Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 13 October 2023
Sec. Movement Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Moving the Mind, Thinking the Body: New Insights on the Mind-Body Connection From the Neuroscience of Movement, Sports, Arts, Yoga, and Meditation View all 8 articles

Aerobic physical exercise versus dual-task cognitive walking in cognitive rehabilitation of people with stroke: a randomized clinical trial

  • 1Institute of Health Sciences, Universidade Católica Portuguesa, Lisbon, Portugal
  • 2Faculdade de Ciências da Saúde e Desporto, Universidade Save, Maxixe, Mozambique
  • 3Escola Superior de Saúde de Alcoitão, Alcabideche, Portugal
  • 4Faculty of Health Sciences, Universidade Europeia, Lisbon, Portugal
  • 5Center for Research in Neuropsychology and Cognitive and Behavioral Intervention (CINEICC), Faculdade de Psicologia e de Ciências da Educação (FPCE), Universidade de Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal
  • 6Psychological Assessment and Psychometrics Laboratory (PsyAssessmentLab), Faculdade de Psicologia e de Ciências da Educação (FPCE), Universidade de Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal
  • 7Center for Interdisciplinary Research in Health, Universidade Católica Portuguesa, Lisbon, Portugal

Introduction: Stroke is a neurological deficit caused by an acute focal injury to the central nervous system due to vascular injury that can result in loss of neurological function, lasting brain damage, long-term disability and, in some cases, death. The literature reports that aerobic physical exercise, as well as dual-task cognitive walking, are used for the cognitive recovery of people with stroke. We aimed to assess whether aerobic physical exercise influences post-stroke cognitive recovery, namely performance on selective and sustained attention. We tested the hypothesis that post-stroke aerobic physical exercise leads to more significant gains than post-stroke dual-task cognitive walking.

Methods: We used a Randomized Clinical Trial, single-blind, parallel group, to verify the existence of differences between two groups. A total of 34 patients with subacute to chronic stroke were divided into two groups to train three times a week for 12 weeks: the aerobic physical exercise (PE) group engaged in 20 min on a treadmill, 20 min on a stationary bicycle and 5 min on a desk bike pedal exerciser per session; the dual-task (DT) gait exercise group walked for 45 min while simultaneously performing cognitive tasks per session. All participants were assessed on cognitive functioning with the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and d2 Test of Attention before acute interventions and post interventions. We have also applied a Visual Analog Scale to monitor the participants’ perceived difficulty, pre-, post-acute, and post-chronic interventions. Participants also responded to a Borg Scale of perceived exertion following the acute and the final session of chronic training.

Results: A mixed model ANOVA revealed a significant interaction effect with a large effect size for most of the cognitive variables under study. The variables associated with the d2 Test of Attention showed significant differences between the groups, mainly from T0 to T2. Also for MMSE, an ANOVA revealed a significant interaction effect with significant improvements from T0 to T2. Our results strongly suggest that aerobic physical exercise is more beneficial than dual-task cognitive-gait exercise since in the PE group, cognitive attention scores increase, and cognitive impairment and perception of exertion decrease, compared to the DT group.

Conclusion: These findings support that PE provides more significant benefits for patients post-stroke when compared to DT.

1. Introduction

Nowadays, physical activity is decreasing in tandem with the demands of contemporary life, contributing to a greater sedentary lifestyle with health implications (Sawalla Guseh et al., 2022). Physical activity is a voluntary activity produced by skeletal muscles that results in energy expenditure, and exercise represents its practice in a structured form (Mandolesi et al., 2018). Physical activity is recommended for healthy people to prevent disease, promote well-being, and for people with sicknesses to improve their health status (Kim et al., 2013; Sánchez-González et al., 2021). There is evidence that participation in regular physical activity reduces the incidence of a wide range of chronic diseases and mental disorders (Posadzki et al., 2020; Zeibig et al., 2023), as well as preventing the onset of neurodegenerative processes (Sánchez González et al., 2018). The benefits of physical activity are wide-ranging, including improved muscle function and cardiorespiratory function and metabolic regulation (Pang et al., 2013; Bangsbo et al., 2015; Zheng et al., 2016), decreased anxiety and depression levels, increased self-esteem (Mandolesi et al., 2018) and social integration (Schüller and Demetriou, 2018). Physical activity also contributes to the optimization of global cognitive functioning (Levin et al., 2017; Sánchez González et al., 2018) since during exercise, cerebral blood flow remains relatively constant, which influences cerebral oxygenation (Rooks et al., 2010; Phillips et al., 2014; Moriarty et al., 2019).

Stroke is a neurological deficit resulting from an acute focal lesion of the central nervous system due to vascular causes, including cerebral infarction, intracerebral hemorrhage, and subarachnoid hemorrhage (Sacco et al., 2013; Ojaghihaghighi et al., 2017). It is known that in there are dozens of interconnected neural systems in the brain, each defined as a circuit of neurons that gives rise to a specific behavior (Cramer et al., 2021). Stroke consists of a breakdown of many neural systems that can lead to motor, perceptual, cognitive, and behavioral problems (Gilmore and Spaulding, 2001). Stroke survivors are conditioned by fatigue, with a prevalence between 38 and 77% (Lerdal et al., 2009; Saunders et al., 2014), a fact that predisposes to a sedentary lifestyle and increased risk of recurrent stroke (Billinger et al., 2014; Saunders et al., 2014). Physical activity thus plays a neuroprotective and essential role in improving the health of stroke survivors (Kramer et al., 2019). In fact, aerobic physical exercise plays an important role in improving aerobic fitness, cardiovascular fitness, walking speed, endurance, balance, and cognitive skills, among other post-stroke health outcomes (Han et al., 2017; Unibaso-Markaida et al., 2019).

Research reports that the mechanisms that have been proposed to justify the relationship between physical activity and cognition include structural elements related to angiogenesis, synaptogenesis, and neurogenesis, mainly through the positive regulation of growth factors (Hötting and Röder, 2013; Cheval et al., 2020; Novak and Ellis, 2021), brain metabolism, neurotransmitters, oxygen availability, glucose regulation and oxidative stress (Marmeleira, 2013). Physical activity also leads to cardiovascular fitness, which, in turn, is accompanied by increased blood flow, which maintains oxygen levels in the brain, an essential element for maintaining the brain metabolism (Komiyama et al., 2017). Physical activity increases adrenaline in the blood, and high levels of adrenaline in the blood are associated with changes in the central nervous system that improve cognitive functions (Brisswalter et al., 2002). Regarding brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), which is a neurotrophin involved in neural tissue growth and repair (Marston et al., 2017; Lan et al., 2018), studies report that BDNF secretion is enhanced with physical activity and this promotes neuronal survival, increases intracellular calcium levels, facilitates transcription factors, and induces mTOR-mediated mRNA translation for memory consolidation (Loprinzi et al., 2018).

On the other hand, dual task exercises have been encouraged as a type of post-stroke intervention to improve balance, mobility, fall risk and activities of daily living (e.g., Mohammed et al., 2019), and cognition (e.g., Park and Lee, 2019).

Dual tasking is defined as the simultaneous performance of two tasks that can be performed independently, measured separately, and have different objectives (Bayot et al., 2018), such as walking while talking on the phone or while talking to a partner (Agmon et al., 2016). Performing two simultaneous tasks is common in everyday life and represents a highly valuable skill for the individual, and can be considered a prerequisite for a normal life (Fatori et al., 2015; Bayot et al., 2018).

The prefrontal cortex, as well as parietal regions were identified as playing an important role in managing the simultaneous execution of two different tasks, areas associated with attention, verbal fluency, executive functions, working memory, visuospatial (Hars et al., 2014; Yokoyama et al., 2015; Leone et al., 2017; Worringer et al., 2019). A recent study points out that Dual Task gait performance is associated with functional inter-network connectivity between motor and divided attention networks, as well as processing speed (Droby et al., 2022). It has been shown that, dual-task cognitive-gait allows direct training of the brain’s parietal–frontal networks to divide attention and coordinate actions more efficiently (Gregory et al., 2017; Baek et al., 2021b; Yang et al., 2022). Dual-task exercise with cognitive tasks has been shown to benefit cognitive and gait ability after stroke (Kim G. Y. et al., 2014). Since dual-tasking is a crucial ability that impacts the quality of life, dual-task training has been suggested to be a clinically viable intervention for stroke survivors. However, most research finds more significant benefits for motor abilities (e.g., balance ability, gait, and upper limb function) compared to cognitive abilities (Zhou et al., 2021). A possible reason for this is suggested in a recent review on dual-task interference during walking after stroke. This review points to the decrease in performance in one or both tasks due to several possible reasons, the most common being the secondary task requiring internally-driven responses, imposing a more significant cognitive load that competes with the walking motor task (Tsang et al., 2022).

Fundamentally, aerobic physical exercise increases aerobic fitness, which in turn improves brain structure and function as it preserves brain plasticity (Hars et al., 2014), while cognitive training greatly improves selective brain function (Yu et al., 2018; Liao et al., 2019) as it improves attention, memory, and executive dysfunctions (Kounti et al., 2011). Both aerobic physical exercise and dual task gait involve brain structures such as the prefrontal cortex and cerebellum that interact and exchange motor and cognitive information, affording mutual support and inhibition (Kounti et al., 2011; Yokoyama et al., 2015). However, despite reports of improvement in motor skills as a result of dual-task training, a systematic review of 13 studies shows that the beneficial effect of dual-task training on cognitive function was provided by only one of the 13 studies, rendering the cognitive benefits of dual-task training, inconclusive (He et al., 2018).Hence, we enquire if aerobic physical exercise, which also impacts motor performance, might reap greater cognitive benefits as it does not entail interference and has been shown to impact cognition via direct and indirect mechanisms. Indeed, aerobic physical exercise promotes various physiological adaptations, such as angiogenesis and neurogenesis, that positively affect cortical function (Chin et al., 2015; Koch et al., 2020). Despite the literature suggesting both aerobic physical exercise and dual-task cognitive-gait as beneficial for stroke survivors, we hypothesize that post-stroke aerobic physical exercise might lead to greater cognitive gains when compared to performing post-stroke dual-task cognitive-gait, as the constant foot drag produced by aerobic physical exercise on the treadmill provides an increase in gait rhythm and automaticity, and favors cognitive improvement as well as improvement in motor function (Penati et al., 2019). Therefore, with this study, we aim to investigate if aerobic physical exercise and dual task cognitive-gait differently influence the cognitive recovery of patients with stroke, namely in attention and concentration performance. Importantly, this is the first study that compares the impact of an aerobic exercise series with a more traditional dual-task cognitive walking task on cognitive recovery of people with stroke. Moreover, to our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the relationship between effort perception and exercise type. These elements are very important for the elaboration of an optimized rehabilitation protocol involving people with stroke.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Trial design

This randomized controlled trial, used a single-blind, parallel group, took place at Inhambane Provincial Hospital and Chicuque Rural Hospital, both in Inhambane Province, Mozambique. Individuals with acute and chronic ischemic stroke were recruited and randomly selected for: (1) Physical Exercise (PE) or (2) Dual Task (DT) groups.

This manuscript has been prepared in accordance with the Standards of CONSORT 2010 statement: Updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomized trials (Schulz et al., 2011) and Template for Intervention Description and Replication (Hoffmann et al., 2014).

The Ethics Committee for Health of the local university in Portugal and the National Committee on Bioethics for Health in Mozambique approved the study. All participants provided written informed consent, and the study followed the ethical guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki for research involving human subjects (World Medical Association, 2013).

2.2. Participants

Participants were recruited from rehabilitation centers in Inhambane in Mozambique, and through word of mouth between July 2021 and May 2022.

