37, p = 0.07, ηp2 = 0.02). Notably, Miles and colleagues detected a significant difference in well-being between the experimental and control groups; however, the significant value disappeared when controlling the covariates (trait self-control and conscientiousness) (F(3, 167)= 1.83, p = 0.14). Such effect is not elaborated on in the current review, because the result of well-being is out of the scope. However, future research should not disregard the covariates of physical and cognitive performance to better understand the effects of the a self-regulatory training programme.

Cognitive Performance Outcome

Eight studies examined the cognitive performance after training programmes. Specifically, to test whether self-regulation is operating like a “muscle,” Oaten and Cheng provided more “ecologically valid” contexts, including regular academic study, physical exercise, and financial monitoring of everyday self-regulatory behaviors. The results showed the three training programmes significantly decreased error: F(1, 44) = 2,395.40, p < 0.001 (45), F(1, 23)= 966.34, p < 0.001 (46), and F(1, 47) = 1,690.20, p < 0.001 (47) in visual tracking task, respectively. However, whether the effect of these training programmes could last for a certain period is not clear.

Bertrams and Schmeichel (50) first tested cognitive performance in anagram task after a week and recruited a training programme associated with the more complex cognitive process of logical reasoning. The results showed the number of anagrams solved in the experimental group increased considerably after the intervention compared to the control group [F(1, 47) = 5.05, p = 0.03, ηp2 = 0.11]. However, the performance did not improve at the follow-up test (after 1 week), which indicated the effect of the training programme might be temporary. The authors further argued that a variety of executive functions (e.g., logical reasoning) and self-regulation share one common resource, based on previous studies [e.g., (59, 60)]. A similar outcome in anagram was also obtained by Gailliot et al. (1) (study 1 and study 2) after 2 weeks of self-regulatory training programmes (verbal mannerism modifying and non-dominant hand using). However, motivation plays a key role. Specifically, the significant results were only detected in the low-motivation group, rather than the high-motivation group (1).

To investigate different dependent measurements, such as the Stroop task, Gailliot et al. (1) (study 4) found marginally higher accuracy in the experimental group (M = 97.50, SD = 3.04) compared to the control group (M = 94.04, SD = 6.81) after 2 weeks of self-regulatory training programme using non-dominant hands. Moreover, Cranwell et al. (49) (study 1) found that 4 weeks of smartphone application-based Stroop task training programme can significantly improve reaction time in the experimental group compared to the control group (M = 725.39, SD = 207.06 vs. M = 985.22, SD = 197.26, F(1, 26) =10.84, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.294). Furthermore, to conduct more ecologically valid research and determine whether increased self-regulation strength could transfer from a lab setting to the real world, Allom and Mullan (52) conducted two studies and found that after a certain period of self-regulatory strength training, the vulnerability of depletion among participants reduced dramatically, rather than health outcomes (e.g., eating behavior and body mass). More important to the current review, the cognitive outcomes showed inconsistent results. Specifically, when participants performed 20 number trials of the Stroop task, there was no significant difference in the reaction time (p > 0.05). In study 2, Allom and Mullan increased the number of trials in the Stroop task to 50, and the results showed a significant difference in the reaction time among food-specific (M = 32.10, SD = 69.64), general (M = 45.33, SD = 35.21), and no inhibition (M = 132.45, SD = 35.21) groups. However, consistent with the previous study (50), follow-up test after 1 week of training programme did not show any differences among food-specific (M = 108.92, SD = 74.55), general (M = 115.03, SD = 84.25), and no inhibition (M = 122.33, SD = 86.05) groups. Therefore, Allom and Mullan argued that a certain training paradigm could strengthen self-regulation, but does not necessarily benefit health behaviors (e.g., rejecting chocolate) in real life associated with self-regulatory strength. Moreover, improvements in self-regulation could not be maintained over time.

