Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 02 September 2022
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic Uncovering the Neural Correlates of Prism Adaptation: Evidence from the Brain Network Approach. View all 7 articles

Effective connectivity underlying neural and behavioral components of prism adaptation

  • 1Behavioral Neurology Unit, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, Bethesda, MD, United States
  • 2Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, The George Washington University, Washington, DC, United States
  • 3Center for Mind/Brain Sciences-CIMeC, University of Trento, Rovereto, Trentino, Italy
  • 4Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD, United States
  • 5Department of Kinesiology and Health Education, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, United States

Prism adaptation (PA) is a form of visuomotor training that produces both sensorimotor and cognitive aftereffects depending on the direction of the visual displacement. Recently, a neural framework explaining both types of PA-induced aftereffects has been proposed, but direct evidence for it is lacking. We employed Structural Equation Modeling (SEM), a form of effective connectivity analysis, to establish directionality among connected nodes of the brain network thought to subserve PA. The findings reveal two distinct network branches: (1) a loop involving connections from the parietal cortices to the right parahippocampal gyrus, and (2) a branch linking the lateral premotor cortex to the parahippocampal gyrus via the cerebellum. Like the sensorimotor aftereffects, the first branch exhibited qualitatively different modulations for left versus right PA, and critically, changes in these connections were correlated with the magnitude of the sensorimotor aftereffects. Like the cognitive aftereffects, changes in the second branch were qualitatively similar for left and right PA, with greater change for left PA and a trend correlation with cognitive aftereffects. These results provide direct evidence that PA is supported by two functionally distinct subnetworks, a parietal–temporal network responsible for sensorimotor aftereffects and a fronto-cerebellar network responsible for cognitive aftereffects.

Introduction

Prism adaptation (PA) is a classic technique to reversibly modify sensorimotor mapping (Helmholtz, 1867). By practicing pointing movements to visual targets that are laterally shifted, PA modifies sensorimotor coordinates and also alters higher-level cognition in both healthy individuals (Frassinetti et al., 2009; Bultitude and Woods, 2010; Schintu et al., 2018) and patients with hemispatial neglect (McIntosh et al., 2002; Bultitude and Woods, 2010; Jacquin-Courtois et al., 2010). Adaptation to right-shifting prisms (right PA) reduces the pathological rightward bias exhibited by neglect patients (Rossetti et al., 1998), and has become a promising tool for improving negelct symptoms (Luaute et al., 2006; Newport and Schenk, 2012). Adaptation to left-shifting prisms (left PA), by reducing the inherent leftward bias exhibited by healthy individuals called pseudoneglect (Jewell and McCourt, 2000), it causes a visuospatial bias, similar in direction but not in magnitude, to the pathological neglect’s one (Colent et al., 2000; Schintu et al., 2014, 2017; Michel, 2016) and has been extensively employed to model neglect like cognition.

The mechanism of PA has been widely investigated for the last 20 years. Neuroimaging studies have identified the parietal cortex and the cerebellum as key regions (Danckert et al., 2008; Chapman et al., 2010; Küper et al., 2014) but also showed that PA modulates fronto-parietal connectivity (Saj et al., 2013; Schintu et al., 2016; Gudmundsson et al., 2020) and, more generally, that it affects parietal, temporal, and frontal regions associated with spatial attention and awareness (Crottaz-Herbette et al., 2014, 2017; Tsujimoto et al., 2018; Schintu et al., 2020b) and that it even enhances the decoupling between networks such as the default mode and the attention networks (Wilf et al., 2019). However, how sensorimotor adaptation to laterally displaced vision leads to cognitive spatial changes is still unclear.

Recently, Panico et al. (2020), based on published neuroimaging and non-invasive brain stimulation studies, put forward a possible framework that would account for the two types of prism-induced aftereffects: the sensorimotor aftereffect usually quantified via pointing movements toward a target (Rossetti et al., 1998) and cognitive, aftereffects measured via cognitive tasks, such as midline judgment (Colent et al., 2000; Schintu et al., 2014). They proposed that the sensorimotor aftereffect relies on a cerebello-parietal network, whereas the cognitive effects are mediated by bottom-up activation of prefrontal and temporal regions, and that their consolidation involves the motor cortex. However, direct evidence of this hypothesis is lacking.

With the aim of defining the networks underlying PA-induced aftereffects, we used structural equation modeling (SEM; McIntosh and Gonzalez-Lima, 1994; Price et al., 2009; Chen et al., 2011; Reid et al., 2019), a form of effective connectivity analysis (Friston, 1994, 2011; Smith et al., 2012) to uncover connectivity directionality and causality among a set of nodes. We applied SEM on a network we identified in a previous study where we investigated changes in functional connectivity following right and left PA in healthy individuals (Schintu et al., 2020b). In this study, we used a seed-based analysis and found differences in resting-state functional connectivity between regions connected to the posterior parietal cortices (PPC): left cerebellar declive, right parahippocampal gyrus (PHG), and frontal areas such as the right lateral premotor cortex (PMC). While this study revealed differences in resting state functional connectivity between the two groups of healthy individuals adapted to left or right prisms, this type of analysis can only establish patterns of statistical covariation among a set of regions. Effective connectivity, in contrast, attempts to establish networks of causal, directional influences among a set of brain regions. Very little can be said about the nature of the underlying network interactions at the level of functional connectivity alone, as underlying sources/causes can be blurred across multiple regions; to understand the network interactions at a deeper level, effective connectivity estimation is required (e.g., Friston, 2011; Reid et al., 2019).