Eligibility criteria included: patients with subacute to chronic ischemic stroke (Guo et al., 2020; Stinear et al., 2020); age equal to or greater than 40 years; comprehension of the Portuguese language; absence of communication and motor deficits that would limit participation in the study protocol; Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) cut-off point ≥19, taking into account the low education level of the participants (Bertolucci et al., 1994; Sasaki et al., 2015; Yeh et al., 2019); and residence less than 50 km from the study site, so as not to compromise assiduity. Of the 57 participants recruited, 12 were excluded: two were under 40 years old; two due to incomprehension of Portuguese; three due to aphasia and absence of communication; one due to motor deficits that limited participation in the protocol; one due to a MMSE score less than 19; and three because they lived more than 50 kilometers from the site where the research was carried out. Of the remaining 45 participants who signed informed consent, four did not attend the study and seven dropped out before randomization, so the final sample totaled 34 participants, aged between 40 and 75 years, including 19 men and 15 women.

2.2.1. Randomization and blinding

Using a stratified randomization technique (Ivers et al., 2012; Broglio, 2018), blinded participants were assigned to one of two groups, with similar entry characteristics, the aerobic physical exercise group (n = 17) and the Dual Task gait exercise group (n = 17). The stratification took into account the stroke stages (acute x chronic) (Acharya et al., 2013; Mayo et al., 2015). Participants’ ongoing enrollment followed a “first-come, first-served” basis (Kim S. H. et al., 2014; Mayo et al., 2015; Ozdogar et al., 2020).

2.3. Experimental procedure

The study was conducted by a rehabilitation team who monitored the participants’ health status, including a doctor responsible for medical follow-up, a speech therapist who assisted in the screening process of participants, three physiotherapists and an occupational therapist responsible for exercise sessions and conventional physiotherapy, a psychologist who applied neuropsychological tests, and physical education finalists who helped supervise the training sessions.

2.3.1. Intervention

All participants performed three exercise sessions over 12 weeks, totaling 36 sessions. The aerobic physical exercise consisted of 20 min on a treadmill, 20 min on a stationary bike, and 5 min on a table bike. Intensity on the treadmill and resistance on the stationary bike and table bike were adapted to cardiovascular performance, i.e., there was a gradual increase according to the participant’s respiratory rate (Boss et al., 2017; Kim and Yim, 2017). The intensity of aerobic physical exercise was monitored by the Borg Rating of Perceived Exertion (Liu-Ambrose et al., 2022). All participants were encouraged to meet their goals, which would translate into overcoming weekly goals on the treadmill and on the stationary bike (self-overcoming) to improve cardiovascular provision (Jin et al., 2013; Danks et al., 2014). To ensure the physical safety of the participants, two physical education finalists positioned themselves on each side of the treadmill to supervise the training, increase or decrease the intensity according to physical conditioning, and ensure that the participants did not fall. During training both on the ergometric bicycle and on the table bicycle, to strengthen adherence to the pedal, an elastic band was placed on the hemiparetic limb, involving the limb and the pedal. Two experienced physiotherapists supervised all training.

The dual-task exercise consisted of walking for 45 min and performing tasks such as: sitting/standing up while holding a ball, summing numbers, listening and remembering a story, saying the letters of the alphabet, repeating sentences, subtracting numbers in reverse, naming colors of flags, saying words that start with the last letter of a previous word, saying sentences in reverse, and naming fruits. These tasks were selected from previous studies (Kim G. Y. et al., 2014; Sasaki et al., 2015; Liu et al., 2017) and cover several cognitive functions such as attention, memory, language and executive functions (Kim G. Y. et al., 2014; Sasaki et al., 2015; Liu et al., 2017). Similar to the aerobic physical exercise group, participants in the cognitive dual-task gait group were encouraged to walk with weekly goals concerning the covered distance (Danks et al., 2014) as a way to break the vicious cycle of a sedentary lifestyle associated with stroke. Two physiotherapists and one occupational therapist guided the dual-task cognitive-gait sessions. In both groups, considering the installed conditions and the need to prevent adverse events, the intervention was provided to one participant at a time, depending on the order of arrival each day of the intervention. Both groups also benefited from 30 min of conventional physical therapy after each training session, which consisted of muscle-strengthening, balance, and gait activities like previous protocols (Liu et al., 2017).

In both groups, physical parameters such as height and body weight were evaluated immediately before the first training session. Blood pressure and oxygen saturation were measured before and after each training session to monitor participants’ health since low saturation values are often used as a criterion for stopping a stress test (Gaskin and Thomas, 1995). We did not record any incidents or worsening of the participants’ health status during the study.

To assess the presence of cognitive impairment that might compromise participation, we used the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) cut-off point ≥19 (Yeh et al., 2021) before acute (T0) and after 12-week chronic exercise (T2). Attention and concentration performance was assessed at baseline (T0), post-acute (T1) and post-chronic intervention (T2) using the d2 Test of Attention (Brickenkamp, 2007; Maeneja et al., 2022).

To assess participants’ perceived difficulty after 5 min of recovery from the task, a Visual Analog Scale (VAS) (Maeneja et al., 2022), was applied after the d2 Test of Attention. To assess participants’ perceived exertion, the Borg Perceived Effort Rating Scale (Borg, 1982; Aamot et al., 2014) was applied after the acute and last training session of each week throughout the 12-week chronic intervention protocol (Borg, 1982; Maeneja et al., 2022), as shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Research design timeline.

2.3.2. Exercise safety and adherence

Before the start of the exercise, the participants learned to measure their level of effort according to the Borg rating of perceived exertion (RPE). Participants were able to rest whenever they exhibited signs of fatigue. During the 12 weeks of the intervention, participants were instructed to report any adverse events or effects.

Adherence was determined from sessions attendance and calculated as the percentage of total sessions attended. And the average number of sessions attended was 35 in both groups.

2.4. Study measures

2.4.1. d2 test of attention

The d2 Attention Test is a timed, paper-and-pencil cancelation neuropsychological test that can measure individual attention and concentration performance, such as processing speed and quality of short-term performance (Siennicka et al., 2019; Woo et al., 2022). This test includes 14 lines, each consisting of 47 characters ‘p’ and ‘d’ with one to four strokes, arranged around the letters (Siennicka et al., 2019; Maeneja et al., 2022). The test presents three targets: the letter ‘d’ with two strokes above it, two strokes below it, or one stroke above and one below (da Silva-Sauer et al., 2022). The participant must go through each line to identify and cross out as many ‘d’ letters with two strokes as possible in a limited time of 20 s. The total test duration is 4 min and 40 s (Siennicka et al., 2019; Woo et al., 2022).

Evidence suggests that attention is one of the domains that is typically compromised with the onset of stroke (Cumming et al., 2013). Furthermore, it has been shown that attention and concentration improve with physical activity (Spitzer and Hollmann, 2013; Vanhelst et al., 2016; Reigal et al., 2020). Hence, in order to assess if chronic aerobic physical exercise might enhance attention and concentration, we chose the d2 test of attention, which was used before the start of the cognitive rehabilitation exercise program and after the end of the rehabilitation exercise program, as it measures these two processes, directly associated with the cognitive functions of our brain (Brickenkamp and Zilmer, 1998; Loetscher et al., 2019).

According to Brickenkamp (2007) and Steinborn et al. (2018), the d2 Test of Attention can be analyzed by assessing the total characters (TC), representing the number of processed characters, which informs about the speed with which the information is processed, specifically the speed of execution as well as productivity. Moreover, one can assess the total hits (TA) representing the number of characters marked correctly, which informs about the accuracy with which the task is performed. We were particularly interested in assessing the following domains: General Efficiency score (TC-E), representing the total number of characters processed minus the total number of errors, which assesses attention control and inhibition, as well as the relationship between speed and meticulousness in performance, this result informs about the overall performance; Concentration Index (CI), representing the total hits minus total omissions, which assesses the ability to concentrate by evaluating the combination between speed and accuracy; Variability Index (IV), representing the difference between the maximum value (TC max) and the minimum value (TC min), which is an indicator of consistency in the execution of the task; and finally, Error Percentage (E%), representing the percentage of errors made throughout the test, which is an indicator of meticulousness and quality of performance.

As referred above, here we were interested in attention and concentration. Thus, we assessed the following domains: General Efficiency score (TC-E), Concentration Index (CI), Variability Index (IV), and Error Percentage (E%), since with these results it is possible to obtain data on attentional control, ability to concentrate, stability and consistency in the execution of the task. Further, these domains afford the evaluation of the qualitative aspects of performance (Lindemann-Matthies et al., 2021; Maeneja et al., 2022).

2.4.2. Mini-Mental State Examination

The Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) is a 30-point cognitive screening test including five categories, namely orientation, recording, attention and calculation, recall, and language (Molloy and Standish, 1997; Larner, 2020). It is an easy-to-apply test, completed in about 5 to 10 min, depending on the individual’s disability (Larner, 2020). Studies have shown that sociocultural variables, age, and education, can affect individual MMSE scores (Arevalo-Rodriguez et al., 2021). Given the level of education of the participants, wherein 29 of the 34 participants presented a basic schooling level, i.e., between 4 years and 10 years of schooling, we chose to use the Mental State Examination cutoff ≥19 (Paddick et al., 2015, 2017; Lestari et al., 2017; Yeh et al., 2021). Thus, in this study, the MMSE was used before starting the exercise program to screen eligible participants, as well as after the 12-week duration of the exercise program, with the aim of comparing cognitive performance in the pre and post exercise program, as MMSE is a widely used tool to measure cognitive impairment and detect changes over time (Canli and Ozyurda, 2020).

2.4.3. Visual Analog Scale

The VAS was created in a clinical situation in order to measure pain perception (Ismail et al., 2015). It has since been used to assess perceived exertion after physical exercise (Tseng et al., 2010), mental effort (Pageaux and Lepers, 2018; Smith et al., 2019), the psychophysiological response in cognitive tasks (Fuentes-García et al., 2019), and perceived task difficulty (Ouwehand et al., 2021). Given that the participants experienced effort to perform the neuropsychological tests (MMSE and d2), we used the VAS to assess the perceived difficulty associated to this effort. It is possible that perceived difficulty may decrease for some participants, associated to the putative improvement in cognitive performance (Abd-Elfattah et al., 2015; Pageaux and Lepers, 2018; Smith et al., 2019). The Visual Analog Scale (VAS) measures a characteristic or attitude that on a continuum of values showing parametric properties (Dompeyre et al., 2007). It consists of a 100 mm straight line, with a (+) and (−) sign at each end, in which the participant draws a perpendicular line to identify their subjective state according to the research question (Delgado et al., 2018). The VAS is a simple pen and paper test, requiring only a sheet of paper with a printed line in the aforementioned dimension, a preceding question, and a plus and a minus symbol at either end of the line. The line can be horizontal or vertical (we chose vertical lines), as suggested by some authors for use in stroke (Ozyemisci-Taskiran et al., 2019).

Here we asked the participant to rate their perception of difficulty in the MMSE and d2 tasks, by bisecting the vertical line closest to the (+) or (−) symbols of the VAS (Johnson, 2001; Tseng et al., 2010; Delgado et al., 2018).

We used the VAS before starting the exercise program to assess the perception of difficulty in the cognitive tasks, in order to obtain data at baseline. Further, we used the VAS after the cognitive tasks subsequent to the acute exercise, and subsequent to the 12 week exercise program, with the aim of comparing the perception of difficulty in the pre- and post-exercise program (Shahar et al., 2019; Smith et al., 2019; Ouwehand et al., 2021).