Finally, to test a different dependent measurement (impulsive aggression), Denson et al. (48) conducted a 2-week non-dominant hand training programme. The result showed that impulsive aggression was significantly reduced among the participants who were high in trait aggression (t(66) = 2.15, p = 0.04) (48). The study further demonstrated that the effect of self-regulatory strength is cross-domain in various performances.

Summary of Findings

The summary of findings table (Table 3) shows the certainty of evidence assessment based on different outcomes in the subsequent performance. Overall, the certainty or quality is low due to the serious risk of bias and imprecision. All included studies initially were graded from high-level certainty because their study designs are RCTs. Then, the overall certainty was downgraded, if necessary, as each domain was assessed. Finally, the evidence for physical performance measured by the handgrip test was downgraded to very low as a result of inconsistency (53), high risk of bias (49), and imprecision in sample size calculation (19, 49, 53) in the reported studies. Meanwhile, the other three outcomes (see Table 3) showed downgraded scores due to the high risk of bias (1, 5052, 54) and imprecision (4550, 54).

Discussion

In this review, we sought to evaluate the literature on the manipulation of self-regulatory strength to counter mental exertion and improve physical and/or cognitive performance according to the strength model of self-regulation. The results shed light on the intervention for future studies.

Prior Mental Exertion

The duration of the majority of prior mental exertion is less than 30 min (see Table 2), which has been recognized as a cut point of ego depletion and mental fatigue (33, 39). Only one study recruited a 90-min mental exertion (54) programme and investigated the intervention to counter mental fatigue. Since various durations of cognitive stimulation may have different effects (61, 62) of prior mental exertions on subsequent performance, it raises a variety of questions. For example, could a longer duration of prior mental exertion induce a higher level of fatigue? Can self-regulatory training programmes increase sufficient strength to counter all the exertions? And perhaps more likely, could the ensuing 2 weeks of strength training (typically used in previous studies; see Table 2) be of insufficient intensity and duration to have an impact on the effect of pre-mental exertion of more than 30 min in ‘mental fatigue' subject area [e.g., (34, 62, 63)] This may be the reason for the study of Filipas et al. (54) to recruit 4 weeks of the training programme and counter mental fatigue, rather than 2 weeks of training.

The majority of extracted studies recruited prior mental exertion, which tested inhibition, such as thought suppression (1, 19, 4547), letter typing (50, 52), four consecutive tasks (53), and a Stroop task (49, 51). Inhibition refers to controlling one's impulse about attention, emotion, and behavior to override an automatic response (64, 65), which usually happens in sports scenarios. For example, soccer players should exert inhibition frequently to suppress an ongoing activity because they perform in a rapidly changing environment (66, 67) and easy to get mental fatigue in a prolonged duration of matches (90 min). Therefore, it may be promising to implement some training programmes in the subject areas of “ego depletion” (see Table 2) and “mental fatigue” to counter fatigue and improve subsequent performance in sports. However, future studies should consider the duration of the training programme.

Notably, the strength model indicates that self-regulation is a global resource account, suggesting a domain interaction for ego depletion, and a prominent analysis investigated by Hagger (9) first showed the same effects for matched and unmatched tasks.

The current review supported this “global” hypothesis and indicated training programmes could counter prior mental exertion tasks in different domains. For example, Denson et al. (48) recruited a non-dominant hand using programme to increase self-regulatory strength. The result showed the counteractive effect could appear in the emotional domain with the Taylor aggression paradigm. Thus, future studies could consider manipulating training programmes to counter prior mental exertion in different domains, such as emotion and cognition.

More to the point, the study (48) shed light on the fact that emotional regulation could consume the same resource pool of self-regulation and can also be increased by the self-regulatory strength training. According to the neurovisceral integration model (68, 69), emotional, cognitive, and behavioral self-regulation is correlated with the autonomic nervous system measured by some physiological indicators, such as heart rate variability (70, 71) and skin conductance response (72, 73). Also, because these indicators changed significantly when an individual is involved in social interactions (74, 75), these training programmes may be beneficial for some behaviors, such as the maintenance of comfortable interpersonal space and defensive responses of fearful faces. However, this hypothesis should be tested by future studies.