Here, we set out to model effective connectivity between areas identified in the contrast between left and right PA, along with the PPC seeds for that whole-brain analysis. Based on the recent framework proposed by Panico et al. (2020), we expected to identify separate networks or branches of the model associated with the sensorimotor and cognitive aftereffects.

Materials and methods

Participants

Forty right-handed (Edinburgh Inventory; Oldfield, 1971) and right-eye dominant (hole-in-card test; Miles, 1930) healthy adults participated in the original study (Schintu et al., 2020b), approved by the National Institutes of Health, Central Nervous System Institutional Review Board and conducted in accordance with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration (World Medical Association, 2013). Participants were compensated for participation and gave written informed consent. Twenty participants underwent left PA and twenty right PA. After data collection, one participant was excluded because of excessive motion during scans (average motion >0.2 cm) and one because of a congenital cerebral cyst. After effective connectivity analysis criteria were satisfied (see effective connectivity analysis paragraph), the statistical analysis included 36 participants: left PA group (N = 18; 11 females; age = 25.9 ± 0.9 SEM) and right PA group (N = 18; 12 females; age = 25.7 ± 1.1 SEM). The left and right PA groups did not differ in age (t(34) = 0.131, p = 0.896). Power calculations for the necessary sample size were derived from a previously published study (Albert et al., 2009) of changes in resting state functional connectivity (pre-to post-training) in a fronto-parietal network with motor learning. The effect size (Cohen’s d) of the training-related changes by group in fronto-parietal connectivity was estimated to be 0.9, suggesting that the needed total sample size (two groups combined) to detect an effect of p < 0.05 with 80% power was N = 39.5 or approximately 20 participants per group.

Procedures

As reported in Schintu et al. (2020b) the study consisted of two sessions of behavioral testing and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), one before and one after PA (Figure 1). Prior to each session (baseline) spatial attention was quantified with the perceptual and manual line bisection tasks. In the pre-adaptation session participants underwent the first neuroimaging session, consisting of a resting-state scan (two runs of 5 min) and a population receptive field scan (30 min, reported in Schintu et al., 2022), and the first behavioral assessment consisting of perceptual and manual line bisection tasks, along with two other tasks assessing proprioceptive (straight-ahead pointing task) and visuomotor (open-loop pointing task) performance. Participants were then adapted to left or right-shifting prisms. In the post-adaptation session proprioceptive and visuomotor performance were assessed with the straight-ahead and open-loop pointing tasks immediately after PA (early post-adaptation assessment), then resting-state and population receptive field scans were run, followed by the administration of the perceptual and manual line bisection along with another repeat of the straight-ahead and open-loop pointing tasks (late post-adaptation assessment; Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental design. PLB, perceptual line bisection; MLB, manual line bisection; SA, straight-ahead pointing; OL, open-loop pointing; PA, prism adaptation. Adapted from Schintu et al. (2020b).

During the behavioral measurement and PA participants seated in front of a horizontal board with their heads supported by a chinrest. See Schintu et al. (2020b) for details.

Prism adaptation

During PA, participants were fitted with prism goggles deviating the visual field either 15° left or right and performed, with their right hand, 150 pointing movements to the right and left targets, positioned at −10° (left) and + 10° (right) from the participant’s midline. See Schintu et al. (2020b) for details.

Behavioral assessment

As described in detail in Schintu et al. (2020b), we employed four different tasks quantifying PA-induced aftereffects.

The perceptual line bisection task, a modified version of the Landmark task (Milner et al., 1992), was used to measure the visuospatial bias’ perceptual component. Participants judged a series of 66 pre-bisected lines appearing on a computer screen placed in front of them. They were asked to judge whether the mark (transector) was closer to the left or right end of the line. For each participant, the percentage of “right” responses was plotted as a function of the position of the transector. These data were then fitted with a sigmoid function and the value on the x-axis corresponding to the point at which the participant responded “right” 50% of the time was taken as the point of subjective equality (PSE).

The manual line bisection task (Schenkenberg et al., 1980) was employed to measure the motor component of the visuospatial bias. Participants judged a series of 10 lines printed on sheets of paper placed on a computer screen in front of them. They were asked to draw a vertical mark where they thought the center of the line was. For each of the ten lines the distance between the mark placed by the participant and the line true center was calculated. The PSE was calculated as the average distance between the true center and the mark draw by the participant, with marks to the right of center coded as positive and to the left as negative.