2.4.4. The Borg Scale

The Borg Scale or Borg Rating of Perceived Exertion (Borg, 1982) is a method for obtaining subjective estimates of exercise intensity and exertion and in both healthy and patient populations (Aamot et al., 2014), with scores ranging from 6 to 20. Perceived effort is defined as the ability to detect and respond to sensations that arise from the body during exercise (Pageaux, 2016; Cabral et al., 2020). The intensity of a training program is a critical variable in post-stroke treatment gains, and the Borg Scale is a valuable tool for measuring intensity after a stroke, as it is an easy-to-use scale that measures an individual’s self-rated physical exertion during exercise, which can even be accurately assessed by individuals with stroke, regardless of the severity of their motor impairments (Milot et al., 2019). To monitor the participants’ perceived exertion during the activity, the Borg Scale was used after the acute and last training session of each week throughout the 12-week chronic intervention protocol as in a previous study (Maeneja et al., 2022), since the physical fatigue that occurs during physical exercise seems to influence the specific performance of the task, as well as impair performance in tasks that require target detection (Moore et al., 2012). The development of perceived exertion involves numerous neural processes occurring in various regions of the brain, wherein efferents from the central unit are sent from the motor to the sensory regions of the brain, and therefore perceived exertion is considered extremely important in regulating intensity during physical activity (Abbiss et al., 2015). Specifically, during the rehabilitation of people with stroke, exercise intensity is often guided by subjective reports using surrogate measures such as the Borg Scale of Rating of Perceived Exertion (Spring, 2016).Thus, the Borg Scale was used to assess the intensity of exercise from the perspective that it was safe, allowing participants to acquire cardiovascular fitness little by little, thus avoiding adverse events, as it is known that post-stroke individuals have less than half of the cardiorespiratory fitness (Crozier et al., 2018).Therefore, the assessment of subjective perception of effort is essential during physical work, as the symptom of exertion is unique to an individual and can be used as a subjective estimate of physical intensity (Williams, 2017). We would assume that, as the participants rehabilitation progresses, perception of effort should decrease allowing for an increase in physical intensity.

2.5. Statistical analysis

Based on previous studies (Billoir et al., 2015), we calculated the sample size, taking into account a previous study of 20 participants conducted by Kim G. Y. et al. (2014), where the data analyses were performed using SPSS, wherein the normal distribution of the data was confirmed using the Shapiro–Wilk test, and two-way repeated measures ANOVA was also performed to compare changes in cognition between two groups. In this study by Kim G. Y. et al. (2014), the measure of the effect size was large, and therefore we considered an effect size F of 0.46 which corresponds to a large effect. On the other hand, resorting to simulation approaches (Billoir et al., 2015), we calculated on a priori number of participants needed to reach sufficient power using a free software G * Power (Faul et al., 2007, 2009). For an ANOVA statistical test: repeated measures, intermediate interaction, with an effect size of 0.25 (medium effect), a significance level of 0.05, and an observed power of 0.80, we determined a sample of 28 participants. A more conservative sample of 57 participants was engaged to control for experimental mortality and attrition. The analysis followed the intention-to-treat principle (Best et al., 2018), as all randomized participants were included to estimate treatment effects. All participants were engaged in the treatment up to the end of the experiment.

In order to verify the existence of significant differences between the two groups (aerobic physical exercise/dual task exercise) in the sociodemographic variables, inferential statistics were used. The non-parametric Chi-square test was used to compare the two groups regarding gender, since this variable has a nominal scale, and the Fisher test was used to compare the two groups by type of stroke and education, since there was more than 20% of cells with an expected count less than 5. A parametric Independent Sample t-test was also used to compare the two groups in quantitative variables (age, weight, height and MMSE results). These parametric statistical tests were chosen since the distribution was normal (Shapiro test) or presented negligible deviations from normality (SK from −3 to 3 and KU from −7 to 7) according to Marôco (2018). To investigate the primary outcomes, we used Mixed-Model ANOVAs. A mixed model ANOVA, is appropriate when examining for differences in a continuous level variable by group and time. The primary purpose of a mixed ANOVA is to understand if there is an interaction between these two factors on the dependent variable. The independent variable of this study is the type of exercise (aerobic physical exercise /dual-task exercise) and the dependent variable are: Attention, General Efficiency, Concentration Index, Variability Index, Error Percentage. To investigate the secondary outcomes, we also used mixed-model ANOVAs. As we mentioned earlier, the independent variable is the type of exercise (aerobic physical exercise/dual task exercise), and in this case, the secondary dependent variables are: perceived task difficulty measured by VAS and perceived effort measured by the Borg Scale. These are particularly useful as they provide supporting evidence for the primary outcome (Vetter and Mascha, 2017).

These parametric statistical tests were chosen since the distribution was normal or had a less severe deviations from normality (SK from −3 to 3 and KU from −7 to 7), and presenting sphericity (Mauchly test) in the Within ANOVAs and homogeneity of variances (Levéne test) in the Between ANOVAs. We did not include any covariates in this analysis since the groups are similar in sociodemographic variables. A measure of effect size of the was also used (partial eta square effect). The following rules of thumb are used to interpret values for Partial eta squared: 0.01: Small effect size, 0.06: Medium effect size 0.14 or higher: Large effect size. We complemented the mixed ANOVA with one-way ANOVAs and Eta partial square to detect at which moments there are significant differences between the 2 groups and with ANOVAs for repeated measures to see the evolution within each group over the 3 moments. These analyses thus allow a better understanding of the interaction effect.

3. Results

3.1. Participant characteristics

Recruitment took place between July 2021 and May 2022. A total of 34 patients were included in the final analysis in this randomized clinical trial with blinded participants. The study ended when data collection for all recruited participants was complete (Aug 31,2022). Seventeen participants were assigned to each group as shown in Figure 2. As shown in Table 1, the PE and DT training groups were similar concerning baseline characteristics including demographics (age, gender, and education level), weight, and cognitive outcomes on MMSE. In both PE and DT groups, we find a slightly superior percentage of men compared to women (52.9% in PE and 58.8% in DT). In both groups, most participants had been enrolled in basic schooling (up to 10 years of school) (94.1% in PE and 76.5% in DT). The only variable presenting a significant difference between groups is height (p = 0.039), with the PE group with a mean height of 1.63 m and the DT group slightly taller with a mean height of 1.65 m. Although not expressive, the differences in the ratio between men and women amongst the two groups might subtend the difference in height in these same groups.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Flow diagram.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sociodemographic characterization of the PE and DT groups concerning gender, age, weight, height, MMSE at baseline, and schooling.

3.2. Primary outcomes

3.2.1. Cognitive performance pre- to post-intervention

3.2.1.1. d2 test of attention

To assess possible differences on the principal domains of the d2 test of attention, between the PE and DT groups from pre- to post-intervention, we computed several ANOVAs within and between factors.

The results regarding d2 are presented in Table 2. For the General Efficiency score Mixed-Model ANOVA revealed a significant interaction effect with a high large effect size [F(2) = 8.499, p = 0,001, ηp2 = 0.210] and observed power = 0.959. There were no significant differences between groups at T0 and T1, with a significant difference for T2 and a large effect (ηp2 = 0,552) with the PE group showing a higher performance (M = 302.41; SD = 90.68) compared to the DT group (M = 141.29; SD = 54.37). Considering performance over time, in the PE group there was a significant improvement from T0 to T2 (p = 0.006), with a significant difference from T0 to T1 and stabilization from T1 to T2; in the DT Group, there were no significant differences between T0 to T2 (p = 0.317), with an increase from T0 to T1 and decrease from T1 to T2 (Figure 3A).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. ANOVA tests of within and between effects for d2 domains.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Distribution of the mean score over time (from T0 to T1 and from T1 to T2,) in different d2 domains: (A) General Efficiency; (B) Concentration Index; (C) Variability Index; and (D) Error Percentage. * indicates significant differences for p < 0.05; **Indicate significant differences for p < 0.01; and ***Indicate significant differences for p < 0.001. Error bars represent standard errors. Only Between group differences are portrayed. For differences Within groups, please see Table 2.

As for the Concentration Index a Mixed-Model ANOVA revealed a significant interaction effect with a large effect size [F(2) = 17,580, p = 0,000, ηp2 = 0,355] and an observed power = 1.00.

There were no significant differences between groups at T0 and T1, with a significant difference at T2 and a large effect (ηp2 = 0,486), with the PE group presenting a higher performance (M = 104.76; SD = 36.72) compared to the DT group (M = 44.29; SD = 26.63). Considering performance over time, in PE group there was a consistent improvement from T0 to T2 (p = 0.000), while such increase was not observed in the DT Group (p = 0.746) (Figure 3B).

Concerning the Variability Index Mixed-Model ANOVA revealed no significant interaction effect [F(2) = 0,561, p = 0,573, ηp2 = 0,017]. There were significant differences between the groups in the three assessment moments, with the PE group having higher averages [despite performance within each group not showing a significant evolution (Figure 3C)].

Regarding Error Percentage a Mixed-Model ANOVA revealed a significant interaction effect [F(2) = 4.797, p = 0.011, ηp2 = 0.130] and a medium effect size, there were no significant differences between the two groups at T0 (p = 0.176) and T2 (p = 0.411), with a significant difference p < 0.01 at T1 and a large effect size (p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.150), with the PE group presenting a higher result in Error Percentage (mean = 28.06), compared to the DT group (mean = 15.36). Regarding evolution over time within each group, in the PA Group there were no significant differences from T0 to T1 (p = 0.248), with an extremely significant increase in the result from T1 to T2 (p = 0.000), and an extremely significant difference between T0 and T2 (p = 0.000) as well. In the DT Group, the result decreases significantly from T0 to T1 (p = 0.005), and remains stable from T1 to T2 (p = 0.999), with a significant difference between T0 and T2 (p = 0.007) (Figure 3D).

3.2.2. MMSE

A Mixed-Model ANOVA revealed a significant interaction effect [F(1) = 43.267, p = 0.000, ηp2 = 0.575] and a large effect size. As desirable, MMSE test results of did not differ in T0 (MPE = 25.18 vs. MDT = 25.29; p = 0.904) showing no differences between groups pre-intervention, and a small effect size. However, in T2, PE and DT present an extremely significant difference (p = 0.000), and a large effect size, with the PE group increasing its mean score (M = 27.12) and the DT group decreasing its mean score (M = 23.82) as shown in Table 3; Figure 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. MMSE: intra- and inter-group comparisons.

3.3. Secondary outcomes

3.3.1. Perceived difficulty and perceived exertion pre- to post-intervention

3.3.1.1. Visual Analog Scale

Regarding the VAS applied after the d2 Test of Attention, a Mixed-Model ANOVA revealed a significant interaction effect [F(2) = 6.190, p = 0,003, ηp2 = 0.161] and a large effect size. There were no significant differences between PE and DT in T0 and T1. However, we observed a significant difference in T2 (p = 0.004), having the PE group decreased perception of difficulty (p = 0.004) in the cognitive tasks (d2 and MMSE) (M = 2.98; SD = 1.26), compared to the DT group (M = 4.81; SD = 2.08) (Figures 4, 5; Table 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Distribution of the MMSE mean score over time (from T0 to T2). ***Indicate significant differences for p < 0.001. Error bars represent standard errors. Only Between group differences are portrayed. For differences Within groups, please see Table 3.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Evolution of perceived difficulty in cognitive tests (d2 and MMSE in T0 and T2; d2 in T1) across time points. **Indicate significant differences for p < 0.01. Error bars represent standard errors. Only Between group differences are portrayed. For differences Within groups, please see Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. ANOVA tests of within and between effects for the VAS score.

3.3.1.2. Borg rating scale of perceived exertion

Regarding the Borg rating scale, Mixed-Model ANOVA revealed a significant interaction effect [F(1) = 6.963, p = 0.013, ηp2 = 0.179] and a large effect size. Concerning perceived exertion, the groups did not show significant differences after acute training but did so at the end of the intervention program (p = 0.001), with the DT group presenting a higher average (M = 15.07; SD = 2.26) compared to the PE group (M = 13.02; SD = 0.84) (Table 5; Figure 6).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. BORG: intra- and inter-group comparisons.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Evolution of perceived exertion across time points. **Indicate significant differences for p < 0.01. Error bars represent standard errors. Only Between group differences are portrayed. For differences within groups, please see Table 5.