Training Programmes

A variety of training programmes were investigated. Among them, non-dominant hand use is eye-catching, as it was used in four investigations [(1, 48), study 2 and study 4 (53)]. Motor movements with the non-dominant hand are less intuitive and spontaneous, necessitating the use of greater cognitive resources (self-regulation) (76). Image studies have verified that using the non-dominant hand interferes with cognitive processing, and executing a motor task with the non-dominant hand increases cortical activity (77). Consistently, According to Jäncke et al. (78), executing a consecutive movement with the non-dominant hand (in right-handed subjects) leads to increased right hemisphere activation. Thus, it is not surprising that completion number in anagram and accuracy in Stroop task were increased, respectively [(1) study 2 and study 4], because the strength of self-regulation was improved after a 2-week training programme. However, this increased strength seems to be temporary after training programmes, and two studies did not find significant improvement at the follow-up test (1 week after the test) (50, 52).

In the recent decade, the resource model has met many challenges. For example, many scholars questioned what is exactly the resource if self-regulation depends on a limited resource? Gailliot et al. (1) conducted a study to manifest that the metaphorical resource may be blood glucose. Nevertheless, this hypothesis was never tested successfully despite its compelling attractiveness (79). Finally, Finley et al. (80) conducted the most stringent test and found no evidence to support the glucose hypothesis. Although the current review supports the resource model and showed the strength of self-regulation can be improved, future studies should examine the mechanism that underlies this improvement.

Moreover, motivation was placed at the center of self-regulation, rather than a resource, such as a process self-regulation model (81, 82). Specifically, these researchers questioned if individuals are unable to restrain themselves due to a lack of resources, observing how motivating incentives might instantaneously reverse depletion. For example, some studies showed providing additional motivation (e.g., being kind to participants or telling participants the research would have a good cause) could ease the effect of ego depletion (83, 84). Also, the effect of ego depletion could be buffered by either the idea of money (85) or real money (86), because people are motivated to conserve more resources for the next stage (20).

The resource model has been extensively updated. For example, Baumeister and Vohs (13, 20) acknowledged the deficiency and remedied motivation as an ingredient/variable in the model. That is, motivation plays a role in the mobilization of strength. The authors argued that if people have high motivation and want to measure up to some certain standard, this may compensate for somewhat low self-regulatory resources or enhance the monitoring process. Motivational differences may cause inconsistent results between the study of Miles et al. (53) and other studies listed in Table 2.

Therefore, the current review argued that the strength of the self-regulatory training programme makes humans less vulnerable to ego depletion or mental fatigue; however, the effect of these training programmes is dependent on motivation.

Performance Outcome

To evaluate the performance outcome, the current review follows the study of Friedman and Miyake (87) by recognizing the suggested categorization on which the matching of depleting and dependent programs was based. For example, all performance outcomes related to impulse control tasks (e.g., Stroop task, handgrip, and aggression inhibition) are categorized as “inhibition” (see Table 2: Domain of the Outcome).

From this categorization, the current review found the effect of these training programmes could be cross-domain, since many studies recruited unmatched types of tasks (Table: Similarity) between intervention and performance and showed significant improvement [(49, 51), study 2 (1), study 1 and 2 (46)]. This finding supports the strength model of self-regulation that when training this strength in a domain, it can improve the performance in an unrelated task (11, 13, 17, 20). Thus, it confirms the findings described in Section Prior Mental Exertion, which shows the resource of self-regulation is “global”.

It is worthy to further note that Gailliot et al. (1) (study 1 and study 2) used the lower-level executive function of inhibition to increase self-regulatory strength, while the higher-level executive function of problem-solving improved considerably. Therefore, the cross-domain improvement scenario can occur at different orders of executive function (low vs. high order) [see Diamond (64): the clarification of executive functions].