The straight-ahead pointing task was used to measure proprioceptive bias (Rossetti et al., 1998). Participants were asked to point, six times, to their perceived midline with the right index finger at a comfortable and uniform speed while keeping their eyes closed. The proprioceptive bias was measured as the average distance between the landing position and the true midline, with an accuracy of +/− 0.5 cm.

The open-loop pointing task was employed to measure the visuomotor bias (similar to Rossetti et al., 1998). Participants were asked to point, six times, to the central target (0 cm) with their right index finger. Vison of the pointing movement and the landing position was occluded. Visuomotor bias was measured as the average distance between the landing position and the central target with an accuracy of +/− 0.5 cm.

fMRI

Functional and structural MRI data were acquired with a 32-channel head coil on a research-dedicated 3-Tesla Siemens MAGNETOM Prisma MR scanner in the NINDS functional MRI Facility. For each participant, a whole-brain T1-weighted anatomical image (MPRAGE) was obtained, along with a T2* blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) resting-state scans. See Schintu et al. (2020b) for details. During resting-state scans, participants were instructed to relax and to look at a cross appearing on a computer screen while thinking about nothing.

MRI preprocessing

As reported in Schintu et al. (2020b), functional and structural MRI data were preprocessed using the AFNI software package (Cox, 1996) and following a general preprocessing approach (Wang et al., 2014). The anatomical scans were segmented using Freesurfer (Dale et al., 1999). Two initial volumes were removed from each resting-state run. Volumes were then despiked (3dDespike), slice-time corrected, co-registered to the anatomical scan, transformed to TT_N27 Template space (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988), resampled to 2 mm isotropic voxels, smoothed with an isometric 4-mm full-width half-maximum Gaussian kernel, and scaled to percentage signal change. TRs with head movement >0.3 mm were censored, and simultaneously band-pass filtered (3dTproject) from 0.01 to 0.1 Hz. Six motion parameters and their derivatives were regressed, and were also filtered (0.01 to 0.1 Hz) prior to performing the nuisance regression (e.g., Hallquist et al., 2013; Jo et al., 2013). Measures of mean framewise displacement (@1dDiffMag) and the average voxelwise signal amplitude (standard deviation) were calculated for use as nuisance covariates in group-level analyses (Wang et al., 2014; Gotts et al., 2020).

Functional connectivity analysis

The functional connectivity analysis in Schintu et al. (2020b) was a seed-to-whole-brain analysis, with the seeds being the right and left intraparietal sulcus (IPS) regions 1 and 2 combined. These regions of interest (ROI) were chosen because have been shown to affect visuospatial performance when perturbed by TMS (Szczepanski and Kastner, 2013) and were identified using a probabilistic atlas (Wang et al., 2015). The seed-based group analysis identified several additional regions of interest with which they exhibited a significant Phase (pre PA, post PA) x Group (left PA, right PA) interaction. However, since the goal of the current study was to examine effective connectivity models underlying these relationships, we focused the analyses on the largest clusters from the original whole brain analysis reported in Schintu et al. (2020b) that survived both cluster-size correction over a range of voxelwise statistical thresholds and FDR-correction and had a minimum cluster size of 30 voxels (q < 0. 005, p = 0.00033). The time series data in these ROIs, along with the behavioral data, were those previously reported in Schintu et al. (2020b): the left cerebellar declive, right parahippocampal gyrus (PHG), right lateral premotor cortex (PMC), and the left and right IPS seeds used to detect these regions (the ROI mask used is publicly available on FigShare.com: https://figshare.com/articles/dataset/Frontiers_ROI_mask_N_5_ROIs_/20332218/1). Here, the functional connectivity analysis, unlike the original, examined all possible ROI-ROI connections and provided the experimental data for the effective connectivity modeling. Time series were averaged across voxels for each ROI, and the Pearson’s correlation was calculated among all possible pairs of the five ROIs, followed by the Fisher z-transformation. Only participants satisfying the inclusion criteria for effective connectivity analysis (see effective connectivity analysis paragraph) were included in the functional connectivity analysis. This was conducted using linear mixed-effects models (3dLME), with each pairwise connection’s Fisher z’-transformed Pearson’s correlation serving as the dependent variable, and Group (left PA, right PA), Phase (pre PA, post PA), and their interaction serving as fixed effects, and subjects a random effect. Mean framewise displacement and average voxelwise signal amplitude did not differ between left and right PA groups either pre or post PA (all p > 0.14). A false discovery rate (FDR) at q < 0.05 was used to correct for multiple comparisons.