4. Discussion

A recent review that examined randomized clinical trials reports inconclusive evidence of the beneficial effect of exercise interventions exercise on cognitive function (Ciria et al., 2023). Here we aimed to assess whether aerobic physical activity influences post-stroke cognitive recovery, namely on attention and concentration functioning. That is, if aerobic physical exercise presents a distinct impact on cognition compared to the traditional dual-task intervention in patients with stroke. We constituted two groups, with one performing aerobic physical exercise and the other performing dual-task cognitive-gait training, the two groups were equivalent in sociodemographic characteristics such as age, gender, and education. For participant cognitive screening, we used the MMSE which showed that the participants were matched at baseline.

Furthermore, the literature explains that dual tasks include a controlled combination of two tasks or activities performed simultaneously (Netz, 2019), and that its practice reduces the interference between dual tasks over time (Strobach and Schubert, 2017). Here, the PE group consistently presented higher cognitive gains compared to the DT group. Our results showed that cognitive performance of patients with stroke was significantly different after the two types of interventions: aerobic physical exercise and dual-task gait exercise. The results corroborate that in patients with stroke, physical exercise training leads to greater cognitive gains when compared to dual-task gait exercise, with evident improvements after 12 weeks of chronic exercise.

We showed that there are differences in the General Efficiency score over time in the PE group, while in the DT group there was no significant improvement. There was a significant reduction in attention errors after the intervention in the PE group. The decrease in scores observed from T1 to T2, suggests that the DT was not only less effective but even seemed to have been detrimental. It is possible that this might result from therapeutic saturation (Ahmed et al., 2021) that explains that repetitive exercises cease to be challenging, especially from the intensity point of view (Page et al., 2004; Ciria et al., 2023). Another possibility might consist in the so-called “principle of posture first’ where, in challenging tasks (dual-task), there is a tendency to prioritize the motor task to the detriment of the cognitive task (Timmermans et al., 2018). This causes cognitive performance to decrease significantly (Smulders et al., 2012), in association with low motivation, since the tasks introduced by the dual-task were not new, may not have corresponded to the expectations of the participants, and consequently, might not have been motivating. As it is well known, a person with low expectations will have a more internal focus and lower performance (Machan and Krupps, 2021). Therefore, premorbid low levels of cardiorespiratory fitness that likely contribute to post-stroke deconditioning (Billinger et al., 2015), require aerobic exercise at high enough levels to improve cardiorespiratory fitness to promote plasticity (Ploughman and Kelly, 2016). As for the Concentration Index, in the PE group there was a consistent improvement from T0 to T2, while such an increase was not observed in the DT group. This sustains that PE has a superior benefit on concentration. As for the Variability Index, there were significant improvements between the groups across the three moments of evaluation, which points to the possibility that both PE and DT have similar contributions to the consistency of performance. Finally, in relation to the Error Percentage, we noticed a significant decrease in errors post-acute intervention for DT, with stabilization throughout chronic intervention. In the PE group the decrease in error percentage occurred only post-chronic intervention with PE and DT groups ending up with similar error percentages at T2. Thus, there was an increase in meticulousness and quality of performance occurring at different time-points but evolving to the same endpoint. Together, these data show that PE seems to show greater benefits across cognitive domains compared to DT and that the gains in attention and concentration occur in chronic exercise and not exactly in acute exercise, and this is consistent with the fact that relatively long rehabilitation training programs tend to be more effective (Bernhardt et al., 2017; Bo et al., 2019).

These results translate into an improvement in selective and sustained attention, which is in line with other investigations in patients with traumatic brain injury (Blanchet et al., 2016; Romanov et al., 2021).We observed similar results in the MMSE, with a significant improvement in the score between the baseline and post-intervention, and greater significance for the PE group. These results are consistent with previous studies (Fernandez-Gonzalo et al., 2016; Yeh et al., 2021; Liu-Ambrose et al., 2022) and emphasize the influence of physical exercise on improving post-stroke cognitive performance (Chan and Tsang, 2017; Barha et al., 2019; Yeh et al., 2019; Zheng et al., 2020).

The possible motive for the higher cognitive improvement in the PE compared to the DT task might be due to the low motor intensity applied to walking in DT, given the walking difficulties that usually accompany stroke survivors prevented the implementation of a walking pace that would help promote relevant physiological changes (MacKay-Lyons et al., 2020; Yang and Wang, 2021) (in PE the stride was aided by the use of the treadmill), also associated with the fact that in the face of challenging walks (dual-task), a very common adaptation is to walk slower (Timmermans et al., 2018). However, this reflection around the possible reason for the greater cognitive improvement in the PE group compared to the DT task seems confusing, especially when there is a higher RPE (M = 15.07) in the DT group compared with the PE group (M = 13.02). These data have an explanation, as the literature reports that RPE is greater when performance involves a cognitive component (Condello et al., 2019). Further, in this protocol we compare two different exercise modalities, it is natural that the RPE is different, as there is evidence that indicates that perceived exertion is associated with the type of exercise (Andrade et al., 2020; Herold et al., 2020; Lea et al., 2022).

Furthermore, since dual-task interference occurs between 40 and 60% of motor intensities (Kimura et al., 2022), when gait performance is low there is also low performance on neuropsychological measures (Blumen et al., 2014). Therefore, intensity is a factor that drives affective responses to exercise (Jones and Zenko, 2021), as well as associated neural changes and cognitive outcomes (Dhir et al., 2021). A systematic review by Lauenroth et al. (2016), also highlights the importance of intensity, by explaining that an important factor to consider when choosing the type of training, is the intensity of the exercise, as for an intervention program to be effective, it needs an increasing level of difficulty (Lauenroth et al., 2016) to optimize the use of an individual’s latent potential (Law et al., 2014).

Regarding the significant improvements in cognition after the sequenced aerobic and resistance physical activity program (PE group) on the treadmill, stationary bike, and desk pedal exerciser, we believe that this program was motivating from the perspective of novelty, since those training devices were novel to all participants (Parfitt et al., 2012; Wulf and Lewthwaite, 2016; Marcos-Pardo et al., 2018; Lakicevic et al., 2020; Schättin et al., 2021). In addition, the PE condition helped promote generalized biological changes of a rhythmic nature in cardiorespiratory rate, muscle activation time, and brain activity (Combourieu Donnezan et al., 2018; Tomporowski and Qazi, 2020), as the rhythm imposed by the machine regularizes the gait of the person with stroke, with increasingly longer and less variable steps, which generates positive effects on the central nervous system (Borrione, 2014; Penati et al., 2019). Therefore, unlike cognitive training which is designed to have specific effects and improve functioning in the neurocognitive domain, the impact of exercise, on brain health and cognition are broad and non-specific (Dhir et al., 2021). The cardiovascular fitness imposed by this type of program (aerobic and resistance physical exercise) is shown to have sparing effects on the prefrontal, superior parietal, and temporal cortices; as it affects cerebral blood flow, increases capillary density, cerebral angiogenesis, causes neurotrophic stimulation, increases plasticity of neurotransmitter systems, and increases white matter volume in the prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and cerebellum and motor cortex with global cognitive implications (Netz, 2019; Williams, 2021).

Furthermore, it is possible that the cognitive gains observed in the PE group led to the decrease in perceived difficulty in the cognitive task. This might be due to the actual increase in attention and concentration, affording a more focused performance leading to the perception of decreased difficulty (Blanchet et al., 2016; Reigal et al., 2020). Conversely, previous studies (Marino et al., 2009; Ouwehand et al., 2021) have shown that subjective perception of cognition is more related to mood than to performance. Given that mood benefits from even a single bout of exercise as early studies suggest (Yeung, 1996), it is possible that the decrease in perceived difficulty in the cognitive tasks might be an indirect consequence of the PE mechanisms. Indeed, exercise affects mood through certain psychosocial mediators such as self-efficacy, self-esteem, decreased stress and anxiety levels (Chan et al., 2018), as well as through neurophysiological adaptations in the nervous system, such as elevation of body temperature that activates the sympathetic nervous system and the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal hormonal axis, and the hormonal changes that result in reduced pain perception and improved mood, increased endorphin levels and neurotransmitter production (Mikkelsen et al., 2017). The direct benefits of PE such as cardiovascular conditioning (Chapman et al., 2013; Netz, 2019) and increase in arousal (Gallotta et al., 2015; Ouwehand et al., 2021) might affect perceived exertion. The PE group decreased perception of exertion compared to the DT group suggesting that the intervention also contributed to increased fitness of this group.

Our results, along with evidence from the literature (Desjardins-Crepeau et al., 2016; Bo et al., 2019; Baek et al., 2021a; Kimura et al., 2022), suggest that both a combination of exercise (e.g., aerobic and strength exercise, or aerobic, strength and endurance) as well as dual task-based cognitive intervention are beneficial for post-stroke cognitive rehabilitation. This occurs since aerobic physical exercise affects global cognition through improved cardiovascular conditioning, while motor training is task specific in increasing brain neuroplasticity, affecting cognition (Netz, 2019). However, due attention must be paid to exercise intensity for relevant physiological changes to occur (Howard-Jones, 2014; MacKay-Lyons et al., 2020; Yang and Wang, 2021). Despite extensive literature showing the benefits of both dual task and combined exercise, there are studies that question the improvements attributed to dual task components, arguing that this type of training has limited transferability (Law et al., 2014; Embon-Magal et al., 2022). Others further claim that it is unclear whether Dual Task skills can be transferred into new Dual Task contexts and maintained over time (Joubert and Chainay, 2018; Lemke et al., 2018). Physical exercise, on the other hand presents evidence that its effects transfer to untrained contexts (Dhir et al., 2021; Kardys et al., 2022) given that the neural mechanisms responsible for the impact of physical training on cognition stem from neurogenesis (i.e., production of new neurons), angiogenesis (i.e., growth of new blood vessels from existing ones), synaptogenesis (i.e., formation of synapses between neurons), and the action of neurotrophins (proteins that support the survival, development, and functions of neurons) (Liu-Ambrose et al., 2008; van Praag, 2009; Joubert and Chainay, 2018; Dhir et al., 2021).

Importantly, our initial expectation that post-stroke physical activity would lead to greater cognitive gains compared to post-stroke dual-task cognitive walking was supported by our results after 12 weeks intervention. Clinicians and technical staff should be aware of the heightened benefits of aerobic exercise, not only for attention and concentration, but also for perception of difficulty and perceived exertion concerning the intervention per se in post-stroke cognitive rehabilitation. The benefits of dual-task cognitive walking are supplanted by simpler, more engaging aerobic exercise protocols.

5. Limitations

This work was not without limitations. First, we recruited patients with subacute to chronic ischemic stroke. This means that we obtained a heterogeneous sample of participants, including people in the process of cognitive and neurofunctional rehabilitation and people that have already stabilized concerning cognitive and functional processes. In the future, it is important to address this issue. Second, the use of the MMSE for cognitive screening has been considered outdated by some researchers, (e.g., Carnero-Pardo, 2014). However, we were limited by the instruments available at the participating institutions. In the future, more effective and inclusive short cognitive screening tests should be considered. Third, the fact that the intervention took place in parallel in two different places, combined with the trust placed in the evaluators (experienced health professionals) contributed to the non-evaluation of reliability in the terms defined by Hoffmann et al. (2014), as the degree to which an intervention occurred as intended. Finally, the comparison of two exercise modalities (PE vs. DT), without control, might also constitute a limitation. Specifically, to determine the strength of the aerobic physical exercise training, a control group with conventional physiotherapy would have been necessary. In the future, to better understand the efficiency of aerobic physical exercise, these aspects should be considered accordingly.

6. Conclusion

Cardiovascular skills through aerobic physical exercise reduce cognitive deficits, as cardiovascular capacity contributes to the improvement of cognitive performance. Aerobic physical exercise promotes angiogenesis in areas of the brain that were previously ischemic as a result of improved cardiovascular competence (Bliss et al., 2021; Zhou et al., 2021).