Based on this review, only one investigation examined the training programme to overcome mental fatigue and improve subsequent endurance performance (54). Here, we highlight the caution, because this study did not explicitly mention that the training programme is related to the strength of self-regulation. Nevertheless, we include it because numerous studies have shown that regular physical exercise can increase self-regulation resources (88, 89), even though it did not report or measure the ability of self-regulation.

Limitations

This systematic review poses a few noteworthy limitations. First, this systematic review, conducted rigorously, is not a meta-analysis, because of the heterogeneity across measurement and training programmes. Moreover, the suggested categorization of self-regulatory tasks (between intervention and performance test) did not present specific task demands, such as inhibition vs. initiating actions. Thus, a future study can investigate more details about this similarity between the training programme and performance test to see the cross-domain effect of self-regulatory strength. Finally, selecting only publications written in English may further limit the representation of the results.

Conclusion

Overall, strength as an important ingredient in the resource model can be trained to counter prior mental exertion and improve subsequent cognitive and physical performance. Cross-domain effects (emotional and cognitive domains; higher and lower levels of executive functions) were found for self-regulatory strength. However, motivation plays a key role to mobilize this resource. Future studies should examine the mechanism that underlies the strength and should also apply these training programmes for social interactions.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2022.904374/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Gailliot MT, Plant EA, Butz DA, Baumeister RF. Increasing self-regulatory strength can reduce the depleting effect of suppressing stereotypes. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. (2007) 33:281–94. doi: 10.1177/0146167206296101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Nigg JT. Annual research review: On the relations among self- regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Physiol Behav. (2017) 176:139–48. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12675

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Bray SR, Ginis KAM, Hicks AL, Woodgate J. Effects of self-regulatory strength depletion on muscular performance and EMG activation. Psychophysiology. (2008) 45:337–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00625.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Bray SR, Ginis KAM, Woodgate J. Self-regulatory strength depletion and muscle-endurance performance: A test of the limited-strength model in older adults. J Aging Phys Act. (2011) 19:177–88. doi: 10.1123/japa.19.3.177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Englert C, Wolff W. Ego Depletion and Persistent Performance in a Cycling Task. Jpn J Radiol Technol. (2013) 49:785–785.

Google Scholar

6. Furley P, Bertrams A, Englert C, Delphia A. Ego depletion, attentional control, and decision making in sport. Psychol Sport Exerc. (2013) 14:900–4. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2013.08.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Englert C. The strength model of self-control in sport and exercise psychology. Front Psychol. (2016) 7:1–9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Englert C, Graham JD, Bray SR. Self-control in sport and exercise psychology. Sport Exer Perform Psychol. (2020) 9:161–6. doi: 10.1037/spy0000205

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Hagger. Sleep, self-regulation, self-control and health. Stress Health. (2010) 26:181–5. doi: 10.1002/smi.1345

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. McMorris T, Barwood M, Hale BJ, Dicks M, Corbett J. Cognitive fatigue effects on physical performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Physiol Behav. (2018) 188:103–7. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.01.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Baumeister RF, Bratslavsky E, Muraven M, Tice DM. Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? J Pers Soc Psychol. (1998) 74:1252–65. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Muraven M, Tice DM, Baumeister RF. Self-control as limited resource: regulatory depletion patterns. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1998) 74:774–89. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Baumeister RF, Vohs KD. Self-Regulation, Ego Depletion, and Motivation. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. (2007) 1:115–28. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00001.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Baumeister RF, Vohs KD. Willpower, choice, and self-control. In: Loewenstein G, Read D, Baumeister R, (Eds.) Time and Decision: Economic and Psychological Perspectives on Intertemporal Choice. (2003) (pp. 201–216). Russell Sage Foundation.