Effective connectivity analysis

The five ROIs identified in the whole-brain functional connectivity analyses were submitted to SEM (McIntosh and Gonzalez-Lima, 1994; Price et al., 2009; Chen et al., 2011), a form of effective connectivity analysis (Friston, 1994, 2011; Smith et al., 2012) which detects connectivity directionality and causality. We first performed a search for the model that accounted best for the correlation matrix among the five ROIs when pooling data across all conditions and participants. To do this, the fMRI time series data during rest from the five ROIs were concatenated for each participant for the pre-and post-PA scans, then concatenated further across participants. These data were then submitted to AFNI’s 1dSEM (Chen et al., 2011) to perform an exhaustive search for the optimal SEM model using both tree growth and forest growth algorithms, where optimality was defined as the model that resulted in the smallest out-of-sample generalization error as quantified by the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC; Price et al., 2009), simultaneously maximizing model generalization and minimizing overfitting. The optimal model was then parameterized for each participant’s individual pre and post PA resting-state scans using AFNI’s 1dSEMr (Chen et al., 2011). Only participants with model parameters and post PA versus pre PA contrasts that were less than 3 standard deviations away from the group mean were retained for subsequent analyses, leading to the exclusion of two participants (final sample = 36; see also Gotts et al., 2021). As with functional connectivity, analysis was conducted using linear mixed-effects models (3dLME), with each model parameter serving as the dependent variable, and Group (left PA, right PA), Phase (pre PA, post PA), and their interaction as fixed effects. Partial correlations with behavior were analyzed by calculating the Spearman’s r value between the change in model parameters from pre to post PA at each connection (simply post-PA_parameter minus pre-PA_parameter) and the change in behavior from pre to post PA (post-PA_behavioral_score minus pre-PA_behavioral_score), with mean framewise displacement and average voxelwise signal amplitude removed from the fMRI and behavioral measures as nuisance covariates. For the open-loop and straight-ahead pointing tasks, the early-post-adaptation measures were used as the post PA measure, since this period was the closest in time to the training and maximized the behavioral effects of interest. An FDR at q < 0.05 was used to correct for multiple comparisons and two-tailed paired or independent z-tests were carried out for post hoc comparisons. An overview of the workflow involved in the effective connectivity analyses is shown in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Analysis workflow. PLB, perceptual line bisection; MLB, manual line bisection; SA, straight-ahead pointing; OL, open-loop pointing; PA, prism adaptation; SD, standard deviation; SEM, structural equation modeling.

Results

Behavioral measurements

As in Schintu et al. (2020b), the behavioral data were submitted to mixed-model ANOVAs with Phase (pre PA, post PA for perceptual and manual line bisection tasks and pre PA, early-post PA, late-post PA for the straight-ahead and open-loop pointing tasks) as within-subjects variable, and Group (left PA, right PA) as between-subjects variable. The behavioral results summarized here (see Figure 3) are nearly identical to those reported in Schintu et al. (2020b), with the only difference being the total number of participants included (N = 36, as opposed to N = 38 in Schintu et al., 2020b).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Behavioral results. Negative and positive values represent left and right of the true center (0 cm), respectively. PSE, point of subjective equality; PA, prism adaptation. Error bars represent standard error of the mean. *p < 0.05. Filled circles represent right PA group individual data and open circles represent left PA group individual data. Adapted from figure 2 in Schintu et al. (2020b).

On the perceptual line bisection task, there was a main effect of Phase [F(1, 34) = 4.712, p = 0.037, ηp2 = 0.122], such that both groups’ midline judgment shifted rightward of the true center after PA (from − 1.40 to − 0.39 mm), meaning that PA caused a rightward visuospatial bias independently of prism direction. Other main effect or interaction did not reach significance [Fs ≤ 0.903, ps ≥ 0.349].

On the manual line bisection task, there was also a main effect of Phase [F(1, 34) = 9.105, p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.211] such that both groups’ performance shifted rightward of the true center after PA (from − 2.46 to − 0.10 mm). Other main effects or interaction did not reach significance [Fs ≤ 2.364, ps ≥ 0.133].

On the straight-ahead pointing task, there was a significant Phase x Group interaction [F(2, 68) = 31.320, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.479], such that the left PA group shifted rightward at both early [t(17) = −5.722, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.35] and late post measurements [t(17) = −4.246, p = 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.32], and right PA group shifted leftward at both early [t(17) = 4.182, p = 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.99] and late [−0.81 cm; t(17) = 4.880, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.96] post measurements, meaning that left and right PA affected proprioceptive performance in the expected direction and that both groups were adapted until the end of the experiment. There was also a main effect of Group [F(1, 34) = 15.276, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.310] indicating a general rightward pointing for the left PA group and leftward pointing for the right PA group and no main effect of Phase [F(2, 68) = 1.517, p = 0.227]. As in Schintu et al. (2020b), the amount of adaptation (i.e., absolute value) did not differ significantly between groups at either time point [ts ≤ 0.803, ps ≥ 0.428].

On the open-loop pointing task, there was a significant Phase x Group interaction [F(2, 68) = 278.251, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.891], with the left PA group shifting rightward at the early [t(17) = −17.822, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 4.20] and late t(17) = −9.054, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 4.11] post measurements, and the right PA group shifting leftward at the early [t(17) = 12.196, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 2.87] and late [−1.93 cm; t(17) = 9.694, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 2.81] post measurements, meaning that left and right PA affected visuomotor performance in the expected direction and that both groups were adapted until the end of the experiment. There was a main effect of Group [F(1,34) = 86.675, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.718] indicating a general rightward pointing for the left PA group and leftward pointing for the right PA group, and no main effect of Phase [F(2, 68) = 1.455, p = 0.241]. Again, the amount of visuomotor adaptation (i.e., absolute value) did not differ between groups [ts ≤ −1.035, ps ≥ 0.308].