With this study we were able to show that the practice of aerobic physical exercise is associated with improvements in cognitive performance in patients with stroke. In addition, we showed that the perception of cognitive difficulty and physical exertion decrease with chronic aerobic physical exercise. Therefore, this study confirmed that the benefits for cognitive rehabilitation obtained through the practice of aerobic physical exercise seem to be relevant and can be recommended as a complementary rehabilitation to improve cognitive functioning in patients with stroke.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found at: https://osf.io/c2hd4/?view_only=4c2326f8714241beb8b7fc3b903e24ac.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Comissão de Ética para a Saúde (CES), UCP. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

RM: Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing – original draft. CS: Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. IF: Methodology, Writing – review & editing. AA: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by National Funds through FCT—Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia, I.P., under the project UIDP/04279/2020 to AMA.

Acknowledgments

We would like to acknowledge the Hospital Provincial de Inhambane and Hospital Rural de Chicuque for having facilitated use of its space and equipment for the development of this research. We would also like to thank the participants for their generous participation.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Aamot, I. L., Forbord, S. H., Karlsen, T., and Støylen, A. (2014). Does rating of perceived exertion result in target exercise intensity during interval training in cardiac rehabilitation? A study of the Borg scale versus a heart rate monitor. J. Sci. Med. Sport 17, 541–545. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2013.07.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abbiss, C. R., Peiffer, J. J., Meeusen, R., and Skorski, S. (2015). Role of ratings of perceived exertion during self-paced exercise: what are we actually measuring? Sports Med. 45, 1235–1243. doi: 10.1007/s40279-015-0344-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abd-Elfattah, H. M., Abdelazeim, F. H., and Elshennawy, S. (2015). Physical and cognitive consequences of fatigue: a review. J. Adv. Res. 6, 351–358. doi: 10.1016/j.jare.2015.01.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Acharya, A. S., Prakash, A., Saxena, P., and Nigam, A. (2013). Sampling: why and how of it. Indian J. Med. Special. 4, 330–333. doi: 10.7713/ijms.2013.0032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Agmon, M., Shochat, T., and Kizony, R. (2016). Sleep quality is associated with walking under dual-task, but not single-task performance. Gait Posture 49, 127–131. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.06.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahmed, U., Karimi, H., Amir, S., and Ahmed, A. (2021). Effects of intensive multiplanar trunk training coupled with dual-task exercises on balance, mobility, and fall risk in patients with stroke: a randomized controlled trial. J. Int. Med. Res. 49:030006052110594. doi: 10.1177/03000605211059413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrade, L. S., Kanitz, A. C., Häfele, M. S., Schaun, G. Z., Pinto, S. S., and Alberton, C. L. (2020). Relationship between oxygen uptake, heart rate, and perceived effort in an aquatic incremental test in older women. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17, 1–12. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17228324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arevalo-Rodriguez, I., Smailagic, N., Roqué-Figuls, M., Ciapponi, A., Sanchez-Perez, E., Giannakou, A., et al. (2021). Mini-mental state examination (MMSE) for the early detection of dementia in people with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2021:CD010783. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD010783.pub3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baek, C. Y., Chang, W. N., Park, B. Y., Lee, K. B., Kang, K. Y., and Choi, M. R. (2021a). Effects of dual-task gait treadmill training on gait ability, dual-task interference, and fall efficacy in people with stroke: a randomized controlled trial. Phys. Ther. 101, 1–10. doi: 10.1093/ptj/pzab067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baek, C. Y., Yoon, H. S., Kim, H. D., and Kang, K. Y. (2021b). The effect of the degree of dual-task interference on gait, dual-task cost, cognitive ability, balance, and fall efficacy in people with stroke: a cross-sectional study. Medicine 100:e26275. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000026275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bangsbo, J., Hansen, P. R., Dvorak, J., and Krustrup, P. (2015). Recreational football for disease prevention and treatment in untrained men: a narrative review examining cardiovascular health, lipid profile, body composition, muscle strength and functional capacity. Br. J. Sports Med. 49, 568–576. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2015-094781

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barha, C. K., Hsiung, G. Y. R., and Liu-Ambrose, T. (2019). The role of S100B in aerobic training efficacy in older adults with mild vascular cognitive impairment: secondary analysis of a randomized controlled trial. Neuroscience 410, 176–182. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.04.052

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bayot, M., Dujardin, K., Tard, C., Defebvre, L., Bonnet, C. T., Allart, E., et al. (2018). The interaction between cognition and motor control: a theoretical framework for dual-task interference effects on posture, gait initiation, gait and turning. Neurophysiol. Clin. 48, 361–375. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2018.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernhardt, J., Hayward, K. S., Kwakkel, G., Ward, N. S., Wolf, S. L., Borschmann, K., et al. (2017). Agreed definitions and a shared vision for new standards in stroke recovery research: the stroke recovery and rehabilitation roundtable taskforce. Int. J. Stroke 12, 444–450. doi: 10.1177/1747493017711816

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertolucci, P. H., Brucki, S. M. D., Campacci, S. R., and Juliano, Y. (1994). O Mini-Exame do Estado Mental em uma população geral. Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 52, 01–07. doi: 10.1590/S0004-282X1994000100001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Best, J. R., Eng, J. J., Davis, J. C., Hsiung, R., Hall, P. A., Middleton, L. E., et al. (2018). Study protocol for vitality: a proof-of-concept randomised controlled trial of exercise training or complex mental and social activities to promote cognition in adults with chronic stroke. BMJ Open 8, 1–10. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-021490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Billinger, S. A., Arena, R., Bernhardt, J., Eng, J. J., Franklin, B. A., Johnson, C. M., et al. (2014). Physical activity and exercise recommendations for stroke survivors: a statement for healthcare professionals from the American Heart Association/American Stroke Association. Stroke 45, 2532–2553. doi: 10.1161/STR.0000000000000022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Billinger, S. A., Boyne, P., Coughenour, E., Dunning, K., and Mattlage, A. (2015). Does aerobic exercise and the FITT principle fit into stroke recovery? Curr. Neurol. Neurosci. Rep. 15:519. doi: 10.1007/s11910-014-0519-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Billoir, E., Navratil, V., and Blaise, B. J. (2015). Sample size calculation in metabolic phenotyping studies. Brief. Bioinform. 16, 813–819. doi: 10.1093/bib/bbu052

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchet, S., Richards, C. L., Leblond, J., Olivier, C., and Maltais, D. B. (2016). Cardiorespiratory fitness and cognitive functioning following short-term interventions in chronic stroke survivors with cognitive impairment: a pilot study. Int. J. Rehabil. Res. 39, 153–159. doi: 10.1097/MRR.0000000000000161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bliss, E. S., Wong, R. H. X., Howe, P. R. C., and Mills, D. E. (2021). Benefits of exercise training on cerebrovascular and cognitive function in ageing. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 41, 447–470. doi: 10.1177/0271678X20957807

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blumen, H. M., Holtzer, R., Brown, L. L., Gazes, Y., and Verghese, J. (2014). Behavioral and neural correlates of imagined walking and walking-while-talking in the elderly. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 4090–4104. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bo, W., Lei, M., Tao, S., Jie, L. T., Qian, L., Lin, F. Q., et al. (2019). Effects of combined intervention of physical exercise and cognitive training on cognitive function in stroke survivors with vascular cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled trial. Clin. Rehabil. 33, 54–63. doi: 10.1177/0269215518791007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borg, G. A. V. (1982). Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 14, 377–381.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Borrione, P. (2014). Effects of physical activity in Parkinson’s disease: a new tool for rehabilitation. World J. Methodol. 4, 133–143. doi: 10.5662/wjm.v4.i3.133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boss, H. M., van Schaik, S. M., Witkamp, T. D., Geerlings, M. I., Weinstein, H. C., and van den Berg-Vos, R. M. (2017). Cardiorespiratory fitness, cognition and brain structure after TIA or minor ischemic stroke. Int. J. Stroke 12, 724–731. doi: 10.1177/1747493017702666

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brickenkamp, R. (2007). d2 Teste de Atenção. Hogrefe & Huber Publishers: Cambridge, MA, pp. 11–47.

Google Scholar

Brickenkamp, R., and Zilmer, E. (1998) d2–test of attention. Hogrefe & Huber Publishers: Göttingen.

Google Scholar

Brisswalter, J., Collardeau, M., and René, A. (2002). Effects of acute physical exercise characteristics on cognitive performance. Sports Med. 32, 555–566. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200232090-00002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broglio, K. (2018). Randomization in clinical trials: permuted blocks and stratification. JAMA 319, 2223–2224. doi: 10.1001/jama.2018.6360

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabral, L. L., Nakamura, F. Y., Stefanello, J. M. F., Pessoa, L. C. V., Smirmaul, B. P. C., and Pereira, G. (2020). Initial validity and reliability of the Portuguese Borg rating of perceived exertion 6-20 scale. Meas. Phys. Educ. Exerc. Sci. 24, 103–114. doi: 10.1080/1091367X.2019.1710709

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canli, S., and Ozyurda, F. (2020). A multi-modal exercise intervention that improves cognitive function and physical performance, elderly with mobility-related disability: a randomized controlled trial. J. Sports Med. Phys. Fitness 60, 1027–1033. doi: 10.23736/S0022-4707.20.10286-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carnero-Pardo, C. (2014). Should the Mini-mental state examination be retired? Neurología (English Edition) 29, 473–481. doi: 10.1016/j.nrleng.2013.07.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, J. S. Y., Liu, G., Liang, D., Deng, K., Wu, J., and Yan, J. H. (2018). Special issue–therapeutic benefits of physical activity for mood: a systematic review on the effects of exercise intensity, duration, and modality. J. Psychol. 153, 102–125. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2018.1470487

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, W. N., and Tsang, W. W. N. (2017). Effect of tai chi training on dual-tasking performance that involves stepping down among stroke survivors: a pilot study. Evid. Based Complement. Alternat. Med. 2017, 1–12. doi: 10.1155/2017/9134173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapman, S. B., Aslan, S., Spence, J. S., Defina, L. F., Keebler, M. W., Didehbani, N., et al. (2013). Shorter term aerobic exercise improves brain, cognition, and cardiovascular fitness in aging. Front. Aging Neurosci. 5, 1–9. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2013.00075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheval, B., Darrous, L., Choi, K. W., Klimentidis, Y. C., Raichlen, D. A., Alexander, G. E., et al. (2020). Physical activity and general cognitive functioning: a Mendelian randomization study. bioRxiv, 1–24. doi: 10.1101/2020.10.16.342675

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chin, L. M., Keyser, R. E., Dsurney, J., and Chan, L. (2015). Improved cognitive performance following aerobic exercise training in people with traumatic brain injury. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 96, 754–759. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2014.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciria, F. L., Román-Caballero, R., Vadillo, M. A., Holgado, D., Luque-Casado, A., Perakakis, P., et al. (2023). An umbrella review of randomized control trials on the effects of physical exercise on cognition. Nat. Hum. Behav. 7, 928–941. doi: 10.1038/s41562-023-01554-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Combourieu Donnezan, L., Perrot, A., Belleville, S., Bloch, F., and Kemoun, G. (2018). Effects of simultaneous aerobic and cognitive training on executive functions, cardiovascular fitness and functional abilities in older adults with mild cognitive impairment. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 15, 78–87. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2018.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Condello, G., Forte, R., Monteagudo, P., Ghinassi, B., di Baldassarre, A., Capranica, L., et al. (2019). Autonomic stress response and perceived effort jointly inform on dual tasking in aging. Brain Sci. 9:290. doi: 10.3390/brainsci9110290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cramer, S. C., Wolf, S. L., Saver, J. L., Johnston, K. C., Mocco, J., Lansberg, M. G., et al. (2021). The utility of domain-specific end points in acute stroke trials. Stroke 52, 1154–1161. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.120.031939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crozier, J., Roig, M., Eng, J. J., MacKay-Lyons, M., Fung, J., Ploughman, M., et al. (2018). High-intensity interval training after stroke: an opportunity to promote functional recovery, cardiovascular health, and neuroplasticity. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 32, 543–556. doi: 10.1177/1545968318766663