Google Scholar

15. Dang J. An updated meta-analysis of the ego depletion effect. Psychol Res. (2018) 82:645–51. doi: 10.1007/s00426-017-0862-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Hagger MS, Wood C, Stiff C, Chatzisarantis NLD. Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. (2010) 136:495–525. doi: 10.1037/a0019486

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Baumeister RF, Heatherton TF. Self-regulation failure: an overview. Psychol Inquiry. (1996) 7:1–15. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0701_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Muraven M, Baumeister RF. Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-Control resemble a muscle? Psychol Bull. (2000) 126:247–59. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Muraven M, Baumeister RF, Tice DM. Longitudinal Improvement of Self-Regulation Through Practice. J Soc Psychol. (1999) 42:130–53.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

20. Baumeister RF, Vohs KD. Strength model of self-regulation as limited resource: assessment, controversies, update. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. (2016) 54:67–127. doi: 10.1016/bs.aesp.2016.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Boksem MA, Tops M. Mental fatigue: costs and benefits. Brain Res Rev. (2008) 59:125–39. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Marcora SM, Staiano W, Manning V. Mental fatigue impairs physical performance in humans. J Appl Physiol. (2009) 106:857–64. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.91324.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Pageaux B, Marcora SM, Lepers R. Prolonged mental exertion does not alter neuromuscular function of the knee extensors. Med Sci Sports Exerc. (2013) 45:2254–64. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e31829b504a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Brownsberger J, Edwards A, Crowther R, Cottrell D. Impact of mental fatigue on self-paced exercise. Int J Sports Med. (2013) 34:1029–36. doi: 10.1055/s-0033-1343402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Pageaux B, Marcora SM, Rozand V, Lepers R. Mental fatigue induced by prolonged self-regulation does not exacerbate central fatigue during subsequent whole-body endurance exercise. Front Hum Neurosci. (2015) 67:1–12. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Boksem MAS, Meijman TF, Lorist MM. Effects of mental fatigue on attention: An ERP study. Cognitive Brain Res. (2005) 25:107–16. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Faber LG, Maurits NM, Lorist MM. Mental fatigue affects visual selective attention. PLoS ONE. (2012) 7:1–10. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Smith MR, Marcora SM, Coutts AJ. Mental fatigue impairs intermittent running performance. Med Sci Sports Exerc. (2015) 47:1682–90. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000592

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Trecroci A, Boccolini G, Duca M, Formenti D, Alberti G. Mental fatigue impairs physical activity, technical and decision-making performance during small-sided games. PLoS ONE. (2020) 15:1–12. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0238461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Fortes Lima-Junior D, Gantois P, Nasicmento-Junior JRA, Fonseca FS. Smartphone use among high level swimmers is associated with mental fatigue and slower 100-and 200-but not 50-meter freestyle racing. Percep Motor Skill. (2021) doi: 10.1177/0031512520952915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Moreira A, Aoki MS, Franchini E, da Silva Machado DG, Paludo AC, Okano AH. Mental fatigue impairs technical performance and alters neuroendocrine and autonomic responses in elite young basketball players. Physiol Behav. (2018) 196:112–8. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.08.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Smith MR, Coutts AJ, Merlini M, Deprez D, Lenoir M, Marcora SM. Mental fatigue impairs soccer-specific physical and technical performance. Med Sci Sports Exerc. (2016) 48:267–76. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000762

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Sun H, Soh KG, Roslan S, Wazir MRWN, Soh KL. Does mental fatigue affect skilled performance in athletes? A systematic review. PLOS ONE. (2021) 16. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0258307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Gantois P, Ferreira MEC, de Lima-Junior D, Nakamura FY, Batista GR, Fonseca FS, et al. Effects of mental fatigue on passing decision- making performance in professional soccer athletes. Eur J Sport Sci. (2019) 20:534–43. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2019.1656781