Resting-state functional connectivity

To characterize the changes in functional connectivity among the five ROIs that survived the whole-brain linear mixed-effects (LME) regression analysis (Schintu et al., 2020b), Pearson’s correlations among the ROI time series were calculated for each participant, Fisher’s z’ transformed, and then submitted to linear mixed-effects analyses with Group (left PA, right PA), Phase (Pre PA, Post PA), and their interaction as fixed effects. While there were no significant main effects of Group, there were significant main effects of Phase involving the left IPS with the right IPS, the right PHG, and the left declive, and the right lateral PMC with the right PHG and left declive (FDR-corrected to q < 0.05; all ps ≤ 0.014). These main effects of Phase corresponded to decreases in connectivity from Pre to Post PA irrespective of PA direction (Figure 4). However, as expected from the original seed-based results (Schintu et al., 2020b) there were significant Phase x Group interactions observed for both the left and right IPS ROIs with the right PHG and left declive, and a trend-level interaction involving the left IPS and the right lateral PMC (p = 0.044, uncorrected). The significant and corrected interactions appeared to be driven exclusively bya decrease in functional connectivity from Pre to Post PA involving the bilateral IPS and the right PHG and left declive in the right PA group (z-tests, all p < 0.001), with non-significant changes at the same connections for the left PA group.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Functional connectivity results. Functional connectivity correlation matrices. PA, prism adaptation. Pre, before PA; Post, after PA; IPS, intraparietal sulcus; PHG, parahippocampal gyrus; PMC, premotor cortex. See also Supplementary material.

Resting-state effective connectivity

The functional connectivity data were submitted to SEM (McIntosh and Gonzalez-Lima, 1994; Price et al., 2009; Chen et al., 2011; Reid et al., 2019) using AFNI’s 1dSEM and 1dSEMr. The model search process (see effective connectivity analysis paragraph) arrived at a five-parameter model involving the following directional connections: (1) left IPS → right PHG, (2) right IPS → left IPS, (3) right PHG → left IPS, (4) left declive → right PHG, and (5) right lateral PMC → left declive. As shown in Figure 5, the model had two branches interacting through the right PHG, (1) a loop between left IPS and right PHG, with additional input to left IPS from right IPS, and (2) a feedforward branch from right lateral PMC to left declive to right PHG. The model was then parameterized for each participant’s experimental conditions (e.g., left PA, pre PA), and each parameter served as a dependent variable in linear mixed-effects modeling for group analysis, with Phase, Group, and their interaction as fixed effects. No connections in the pre and post PA conditions were found to be significantly negative in the left PA or right PA groups and only excitatory connections were detected. As with functional connectivity, there were no significant main effects of Group. There was a significant main effect of Phase with no Phase x Group interaction (p = 0.343) for the right lateral PMC → left declive connection [F(1,34) = 11.628, p = 0.002, q < 0.05], corresponding to a significant decrease in effective connectivity from pre to post adaptation in the left PA group (p = 0.002, q < 0.05) and a non-significant trend for decreased connectivity in the right PA group (p = 0.085). There were significant Phase x Group interactions for left IPS → right PHG [F(1,34) = 13.639, p < 0.001, q < 0.05] and the right PHG → left IPS connections [F(1,34) = 7.498, p = 0.001, q < 0.05]. For left IPS → right PHG, there was a non-significant trend for increased connectivity in the left PA group (p = 0.068) and a significant decrease for the right PA group (p < 0.001, q < 0.05). The reverse of this pattern was observed for the right PHG → left IPS connection, with a decrease in connectivity for the left PA group (p = 0.022) and a numerical increase in connectivity for the right PA group (p = 0.11). Taken together, the results indicate that, for the left PA group, drive is relatively reduced into the left IPS from the right PHG, whereas for the right PA group, drive is relatively reduced out of the left IPS into the right PHG, having qualitatively opposing changes on the flow of activity within the circuit. The feedforward flow from right lateral PMC to left declive, and, indirectly, the right PHG, decreased after PA independent of direction.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Effective connectivity results. (A) Five-parameter model identified by statistical equation modeling (top) and changes post PA relative to pre PA after left and right PA (bottom panels). Dotted line = Decrease in effective connectivity related to the significant main effect of Phase and Phase X Group interactions shown in (B). Effective connectivity correlation matrices. PA, prism adaptation. IPS, intraparietal sulcus; PHG, parahippocampal gyrus; PMC, premotor cortex. See also Supplementary material.