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cumming, T. B., Marshall, R. S., and Lazar, R. M. (2013). Stroke, cognitive deficits, and rehabilitation: still an incomplete picture. Int. J. Stroke 8, 38–45. doi: 10.1111/j.1747-4949.2012.00972.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Danks, K. A., Roos, M. A., McCoy, D., and Reisman, D. S. (2014). A step activity monitoring program improves real world walking activity post stroke. Disabil. Rehabil. 36, 2233–2236. doi: 10.3109/09638288.2014.903303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

da Silva-Sauer, L., Garcia, R. B., Ehrich de Moura, A., and Fernández-Calvo, B. (2022). Does the d2 Test of Attention only assess sustained attention? Evidence of working memory processes involved. Appl. Neuropsychol. Adult. 1–9. doi: 10.1080/23279095.2021.2023152

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delgado, D. A., Lambert, B. S., Boutris, N., McCulloch, P. C., Robbins, A. B., Moreno, M. R., et al. (2018). Validation of digital visual analog scale pain scoring with a traditional paper-based visual analog scale in adults. JAAOS 2:e088. doi: 10.5435/jaaosglobal-d-17-00088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desjardins-Crepeau, L., Berryman, N., Fraser, S., Vu, T. T. M., Kergoat, M. J., Li, K., et al. (2016). Effects of combined physical and cognitive training on fitness and neuropsychological outcomes in healthy older adults. Clin. Interv. Aging 11, 1287–1299. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S115711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dhir, S., Teo, W. P., Chamberlain, S. R., Tyler, K., Yücel, M., and Segrave, R. A. (2021). The effects of combined physical and cognitive training on inhibitory control: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 128, 735–748. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dompeyre, P., Fritel, X., Bader, G., Delmas, V., and Fauconnier, A. (2007). Bladder sensitivity testing using a visual analogue scale: comparative cystometric study on women. Neurourol. Urodyn. 26, 350–355. doi: 10.1002/nau.20301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Droby, A., Varangis, E., Habeck, C., Hausdorff, J. M., Stern, Y., Mirelman, A., et al. (2022). Effects of aging on cognitive and brain inter-network integration patterns underlying usual and dual-task gait performance. Front. Aging Neurosci. 14, 1–12. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.956744

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Embon-Magal, S., Krasovsky, T., Doron, I., Asraf, K., Haimov, I., Gil, E., et al. (2022). The effect of co-dependent (thinking in motion [TIM]) versus single-modality (CogniFit) interventions on cognition and gait among community-dwelling older adults with cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled study. BMC Geriatr. 22:720. doi: 10.1186/s12877-022-03403-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fatori, C. d. O., Leite, C. F., Souza, L. A. P. S., and Patrizzi, L. J. (2015). Dupla tarefa e mobilidade funcional de idosos ativos. Revista Brasileira de Geriatria e Gerontologia 18, 29–37. doi: 10.1590/1809-9823.2015.13180

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., and Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Biomed. Sci. 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandez-Gonzalo, R., Fernandez-Gonzalo, S., Turon, M., Prieto, C., Tesch, P. A., and García-Carreira Mdel, C. (2016). Muscle, functional and cognitive adaptations after flywheel resistance training in stroke patients: a pilot randomized controlled trial. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 13, 1–11. doi: 10.1186/s12984-016-0144-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuentes-García, J. P., Villafaina, S., Collado-Mateo, D., de la Vega, R., Olivares, P. R., and Clemente-Suárez, V. J. (2019). Differences between high vs. low performance chess players in heart rate variability during chess problems. Front. Psychol. 10, 1–9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallotta, M. C., Emerenziani, G. P., Franciosi, E., Meucci, M., Guidetti, L., and Baldari, C. (2015). Acute physical activity and delayed attention in primary school students. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 25, e331–e338. doi: 10.1111/sms.12310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaskin, L., and Thomas, J. (1995). Pulse oximetry and exercise. Physiotherapy 81, 254–261. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9406(05)66819-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilmore, P. E., and Spaulding, S. J. (2001). Motor control and motor learning: implications for treatment of individuals post stroke. Phys. Occup. Ther. Geriatr. 20, 1–15. doi: 10.1300/J148v20n01_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gregory, M. A., Boa Sorte Silva, N. C., Gill, D. P., McGowan, C. L., Liu-Ambrose, T., Shoemaker, J. K., et al. (2017). Combined dual-task gait training and aerobic exercise to improve cognition, mobility, and vascular health in community-dwelling older adults at risk for future cognitive decline 1. J. Alzheimers Dis. 57, 747–763. doi: 10.3233/JAD-161240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, X., Xue, Q., Zhao, J., Yang, Y., Yu, Y., Liu, D., et al. (2020). Clinical diagnostic and therapeutic guidelines of stroke neurorestoration (2020 China version). J. Neurorestoratol. 8, 241–251. doi: 10.26599/jnr.2020.9040026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, P., Zhang, W., Kang, L., Ma, Y., Fu, L., Jia, L., et al. (2017). Clinical evidence of exercise benefits for stroke. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 1000, 131–151. doi: 10.1007/978-981-10-4304-8_9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hars, M., Herrmann, F. R., Gold, G., Rizzoli, R., and Trombetti, A. (2014). Effect of music-based multitask training on cognition and mood in older adults. Age Ageing 43, 196–200. doi: 10.1093/ageing/aft163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, Y., Yang, L., Zhou, J., Yao, L., and Pang, M. Y. C. (2018). Dual-task training effects on motor and cognitive functional abilities in individuals with stroke: a systematic review. Clin. Rehabil. 32, 865–877. doi: 10.1177/0269215518758482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herold, F., Hamacher, D., Törpel, A., Goldschmidt, L., Müller, N. G., and Schega, L. (2020). Does squatting need attention?—a dual-task study on cognitive resources in resistance exercise. PLoS One 15, e0226431–e0226413. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0226431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffmann, T. C., Glasziou, P. P., Boutron, I., Milne, R., Perera, R., Moher, D., et al. (2014). Better reporting of interventions: template for intervention description and replication (TIDieR) checklist and guide. BMJ (Online) 348, 1–12. doi: 10.1136/bmj.g1687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hötting, K., and Röder, B. (2013). Beneficial effects of physical exercise on neuroplasticity and cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 2243–2257. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.04.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard-Jones, P. A. (2014). Neuroscience and education: myths and messages. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 817–824. doi: 10.1038/nrn3817

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ismail, A. K., Abdul Ghafar, M. A., Shamsuddin, N. S. A., Roslan, N. A., Kaharuddin, H., and Nik Muhamad, N. A. (2015). The assessment of acute pain in pre-hospital care using verbal numerical rating and visual analogue Scales. J. Emerg. Med. 49, 287–293. doi: 10.1016/j.jemermed.2015.02.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivers, N. M., Halperin, I. J., Barnsley, J., Grimshaw, J. M., Shah, B. R., Tu, K., et al. (2012). Allocation techniques for balance at baseline in cluster randomized trials: a methodological review. Trials 13, 1–9. doi: 10.1186/1745-6215-13-120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jin, H., Jiang, Y., Wei, Q., Chen, L., and Ma, G. (2013). Effects of aerobic cycling training on cardiovascular fitness and heart rate recovery in patients with chronic stroke. NeuroRehabilitation 32, 327–335. doi: 10.3233/NRE-130852

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, E. W. (2001). Visual analog scale (VAS). Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 80:717. doi: 10.1097/00002060-200110000-00001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, L., and Zenko, Z. (Eds.). (2021). Strategies to facilitate more pleasant exercise experiences. Essentials of exercise and sport psychology: An open access textbook. Society for Transparency, Openness, and Replication in Kinesiology, 242–270.

Google Scholar

Joubert, C., and Chainay, H. (2018). Aging brain: the effect of combined cognitive and physical training on cognition as compared to cognitive and physical training alone – a systematic review. Clin. Interv. Aging 13, 1267–1301. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S165399

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kardys, C., Küper, K., Getzmann, S., Falkenstein, M., and Voelcker-Rehage, C. (2022). A comparison of the effects of short-term physical and combined multi-modal training on cognitive functions. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19, 4–7. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19127506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, G. Y., Han, M. R., and Lee, H. G. (2014). Effect of dual-task rehabilitative training on cognitive and motor function of stroke patients. J. Phys. Ther. Sci. 26, 1–6. doi: 10.1589/jpts.26.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S. H., Kook, J. R., Kwon, M., Son, M. H., Ahn, S. D., and Kim, Y. H. (2014). The effects of laughter therapy on mood state and self-esteem in Cancer patients undergoing radiation therapy: a randomized controlled trial. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 21, 217–222. doi: 10.1089/acm.2014.0152

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y., Park, I., and Kang, M. (2013). Convergent validity of the international physical activity questionnaire (IPAQ): meta-analysis. Public Health Nutr. 16, 440–452. doi: 10.1017/S1368980012002996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J., and Yim, J. (2017). Effects of an exercise protocol for improving handgrip strength and walking speed on cognitive function in patients with chronic stroke. Med. Sci. Monit. 23, 5402–5409. doi: 10.12659/MSM.904723

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimura, D., Hosokawa, T., Ujikawa, T., and Ito, T. (2022). Effects of different exercise intensities on prefrontal activity during a dual task. Sci. Rep. 12, 13008–13010. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-17172-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koch, S., Tiozzo, E., Simonetto, M., Loewenstein, D., Wright, C. B., Dong, C., et al. (2020). Randomized trial of combined aerobic, resistance, and cognitive training to improve recovery from stroke: feasibility and safety. J. Am. Heart Assoc. 9:e015377. doi: 10.1161/JAHA.119.015377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Komiyama, T., Katayama, K., Sudo, M., Ishida, K., Higaki, Y., and Ando, S. (2017). Cognitive function during exercise under severe hypoxia. Sci. Rep. 7, 10000–10011. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-10332-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kounti, F., Bakoglidou, E., Agogiatou, C., Lombardo, N. B. E., Serper, L. L., and Tsolaki, M. (2011). RHEA,* a nonpharmacological cognitive training intervention in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Topics Geriatr. Rehabil. 27, 289–300. doi: 10.1097/TGR.0b013e31821e59a9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramer, S. F., Hung, S. H., and Brodtmann, A. (2019). The impact of physical activity before and after stroke on stroke risk and recovery: a narrative review. Curr. Neurol. Neurosci. Rep. 19:949. doi: 10.1007/s11910-019-0949-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakicevic, N., Gentile, A., Mehrabi, S., Cassar, S., Parker, K., Roklicer, R., et al. (2020). Make fitness fun: could novelty be the key determinant for physical activity adherence? Front. Psychol. 11, 1–5. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.577522

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lan, Y., Huang, Z., Jiang, Y., Zhou, X., Zhang, J., Zhang, D., et al. (2018). Strength exercise weakens aerobic exerciseinduced cognitive improvements in rats. PLoS One 13, 1–13. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0205562

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larner, A. J. (2020). Manual of screeners for dementia, manual of screeners for dementia. Springer Cham: New York