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Russell S, Jenkinsa D, Smith M, Halsone S, Kelly V. The application of mental fatigue research to elite team sport performance: New perspectives. J Sci Med Sport. (2019) 22:723–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2018.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Martin K, Thompson KG, Keegan R, Rattray B. Are individuals who engage in more frequent self-regulation less susceptible to mental fatigue? J Sport Exer Psychol. (2019) 41:289–97. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2018-0222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Martin K, Staiano W, Menaspà P, Hennessey T, Marcora S, Keegan R, et al. intervention Superior inhibitory control and resistance to mental fatigue in professional road cyclists. PLoS ONE. (2016) 11:1–15. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0159907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Thompson CJ, Smith A, Coutts AJ, Skorski S, Datson N, Smith MR, et al. Understanding the presence of mental fatigue in elite female football. Res Q Exerc Sport. (2021) 1–12. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2021.1873224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Van Cutsem J, Marcora S, De Pauw K, Stephen B, Meeusen R, Roelands B. The effects of mental fatigue on physical performance: a systematic review. Sports Med. (2017) 47:1569–88. doi: 10.1007/s40279-016-0672-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Smith MR, Thompson C, Marcora SM, Skorski S, Meyer T, Coutts AJ. Mental fatigue and soccer: current knowledge and future directions. Sports Med. (2018) 48:1525–32. doi: 10.1007/s40279-018-0908-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Russell S, Jenkins D, Rynne S, Halson SL, Kelly V. What is mental fatigue in elite sport? Perceptions from athletes and staff. Eur J Sport Sci. (2019) 19:1367–76. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2019.1618397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. MacMahon C, Parrington L, Pickering T, Aitken B, Schücker L. Understanding the effects of cognitive tasks on physical performance: a constraints framework to guide further research. Int Rev Sport Exer Psychol. (2021) doi: 10.1080/1750984X.2021.1907854

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Brown DMY, Graham JD, Innes KI, Harris S, Flemington A, Bray SR. Effects of Prior Cognitive Exertion on Physical Performance: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis (Vol. 50) (2019). Berlin: Springer International Publishing. doi: 10.1007/s40279-019-01204-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Moher D, Shamseer L, Clarke M, Ghersi D, Liberati A, Petticrew M, et al. Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Syst Rev. (2015) 4:1–9. doi: 10.1186/2046-4053-4-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Oaten M, Cheng K. Improved self-control: the benefits of a regular program of academic study. Ter Arkh. (2006) 65:22–5.

Google Scholar

46. Oaten M, Cheng K. Longitudinal gains in self-regulation from regular physical exercise. Br J Health Psychol. (2006) 11:717–33. doi: 10.1348/135910706X96481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Oaten M, Cheng K. Improvements in self-control from financial monitoring. J Econ Psychol. (2007) 28:487–501. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2006.11.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Denson TF, Capper MM, Oaten M, Friese M, Schofield TP. Self-control training decreases aggression in response to provocation in aggressive individuals. J Res Pers. (2011) 45:252–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2011.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Cranwell J, Benford S, Houghton RJ, Golembewksi M, Fischer JE, Hagger MS. Increasing self-regulatory energy using an internet-based training application delivered by smartphone technology. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. (2014) 17:181–6. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2013.0105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Bertrams A, Schmeichel BJ. Improving self-control by practicing logical reasoning. Self Identity. (2014) 13:419–31. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2013.836562

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Bray SR, Graham JD, Saville PD. Self-control training leads to enhanced cardiovascular exercise performance. J Sports Sci. (2015) 33:534–43. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2014.949830