Brain-behavior correlations

To examine the associations between effective connectivity and behavior, we measured correlations between the changes (post PA minus pre PA) in open-loop pointing, straight-ahead pointing, perceptual and manual line bisection tasks, and changes in the SEM model, partialling motion, and average voxelwise standard deviation. We found significant partial correlations between change in left IPS → right PHG connection and the amount the open-loop and straight-ahead pointing measures [open-loop: Spearman’s partial r(32) = 0.520 (95% CI: 0.2206, 0.7298), p = 0.002, q < 0 0.05; straight-ahead: Spearman’s partial r(32) = 0.493 (95% CI: 0.1858, 0.7124), p = 0.003, q < 0.05; Figure 6A]. The positive sign of these correlations indicates that as drive from left IPS to right PHG increased after PA, the behaviors showed a rightward shift across participants, with the reverse following a decrease in drive from left IPS to right PHG. The right IPS → left IPS connection was positively correlated with the pointing measures [open-loop: Spearman’s partial r(32) = 0.300 (95% CI: −0.0425, 0.5794), p = 0.084; straight-ahead: Spearman’s partial r(32) = 0.356 (95% CI: 0.0203, 0.6196), p = 0.039], whereas the right PHG → left IPS connection was negatively related [open-loop: Spearman’s partial r(32) = −0.317 (95% CI: −0.5917, 0.0237), p = 0.067; straight-ahead: Spearman’s partial r(32) = −0.380 (95% CI: −0.6364, −0.048), p = 0.026]. The FDR-corrected effects involving the left IPS → right PHG connection were further checked to make sure the correlations were not due to the different behavioral center offsets in the left PA and right PA groups by applying the absolute value function to the behavioral and SEM Post PA minus Pre PA changes. For both the open-loop and straight-ahead pointing measures, these correlations with left IPS → right PHG remained significant [open-loop: Spearman’s partial r(32) = 0.413 (95% CI, 0.087, 0.6591), p = 0.015; straight-ahead: Spearman’s partial r(32) = 0.356 (95% CI, 0.0203, 0.6196), p = 0.039] (see Figure 6B), meaning that that the correlation was not driven by the opposite direction of the changes in both connectivity and behavior. Taken together, these results suggest that the behavioral effects of PA on open-loop and straight-ahead pointing are mediated by drive from the left IPS to the right PHG, with greater drive leading to a rightward shift, in a manner qualitatively similar to the group-level effects.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Bain-behavior correlation. (A) Partial correlations between post minus pre PA changes in left IPS → right PHG connection and open-loop pointing (top) and straight-ahead pointing (bottom) tasks. (B) Partial correlations between post minus pre PA changes (absolute values) in left IPS → right PHG connection and open-loop pointing (top) and straight-ahead pointing (bottom) tasks. IPS, intraparietal sulcus; PHG, parahippocampal gyrus.

While no correlations with the perceptual and manual line bisection measures survived correction, one non-significant trend was observed between the perceptual line bisection post PA minus pre PA change and the right lateral PMC → left declive connection [Spearman’s partial r(32) = 0.318 (95% CI: −0.0226, 0.5925), p = 0.066], suggesting the involvement of the second branch in the cognitive aftereffect.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate the network underlying the sensorimotor and cognitive aftereffects of PA. We employed SEM to estimate connectivity directionality in the set of clusters that survived the left PA versus right PA contrast in the whole brain functional connectivity analysis (Schintu et al., 2020b). Based on a recently proposed framework (Panico et al., 2020), we hypothesized that these effects rely on two distinct parts of the network.

At the behavioral level, the group adapted to left prisms showed a significant rightward sensorimotor aftereffect, whereas the group adapted to right prisms exhibited a significant leftward sensorimotor aftereffect, as measured by open-loop and straight-ahead pointing tasks (Schintu et al., 2020b). Both groups of participants were significantly adapted until the end of the experiment and the amount of adaptation did not differ between the two PA directions, ensuring that any possible difference at the neural level would not be due to a different level of sensorimotor adaption achieved by the two groups. Concerning the cognitive aftereffect, while we expected a rightward bias shift on the perceptual line bisection (but not manual line bisection McIntosh et al., 2019; Schintu et al., 2020a), following left, but not right, PA (Colent et al., 2000; Schintu et al., 2014, 2017, 2018), both groups judged the center of a line significantly more rightward than they did at baseline. The lack of a significant rightward shift following left PA could be due to fluctuations of the cognitive aftereffect (Schintu et al., 2014) and does not invalidate the effects on functional connectivity, since also others (see Crottaz-Herbette et al., 2014) have reported significant effects of PA on brain activity in the absence of significant behavioral changes.

At the neural level, the functional connectivity analysis examining all possible connections among the five ROIs replicated the original finding that the Phase x Group interaction was associated with changes involving bilateral IPS, right PHG, and left declive after right PA (Figure 4). The effective connectivity analysis revealed that the optimal solution was a five-parameter model with two separate branches interacting through the right PHG: (1) a loop between left IPS and right PHG, with additional input to left IPS from right IPS, and (2) a feedforward branch from right lateral PMC to left declive to right PHG (Figure 5).