Google Scholar

Lauenroth, A., Ioannidis, A. E., and Teichmann, B. (2016). Influence of combined physical and cognitive training on cognition: a systematic review. BMC Geriatr. 16, 21–23. doi: 10.1186/s12877-016-0315-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Law, L. L. F., Barnett, F., Yau, M. K., and Gray, M. A. (2014). Effects of combined cognitive and exercise interventions on cognition in older adults with and without cognitive impairment: a systematic review. Ageing Res. Rev. 15, 61–75. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2014.02.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lea, J. W. D., O’Driscoll, J. M., Hulbert, S., Scales, J., and Wiles, J. D. (2022). Convergent validity of ratings of perceived exertion during resistance exercise in healthy participants: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med. Open 8:2. doi: 10.1186/s40798-021-00386-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemke, N. C., Werner, C., Wiloth, S., Oster, P., Bauer, J. M., and Hauer, K. (2018). Transferability and sustainability of motor-cognitive dual-task training in patients with dementia: a randomized controlled trial. Gerontology 65, 68–83. doi: 10.1159/000490852

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leone, C., Feys, P., Moumdjian, L., D’Amico, E., Zappia, M., and Patti, F. (2017). Cognitive-motor dual-task interference: a systematic review of neural correlates. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 75, 348–360. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lerdal, A., Bakken, L. N., Kouwenhoven, S. E., Pedersen, G., Kirkevold, M., Finset, A., et al. (2009). Poststroke fatigue-a review. J. Pain Symptom Manag. 38, 928–949. doi: 10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2009.04.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lestari, S., Mistivani, I., Rumende, C. M., and Kusumaningsih, W. (2017). Comparison between mini mental state examination (MMSE) and Montreal cognitive assessment Indonesian version (MoCA-Ina) as an early detection of cognitive impairments in post-stroke patients. J. Phys. Conf. Ser. 884:012153. doi: 10.1088/1742-6596/884/1/012153

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levin, O., Netz, Y., and Ziv, G. (2017). The beneficial effects of different types of exercise interventions on motor and cognitive functions in older age: a systematic review. Eur. Rev. Aging Phys. Act. 14, 1–23. doi: 10.1186/s11556-017-0189-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liao, Y. Y., Chen, I. H., Lin, Y. J., Chen, Y., and Hsu, W. C. (2019). ‘Effects of virtual reality-based physical and cognitive training on executive function and dual-task gait performance in older adults with mild cognitive impairment: a randomized control trial. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindemann-Matthies, P., Benkowitz, D., and Hellinger, F. (2021). Associations between the naturalness of window and interior classroom views, subjective well-being of primary school children and their performance in an attention and concentration test. Landsc. Urban Plan. 214:104146. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2021.104146

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y. C., Yang, Y. R., Tsai, Y. A., and Wang, R. Y. (2017). Cognitive and motor dual task gait training improve dual task gait performance after stroke – a randomized controlled pilot trial. Sci. Rep. 7, 1–8. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-04165-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu-Ambrose, T., Donaldson, M. G., Ahamed, Y., Graf, P., Cook, W. L., Close, J., et al. (2008). Otago home-based strength and balance retraining improves executive functioning in older fallers: a randomized controlled trial. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 56, 1821–1830. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01931.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu-Ambrose, T., Falck, R. S., Dao, E., Best, J. R., Davis, J. C., Bennett, K., et al. (2022). Effect of exercise training or complex mental and social activities on cognitive function in adults with chronic stroke: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Netw. Open 5:E2236510. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.36510

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loetscher, T., Potter, K. J., Wong, D., and das Nair, R., Cochrane Stroke Group (2019). Cognitive rehabilitation for attention deficits following stroke. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2019:2842. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD002842.pub3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loprinzi, P. D., Ponce, P., and Frith, E. (2018). Hypothesized mechanisms through which acute exercise influences episodic memory. Physiol. Int. 105, 285–297. doi: 10.1556/2060.105.2018.4.28

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Machan, T., and Krupps, K. (2021). The neuroplastic adaptation trident model: a suggested novel framework for ACL rehabilitation. Int. J. Sports Phys. Ther. 16, 896–910. doi: 10.26603/001c.23679

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacKay-Lyons, M., Billinger, S. A., Eng, J. J., Dromerick, A., Giacomantonio, N., Hafer-Macko, C., et al. (2020). Aerobic exercise recommendations to optimize Best practices in care after stroke: AEROBICS 2019 update. Phys. Ther. 100, 149–156. doi: 10.1093/ptj/pzz153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maeneja, R., Ferreira, I. S., Silva, C. R., and Abreu, A. M. (2022). Cognitive benefits of exercise: is there a time-of-day effect? Healthcare 10, 1–15. doi: 10.3390/healthcare10091766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mandolesi, L., Polverino, A., Montuori, S., Foti, F., Ferraioli, G., Sorrentino, P., et al. (2018). Effects of physical exercise on cognitive functioning and wellbeing: biological and psychological benefits. Front. Psychol. 9, 1–11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcos-Pardo, P. J., Martínez-Rodríguez, A., and Gil-Arias, A. (2018). Impact of a motivational resistance-training programme on adherence and body composition in the elderly. Sci. Rep. 8, 1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-19764-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marino, S. E., Meador, K. J., Loring, D. W., Okun, M. S., Fernandez, H. H., Fessler, A. J., et al. (2009). Subjective perception of cognition is related to mood and not performance. Epilepsy Behav. 14, 459–464. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2008.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marmeleira, J. (2013). An examination of the mechanisms underlying the effects of physical activity on brain and cognition: a review with implications for research. Eur. Rev. Aging Phys. Act. 10, 83–94. doi: 10.1007/s11556-012-0105-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marôco, J. (2018). Análise Estatística com o SPSS Statistics.: 7a edição. ReportNumber, Lda.

Google Scholar

Marston, K. J., Newton, M. J., Brown, B. M., Rainey-Smith, S. R., Bird, S., Martins, R. N., et al. (2017). Intense resistance exercise increases peripheral brain-derived neurotrophic factor. J. Sci. Med. Sport 20, 899–903. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2017.03.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayo, N. E., Anderson, S., Barclay, R., Cameron, J. I., Desrosiers, J., Eng, J. J., et al. (2015). Getting on with the rest of your life following stroke: a randomized trial of a complex intervention aimed at enhancing life participation post stroke. Clin. Rehabil. 29, 1198–1211. doi: 10.1177/0269215514565396

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mikkelsen, K., Stojanovska, L., Polenakovic, M., Bosevski, M., and Apostolopoulos, V. (2017). Exercise and mental health. Maturitas 106, 48–56. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2017.09.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milot, M. H., Léonard, G., Corriveau, H., and Desrosiers, J. (2019). Using the borg rating of perceived exertion scale to grade the intensity of a functional training program of the affected upper limb after a stroke: a feasibility study. Clin. Interv. Aging 14, 9–16. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S179691

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohammed, A. M., Hassanein, A. A., and Mohammed, H. G. (2019). Effects of dual task exercises training program on stroke patients Asmaa. J. Nurs. Sci., 69–87. doi: 10.21608/JNSBU.2021.186431

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molloy, D. W., and Standish, T. I. M. (1997). A guide to the standardized mini-mental state examination. Int. Psychogeriatr. 9, 87–94. doi: 10.1017/S1041610297004754

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, R. D., Romine, M. W., O'connor, P. J., and Tomporowski, P. D. (2012). The influence of exercise-induced fatigue on cognitive function. J. Sports Sci. 30, 841–850. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2012.675083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moriarty, T. A., Mermier, C., Kravitz, L., Gibson, A., Beltz, N., and Zuhl, M. (2019). Acute aerobic exercise based cognitive and motor priming: practical applications and mechanisms. Front. Psychol. 10, 1–8. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02790

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Netz, Y. (2019). Is there a preferred mode of exercise for cognition enhancement in older age?—a narrative review. Front. Med. 6, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fmed.2019.00057

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Novak, J. R., and Ellis, F. K. (2021). A framework for incorporating physical activity in treatment: competencies, guidelines, and implications for family therapists. J. Marital. Fam. Ther. 48, 523–542. doi: 10.1111/jmft.12550

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ojaghihaghighi, S., Vahdati, S. S., Mikaeilpour, A., and Ramouz, A. (2017). Comparison of neurological clinical manifestation in patients with hemorrhagic and ischemic stroke. World J Emerg Med 8, 34–38. doi: 10.5847/wjem.j.1920-8642.2017.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ouwehand, K., Kroef, A., Wong, J., and Paas, F. (2021). Measuring cognitive load: are there more valid alternatives to likert rating scales? Front. Educ. 6, 1–13. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2021.702616

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozdogar, A. T., Ertekin, O., Kahraman, T., Yigit, P., and Ozakbas, S. (2020). Effect of video-based exergaming on arm and cognitive function in persons with multiple sclerosis: a randomized controlled trial. Mult. Scler. Relat. Disord. 40:101966. doi: 10.1016/j.msard.2020.101966

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozyemisci-Taskiran, O., Batur, E. B., Yuksel, S., Cengiz, M., and Karatas, G. K. (2019). Validity and reliability of fatigue severity scale in stroke. Top. Stroke Rehabil. 26, 122–127. doi: 10.1080/10749357.2018.1550957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paddick, S.-M., Dotchin, C., Gray, W. K., Kisoli, A., Kisima, J., Chaote, P., et al. (2015). Utility of the mini-mental state examination (MMSE) for identification of dementia in a low-literacy setting in rural Tanzania. Age Ageing 44, ii25.2–ii25. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afv116.02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paddick, S. M., Gray, W. K., McGuire, J., Richardson, J., Dotchin, C., and Walker, R. W. (2017). Cognitive screening tools for identification of dementia in illiterate and low-educated older adults, a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. Psychogeriatr. 29, 897–929. doi: 10.1017/S1041610216001976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Page, S. J., Gater, D. R., and Bach-Y-Rita, P. (2004). Reconsidering the motor recovery plateau in stroke rehabilitation. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 85, 1377–1381. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2003.12.031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pageaux, B. (2016). Perception of effort in exercise science: definition, measurement and perspectives. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 16, 885–894. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2016.1188992

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pageaux, B., and Lepers, R. (2018) The effects of mental fatigue on sport-related performance. 1st edn, Progress in brain research. 1st Edn. Amsterdam: Elsevier B.V.

Google Scholar

Pang, M. Y. C., Charlesworth, S. A., Lau, R. W. K., and Chung, R. C. K. (2013). Using aerobic exercise to improve health outcomes and quality of life in stroke: evidence-based exercise prescription recommendations. Cerebrovasc. Dis. 35, 7–22. doi: 10.1159/000346075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parfitt, G., Evans, H., and Eston, R. (2012). Perceptually regulated training at RPE13 is pleasant and improves physical health. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 44, 1613–1618. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e31824d266e

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, M. O., and Lee, S. H. (2019). Effect of a dual-task program with different cognitive tasks applied to stroke patients: a pilot randomized controlled trial. NeuroRehabilitation 44, 239–249. doi: 10.3233/NRE-182563

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penati, R., Schieppati, M., and Nardone, A. (2019). Cognitive performance during gait is worsened by overground but enhanced by treadmill walking. Gait Posture 76, 182–187. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2019.12.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, C., Baktir, M. A., Srivatsan, M., and Salehi, A. (2014). Neuroprotective effects of physical activity on the brain: a closer look at trophic factor signaling. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 8, 1–16. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2014.00170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ploughman, M., and Kelly, L. P. (2016). Four birds with one stone? Reparative, neuroplastic, cardiorespiratory, and metabolic benefits of aerobic exercise poststroke. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 29, 684–692. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0000000000000383

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posadzki, P., Pieper, D., Bajpai, R., Makaruk, H., Könsgen, N., Neuhaus, A. L., et al. (2020). Exercise/physical activity and health outcomes: an overview of Cochrane systematic reviews. BMC Public Health 20, 1724–1712. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-09855-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reigal, R. E., Moral-Campillo, L., Mier, R., Morillo-Baro, J. P., Morales-Sánchez, V., Pastrana, J. L., et al. (2020). Physical fitness level is related to attention and concentration in adolescents. Front. Psychol. 11, 1–9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romanov, R., Mesarič, L., Perić, D., Vešligaj Damiš, J., and Petrova Filišič, Y. (2021). The effects of adapted physical exercise during rehabilitation in patients with traumatic brain injury. Turkish J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 67, 482–489. doi: 10.5606/TFTRD.2021.6145