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Allom V, Mullan B. Two inhibitory control training interventions designed to improve eating behaviour and determine mechanisms of change. Appetite. (2015) 89:282–90. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.02.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Miles E, Sheeran P, Baird H, Macdonald I, Webb TL, Harris PR. Does self-control improve with practice? Evidence from a six-week training program. J Exp Psychol Gener. (2016) 145:1075–91. doi: 10.1037/xge0000185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Filipas L, Martin K, Northey JM, La Torre A, Keegan R, Rattray B. A 4-week endurance training program improves tolerance to mental exertion in untrained individuals. J Sci Med Sport. (2020) 23:1215–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2020.04.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Balshem H, Helfand M, Schünemann HJ, Oxman AD, Kunz R, Brozek J, et al. GRADE guidelines: 3. Rating the quality of evidence. J Clin Epidemiol. (2011) 64:401–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. GRADE working group. Grading quality of evidence and strength of recommendations. BMJ. (2004) 328:1490. doi: 10.1136/bmj.328.7454.1490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Vist G, Kunz R, Brozek J, Alonso-Coello P, et al. GRADE guidelines: 4. Rating the quality of evidence—study limitations (risk of bias). J Clin Epidemiol. (2011) 64:407–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.07.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Martin KM. The Impact of Mental Fatigue on Physiological, Psychological Performance Variables During Exercise. [Doctoral dissertation, University of Canberra] (2016). Research system Canberra. Available on: https://researchsystem.canberra.edu.au/ws/portalfiles/portal/33686043/file

Google Scholar

59. Hofmann W, Schmeichel BJ, Baddeley AD. Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends Cogn Sci. (2012) 16:174–180. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Schmeichel BJ, Vohs KD, Baumeister RF. Intellectual performance and ego depletion: role of the self in logical reasoning and other information processing. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2003) 85:33–46. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.33

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Chen J, Duan Y, Li H, Lu L, Liu J, Tang C. Different durations of cognitive stimulation therapy for Alzheimer's disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Interven Aging. (2019) 14:1243–54. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S210062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Sun H, Soh KG, Xu X. Nature Scenes Counter Mental Fatigue-Induced Performance Decrements in Soccer Decision-Making. Front Psychol. (2022) 13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.877844

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Fortes GP, de Lima-Júnior D, Barbosa BT, Ferreira MEC, Nakamura FY, et al. Playing videogames or using social media applications on smartphones causes mental fatigue and impairs decision-making performance in amateur boxers. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. (2021). doi: 10.1080/23279095.2021.1927036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Diamond A. Executive Functions. ANAE - Approche Neuropsychologique des Apprentissages chez l'Enfant. (2013) 64:135–68. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Bertrams A, Englert C, Dickhäuser O. Self-control strength in the relation between trait test anxiety and state anxiety. J Res Pers. (2010) 44:738-741. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2010.09.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Barte JCM, Nieuwenhuysa A, Geurtsa SAE, Kompier MAJ. Motivation counteracts fatigue-induced performance decrements in soccer passing performance. J Sports Sci. (2018) 37:1189–96. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2018.1548919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Thompson CJ, Noon M, Towlson C, Perry J, Coutts AJ, Harper LD, et al. Understanding the presence of mental fatigue in English academy soccer players. J Sports Sci. (2020) 38:1-7. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2020.1746597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Thayer JF, Lane RD. A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. J Affect Disord. (2000) 61:201–16. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00338-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Thayer JF, Lane RD. Perseverative Thinking and Health: Neurovisceral Concomitants. Psychol Health. (2002) 17:685–95. doi: 10.1080/08870440290025867