Concerning the first branch, results indicate that the drive from left IPS to right PHG decreased following right PA, and the drive decreased in the opposite direction for left PA (right PHG → left IPS). Importantly, the amount of change in effective connectivity for the left IPS to right PHG connection correlated with measures of sensorimotor aftereffects (Figure 6). The positive sign of the correlations indicate that, as the drive from left IPS to right PHG increases(/decreases) after PA, the behaviors exhibit a rightward (/leftward) shift. The characterization of the first branch as the neural basis of the sensorimotor aftereffect is further supported by the significant correlations between the sensorimotor aftereffect, as indexed by the straight-ahead pointing performance, and the connection from right to left IPS and from right PHG to left IPS. It is not surprising that these other two connections show similar behavioral correlations with straight-ahead pointing performance, although at lower magnitudes, given the similar model function of the connections forming a loop among the left and right IPS and the right PHG (changing drive between the IPS and the PHG; Figure 6).

Regarding the second branch, we found significantly decreased connectivity following PA which, like the cognitive aftereffect, was independent of PA direction. Consistent with the literature mainly showing no effects of right PA on spatial bias in healthy individuals (Colent et al., 2000; Bultitude and Woods, 2010; Schintu et al., 2014, 2017), the exploratory post-hoc comparison revealed that the general decrease in effective connectivity from right PMC to the left declive was driven by the left PA group. Furthermore, this feedforward connection was marginally correlated with the amount of change in midline judgment measured by perceptual line bisection. The main effect of Phase on effective connectivity in the second branch of the network parallels the cognitive aftereffects. While no significant cognitive aftereffect for the right PA group was expected, we did expect a significant change in visuospatial bias after left PA (Schintu et al., 2014, 2017, 2020b). The absence of a visuospatial effect for the left PA group strong enough to drive a Phase x Group interaction could be due to the >35 min elapsed from adaptation, since it has been shown that the PA-induced cognitive effect lasts around 35 min (and less than 1 h) and fluctuates (Schintu et al., 2014, 2017). Thus, while tentative, the possible involvement of the second branch in the cognitive effect of prism adaptation suggests a possible division of labor between the two branches of the model, with the involvement of the IPS to PHG loop in the sensorimotor aftereffect and the right PMC to PHG branch in the cognitive aftereffect.

The involvement of the cerebellum in PA is well recognized (Weiner et al., 1983; Pisella et al., 2004). However, whether it is involved in the early recalibration phase, which allows strategic correction of pointing movement, or in the later realignment phase, which consists of automatic remapping and thought to be the basis of the cognitive aftereffect (Redding et al., 2005; Redding and Wallace, 2006), or in both phases (Panico et al., 2016), has been debated (Luauté et al., 2009; Chapman et al., 2010; Küper et al., 2014). One study (Panico et al., 2016) found cerebellar involvement in both phases, however, it only quantified the sensorimotor aftereffect. The cerebellum is involved in the second branch of the model, which is the one we propose to be responsible of the emergence of the cognitive aftereffect, in agreement with the emerging recognition of cerebellar involvement in cognition (Rondi-Reig and Burguière, 2005).

Earlier models have suggested that the mechanism of PA relies on inhibitory, bottom-up, signaling from the cerebellum to the PPC (Pisella et al., 2006; Striemer and Danckert, 2010). All the connections identified in this study are positive, meaning that none of the connections are effectively inhibitory. Instead of a direct connection between the cerebellum and PPC, the effective connectivity modeling identified one between the cerebellum and the parahippocampal gyrus. The PPC is crucial in generating PA aftereffect and the recurrent connections involving the left and right IPS present in our findings further support its central role. Surprisingly, however, the present findings also suggest that the PPC is not the region shared between the two model branches, but that it is only involved in the first branch and thus only in the sensorimotor aftereffect. The hub of the model appears to be the PHG which, along with the PPC, is involved in spatial navigation (Aguirre et al., 1996; Aguirre and D’Esposito, 1997; Maguire et al., 1998) and allocentric (world-referenced) representation (Aguirre and D’Esposito, 1999). Clearly, its role in both sensorimotor and cognitive PA-induced aftereffect requires further investigation and confirmation.

It is important to consider that effective connectivity does not necessarily correspond to physical connectivity, but rather to causal physiological connections. Therefore, there might be connections or ROIs that are not included in the present modeling. SEM is a statistical method for evaluating causality in connectivity and while it provides a window into the mechanism of action of PA, it requires confirmation with anatomical and/or physiological methods. Finally, despite right PA having become the gold standard control condition for left PA, it has consistently failed to produce significant cognitive changes in healthy individuals (Colent et al., 2000; Michel et al., 2003; Loftus et al., 2009; Bultitude and Woods, 2010; Reed and Dassonville, 2014; Schintu et al., 2014, 2017, 2018), not having compared the left PA to a neutral (no shift) group might have prevented the identification of the plastic mechanisms common to both PA directions.