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rooks, C. R., Thom, N. J., McCully, K. K., and Dishman, R. K. (2010). Effects of incremental exercise on cerebral oxygenation measured by near-infrared spectroscopy: a systematic review. Prog. Neurobiol. 92, 134–150. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2010.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sacco, R. L., Kasner, S. E., Broderick, J. P., Caplan, L. R., Connors, J. J., Culebras, A., et al. (2013). An updated definition of stroke for the 21st century: a statement for healthcare professionals from the American heart association/American stroke association. Stroke 44, 2064–2089. doi: 10.1161/STR.0b013e318296aeca

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sánchez González, J. L., Calvo Arenillas, J. I., and Sánchez Rodríguez, J. L. (2018). Efectos del ejercicio físico moderado sobre la cognición en adultos mayores de 60 años. Rev. Neurol. 66:230. doi: 10.33588/rn.6607.2017449

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sánchez-González, J. L., Sánchez-Rodríguez, J. L., Martín-Vallejo, J., Martel-Martel, A., and González-Sarmiento, R. (2021). Effects of physical exercise on cognition and telomere length in healthy older women. Brain Sci. 11:1417. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11111417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasaki, A. C., Pinto, E. B., Mendel, T., Katia, N., Oliveira-Filho, J., D'Oliveira, A., et al. (2015). Association between dual-task performance and balance during gait in community-dwelling elderly people after stroke. Healthy Aging Res. 4, 1–7. doi: 10.12715/har.2015.4.29

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saunders, D. H., Greig, C. A., and Mead, G. E. (2014). Physical activity and exercise after stroke: review of multiple meaningful benefits. Stroke 45, 3742–3747. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.114.004311

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sawalla Guseh, J., Lieberman, D., and Baggish, A. (2022). The evidence for exercise in medicine — a new review series. NEJM Evid. 1, 1–8. doi: 10.1056/evidra2100002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schättin, A., Häfliger, S., Meyer, A., Früh, B., Böckler, S., Hungerbühler, Y., et al. (2021). Design and evaluation of user-centered exergames for patients with multiple sclerosis: multilevel usability and feasibility studies. JMIR Serious Games 9:e22826. doi: 10.2196/22826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schüller, I., and Demetriou, Y. (2018). Physical activity interventions promoting social competence at school: a systematic review. Educ. Res. Rev. 25, 39–55. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2018.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulz, K. F., Altman, D. G., and Moher, D. (2011). CONSORT 2010 statement: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. Int. J. Surg. 9, 672–677. doi: 10.1016/j.ijsu.2011.09.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shahar, N., Schwartz, I., and Portnoy, S. (2019). Differences in muscle activity and fatigue of the upper limb between task-specific training and robot assisted training among individuals post stroke. J. Biomech. 89, 28–33. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2019.04.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siennicka, A., Quintana, D. S., Fedurek, P., Wijata, A., Paleczny, B., Ponikowska, B., et al. (2019). Resting heart rate variability, attention and attention maintenance in young adults. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 143, 126–131. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.06.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, M. R., Chai, R., Nguyen, H. T., Marcora, S. M., and Coutts, A. J. (2019). Comparing the effects of three cognitive tasks on indicators of mental fatigue. J. Psychol. 153, 759–783. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2019.1611530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smulders, K., van Swigchem, R., de Swart, B. J. M., Geurts, A. C. H., and Weerdesteyn, V. (2012). Community-dwelling people with chronic stroke need disproportionate attention while walking and negotiating obstacles. Gait Posture 36, 127–132. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spitzer, U. S., and Hollmann, W. (2013). Experimental observations of the effects of physical exercise on attention, academic and prosocial performance in school settings. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.tine.2013.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spring, C. (2016). An investigation of the association between perceived exertion and heart rate during aerobic training in individuals post-stroke. Available at: https://scholar.archive.org/work/ylq4youztrdkzpv2xcgbyft5ry/access/wayback/https://s3-eu-west-1.amazonaws.com/pstorage-rcsi-9048708668/19320584/AnInvestigationOfTheAssociationBetweenPerceivedExertionAn.pdf (Accessed May 17, 2023).

Google Scholar

Steinborn, M. B., Langner, R., Flehmig, H. C., Huestegge, L., et al. (2018). Methodology of performance scoring in the d2 sustained-attention test: cumulative-reliability functions and practical guidelines. Psychol. Assess. 30, 339–357. doi: 10.1037/pas0000482

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stinear, C. M., Lang, C. E., Zeiler, S., and Byblow, W. D. (2020). Advances and challenges in stroke rehabilitation. Lancet Neurol. 19, 348–360. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(19)30415-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strobach, T., and Schubert, T. (2017). Mechanisms of practice-related reductions of dual-task interference with simple tasks: data and theory. Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 13, 28–41. doi: 10.5709/acp-0204-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Timmermans, C., Roerdink, M., Janssen, T. W. J., Meskers, C. G. M., and Beek, P. J. (2018). Dual-task walking in challenging environments in people with stroke: cognitive-motor interference and task prioritization. Stroke Res. Treat. 2018, 1–8. doi: 10.1155/2018/7928597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomporowski, P. D., and Qazi, A. S. (2020). Cognitive-motor dual task interference effects on declarative memory: a theory-based review. Front. Psychol. 11, 1–13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsang, C. S. L., Wang, S., Miller, T., and Pang, M. Y. C. (2022). Degree and pattern of dual-task interference during walking vary with component tasks in people after stroke: a systematic review. J. Physiother. 68, 26–36. doi: 10.1016/j.jphys.2021.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tseng, B. Y., Gajewski, B. J., and Kluding, P. M. (2010). Reliability, responsiveness, and validity of the visual analog fatigue scale to measure exertion fatigue in people with chronic stroke: a preliminary study. Stroke Res. Treat. 2010, 10–13. doi: 10.4061/2010/412964

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Unibaso-Markaida, I., Iraurgi, I., Ortiz-Marqués, N., Amayra, I., and Martínez-Rodríguez, S. (2019). Effect of the Wii sports resort on the improvement in attention, processing speed and working memory in moderate stroke. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 16, 32–38. doi: 10.1186/s12984-019-0500-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Praag, H. (2009). Exercise and the brain: something to chew on. Trends Neurosci. 32, 283–290. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.12.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanhelst, J., Béghin, L., Duhamel, A., Manios, Y., Molnar, D., de Henauw, S., et al. (2016). Physical activity is associated with attention capacity in adolescents. J. Pediatr. 168, 126–131.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2015.09.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vetter, T. R., and Mascha, E. J. (2017). Defining the primary outcomes and justifying secondary outcomes of a study: usually, the fewer, the better. Anesth. Analg. 125, 678–681. doi: 10.1213/ANE.0000000000002224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, N. (2017). The Borg rating of perceived exertion (RPE) scale. Occup. Med. 67, 404–405. doi: 10.1093/occmed/kqx063

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, L. (2021). Dual-task effects of concurrently coupling aerobic exercise with virtual navigation. Available at: https://uwspace.uwaterloo.ca/handle/10012/16838 (Accessed May 17, 2023).

Google Scholar

Woo, J., Rajagopalan, P., and Andamon, M. M. (2022). An evaluation of measured indoor conditions and student performance using d2 test of attention. Build. Environ. 214:108940. doi: 10.1016/j.buildenv.2022.108940

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Medical Association (2013). ‘Declaration of Helsinki ethical prin_ciples for medical research involving human subjects’, world medical association declaration of Helsinki ethical prin_ciples for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 310, 175–189. doi: 10.4324/9781315572093-15

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Worringer, B., Langner, R., Koch, I., Eickhoff, S. B., Eickhoff, C. R., and Binkofski, F. C. (2019). Common and distinct neural correlates of dual-tasking and task-switching: a meta-analytic review and a neuro-cognitive processing model of human multitasking. Brain Struct. Funct. 224, 1845–1869. doi: 10.1007/s00429-019-01870-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wulf, G., and Lewthwaite, R. (2016). Optimizing performance through intrinsic motivation and attention for learning: the OPTIMAL theory of motor learning. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 23, 1382–1414. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0999-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, B., and Wang, S. (2021). Meta-analysis on cognitive benefit of exercise after stroke. Complexity 2021, 1–12. doi: 10.1155/2021/5569346

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, Z.-Q., Wei, M. F., Chen, L., and Xi, J. N. (2022). Research progress in the application of motor-cognitive dual-task training in rehabilitation of walking function in stroke patients. J. Neurorestoratol. 11:100028. doi: 10.1016/j.jnrt.2022.100028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeh, T. T., Chang, K. C., and Wu, C. Y. (2019). The active ingredient of cognitive restoration: a multicenter randomized controlled trial of sequential combination of aerobic exercise and computer-based cognitive training in stroke survivors with cognitive decline. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 100, 821–827. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2018.12.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeh, T. T., Chang, K. C., Wu, C. Y., Chen, C. J., and Chuang, I. C. (2021). Clinical efficacy of aerobic exercise combined with computer-based cognitive training in stroke: a multicenter randomized controlled trial. Top. Stroke Rehabil. 29, 255–264. doi: 10.1080/10749357.2021.1922045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeung, R. R. (1996). The acute effects of exercise on mood state. Physiother. Theory Pract. 40, 123–141.

Google Scholar

Yokoyama, H., Okazaki, K., Imai, D., Yamashina, Y., Takeda, R., Naghavi, N., et al. (2015). The effect of cognitive-motor dual-task training on cognitive function and plasma amyloid β peptide 42/40 ratio in healthy elderly persons: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Geriatr. 15, 1–10. doi: 10.1186/s12877-015-0058-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, F., Lin, F. V., Salisbury, D. L., Shah, K. N., Chow, L., Vock, D., et al. (2018). Efficacy and mechanisms of combined aerobic exercise and cognitive training in mild cognitive impairment: study protocol of the ACT trial. Trials 19, 1–13. doi: 10.1186/s13063-018-3054-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeibig, J. M., Seiffer, B., Frei, A. K., Takano, K., Sudeck, G., Rösel, I., et al. (2023). Long-term efficacy of exercise across diagnostically heterogenous mental disorders and the mediating role of affect regulation skills. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 64:102340. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2022.102340

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, G., Zheng, Y., Xiong, Z., and Ye, B. (2020). Effect of Baduanjin exercise on cognitive function in patients with post-stroke cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled trial. Clin. Rehabil. 34, 1028–1039. doi: 10.1177/0269215520930256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, G., Zhou, W., Xia, R., Tao, J., and Chen, L. (2016). Aerobic exercises for cognition rehabilitation following stroke: a systematic review. J. Stroke Cerebrovasc. Dis. 25, 2780–2789. doi: 10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2016.07.035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, Q., Yang, H., Zhou, Q., and Pan, H. (2021). Effects of cognitive motor dual-task training on stroke patients: a RCT-based meta-analysis. J. Clin. Neurosci. 92, 175–182. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2021.08.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: stroke, exercise, cognition, cognitive-gait, dual-task

Citation: Maeneja R, Silva CR, Ferreira IS and Abreu AM (2023) Aerobic physical exercise versus dual-task cognitive walking in cognitive rehabilitation of people with stroke: a randomized clinical trial. Front. Psychol. 14:1258262. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1258262

Received: 13 July 2023; Accepted: 26 September 2023;
Published: 13 October 2023.

Edited by:

Maria Elide Vanutelli, University of Milan, Italy

Reviewed by:

Juan Luis Sánchez González, University of Salamanca, Spain
Ruud Knols, University Hospital Zürich, Switzerland

Copyright © 2023 Maeneja, Silva, Ferreira and Abreu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ana Maria Abreu, anamariablom@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.