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Segerstrom SC, Nes LS. Heart rate variability reflects self-regulatory strength, effort, and fatigue. Psychol Sci. (2007) 18:275–81. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01888.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Smith TW, Cribbet MR, Nealey-Moore JB, Uchino BN, Williams PG, Mackenzie J, et al. Matters of the variable heart: respiratory sinus arrhythmia response to marital interaction and associations with marital quality. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2011) 100:103–19. doi: 10.1037/a0021136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Lazarus RS, Speisman JC, Mordkoff AM. The relationship between autonomic indicators of psychological stress: heart rate and skin conductance. Psychos Med. (1963) 25:19–30. doi: 10.1097/00006842-196301000-00004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Mackersie CL, Calderon-Moultrie N. Autonomic nervous system reactivity during speech repetition tasks: Heart rate variability and skin conductance. Ear Hear. (2016) 37:118S−25S. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Candini M, Battaglia S, Benassi M, di Pellegrino G, Frassinetti F. The physiological correlates of interpersonal space. Sci Rep. (2021) 11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-82223-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Ellena G, Battaglia S, Làdavas E. The spatial effect of fearful faces in the autonomic response. Exp Brain Res. (2020) 238:2009–18. doi: 10.1007/s00221-020-05829-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Klaming L, Vlaskamp BNS. Non-dominant hand use increases completion time on part B of the trail making test but not on part A. Behav Res Meth. (2018) 50:1074–87. doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0927-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Mattay VS, Callicott JH, Bertolinoa A, Santhaa AKS, Van Horna JD, et al. Hemispheric control of motor function: a whole brain echo planar fMRI study. Psychiatry Res Neuroimag. (1998) 83:7–22. doi: 10.1016/S0925-4927(98)00023-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Jäncke L, Peters M, Schlaug G, Posse S, Steinmetz H, Müller-Gärtner HW. Differential magnetic resonance signal change in human sensorimotor cortex to finger movements of different rate of the dominant and subdominant hand. Cogn Brain Res. (1998) 6:279–84. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(98)00003-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Kurzban R. Does the brain consume additional glucose during self-control tasks? Evol Psychol. (2010) 8:244–59. doi: 10.1177/147470491000800208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Finley AJ, Tang D, Schmeichel BJ. Sweet nothings: No effects of self-control exertion on blood glucose levels. Soc. Psychol. (2019) 50:322–31. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000376

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Inzlicht M, Schmeichel BJ. What is ego depletion? Toward a mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2012) 7:450–63. doi: 10.1177/1745691612454134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Inzlicht M, Schmeichel BJ, Macrae CN. Why self-control seems (but may not be) limited. Trends Cogn Sci. (2014) 18:127–33. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Luethi MS, Friese M, Binder J, Boesiger P, Luechinger R, Rasch B. Motivational incentives lead to a strong increase in lateral prefrontal activity after self-control exertion. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2016) 11:1618–26. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Masicampo EJ, Martin SR, Anderson RA. Understanding and overcoming self-control depletion. Soc Pers Psychol Compass. (2014) 8:638–49. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Boucher HC, Kofos MN. The idea of money counteracts ego depletion effects. J Exp Soc Psychol. (2012) 48:804–10. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Zhu Z, Li J, Zhang B, Li Y, Zhang H. The effect of motivation and positive affect on ego depletion: Replenishment versus release mechanism. Int J Psychol. (2017) 52:445–52. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Friedman NP, Miyake A. The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. J Exp Psychol Gen. (2004) 133:101–35. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Lambourne K, Tomporowski P. The effect of exercise-induced arousal on cognitive task performance: a meta-regression analysis. Brain Res. (2010) 1341:12–24. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.03.091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Lowe CJ, Hall PA, Vincent CM, Luu K. The effects of acute aerobic activity on cognition and cross-domain transfer to eating behavior. Front Hum Neurosci. (2014) 8:267. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: self-regulation, ego depletion, mental fatigue, self-regulatory strength, physical performance, cognitive performance

Citation: Sun H, Soh KG, Norjali Wazir MRW, Ding C, Xu T and Zhang D (2022) Can Self-Regulatory Strength Training Counter Prior Mental Exertion? A Systematic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials. Front. Public Health 10:904374. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.904374

Received: 25 March 2022; Accepted: 09 May 2022;
Published: 10 June 2022.

Edited by:

Yuka Kotozaki, Iwate Medical University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Haitham Jahrami, Arabian Gulf University, Bahrain
Simone Battaglia, University of Bologna, Italy

Copyright © 2022 Sun, Soh, Norjali Wazir, Ding, Xu and Zhang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: He Sun, verson.upm@gmail.com; Kim Geok Soh, kims@upm.edu.my

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.