Conclusion

In conclusion, this is the first study that investigates the effective connectivity underlying PA and identifies separate subnetworks for the sensorimotor and cognitive aftereffects. These novel findings call for refinement of the current model of PA and further investigation of roles of the PHG and cerebellum in PA and in visuospatial attention more generally.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the National Institutes of Health, Central Nervous System Institutional Review Board. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

SSc, EW, and SSh contributed to the conception and design of the study. SSc collected the data. SSc, SG, and MF performed imaging and statistical analysis. SSc and SG wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Science Foundation (grant number BCS-1921415 to SSh and BCS-2022572 to SSh), the National Institutes of Health Ruth L. Kirschstein National Research Service Award (to SSc), the Clinical Neurosciences Program of the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (to EW), and the NIMH Intramural Research Program (to SG).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.915260/full#supplementary-material

References

Aguirre, G. K., and D’Esposito, M. (1997). Environmental knowledge is subserved by separable dorsal/ventral neural areas. J. Neurosci. 17, 2512–2518. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-07-02512.1997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aguirre, G. K., and D’Esposito, M. (1999). Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy. Brain 122, 1613–1628. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.9.1613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aguirre, G. K., Detre, J. A., Alsop, D. C., and D’Esposito, M. (1996). The Parahippocampus subserves topographical learning in man. Cereb. Cortex 6, 823–829. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.6.823

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albert, N. B., Robertson, E. M., and Miall, R. C. (2009). The resting human brain and motor learning. Curr. Biol. 19, 1023–7. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.04.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bultitude, J. H., and Woods, J. M. (2010). Adaptation to leftward-shifting prisms reduces the global processing bias of healthy individuals. Neuropsychologia 48, 1750–1756. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapman, H. L., Eramudugolla, R., Gavrilescu, M., Strudwick, M. W., Loftus, A., Cunnington, R., et al. (2010). Neural mechanisms underlying spatial realignment during adaptation to optical wedge prisms. Neuropsychologia 48, 2595–2601. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, G., Glen, D. R., Saad, Z. S., Paul Hamilton, J., Thomason, M. E., Gotlib, I. H., et al. (2011). Vector autoregression, structural equation modeling, and their synthesis in neuroimaging data analysis. Comput. Biol. Med. 41, 1142–1155. doi: 10.1016/j.compbiomed.2011.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colent, C., Pisella, L., Bernieri, C., Rode, G., and Rossetti, Y. (2000). Cognitive bias induced by visuo-motor adaptation to prisms: a simulation of unilateral neglect in normal individuals? Neuroreport 11, 1899–1902. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200006260-00019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, R. W. (1996). AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. Int. J. 29, 162–173. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crottaz-Herbette, S., Fornari, E., and Clarke, S. (2014). Prismatic adaptation changes Visuospatial representation in the inferior parietal lobule. J. Neurosci. 34, 11803–11811. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3184-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crottaz-Herbette, S., Fornari, E., Notter, M. P., Bindschaedler, C., Manzoni, L., and Clarke, S. (2017). Reshaping the brain after stroke: The effect of prismatic adaptation in patients with right brain damage. Neuropsychologia 104, 54–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dale, A. M., Fischl, B., and Sereno, M. I. (1999). Cortical surface-based analysis. NeuroImage 9, 179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Danckert, J., Ferber, S., and Goodale, M. A. (2008). Direct effects of prismatic lenses on visuomotor control: an event-related functional MRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 28, 1696–1704. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06460.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frassinetti, F., Magnani, B., and Oliveri, M. (2009). Prismatic lenses shift time perception. Psychol. Sci. 20, 949–954. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02390.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J. (1994). Functional and effective connectivity in neuroimaging: a synthesis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2, 56–78. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460020107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J. (2011). Functional and effective connectivity: A review. Brain Connect. 1, 13–36. doi: 10.1089/brain.2011.0008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gotts, S. J., Gilmore, A. W., and Martin, A. (2020). Brain networks, dimensionality, and global signal averaging in resting-state fMRI: hierarchical network structure results in low-dimensional spatiotemporal dynamics. NeuroImage 205:116289. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gotts, S. J., Milleville, S. C., and Martin, A. (2021). Enhanced inter-regional coupling of neural responses and repetition suppression provide separate contributions to long-term behavioral priming. Commun. Biol. 4:487. doi: 10.1038/s42003-021-02002-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gudmundsson, L., Vohryzek, J., Fornari, E., Clarke, S., Hagmann, P., and Crottaz-Herbette, S. (2020). A brief exposure to rightward prismatic adaptation changes resting-state network characteristics of the ventral attentional system. PLoS One 15:e0234382. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0234382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallquist, M. N., Hwang, K., and Luna, B. (2013). The nuisance of nuisance regression: spectral misspecification in a common approach to resting-state fMRI preprocessing reintroduces noise and obscures functional connectivity. NeuroImage 82, 208–225. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helmholtz, H. (1867). Treatise on physiological optics, Vol. 3, Trans. and ed. J. P. C Southall Dover.(Translated from the 3rd German edition, English edition 1962).[aJN, HER]. Massachusetts: Courier Corporation.

Google Scholar

Jacquin-Courtois, S., Rode, G., Pavani, F., O’Shea, J., Giard, M. H., Boisson, D., et al. (2010). Effect of prism adaptation on left dichotic listening deficit in neglect patients: glasses to hear better? Brain 133, 895–908. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jewell, G., and McCourt, M. E. (2000). Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia 38, 93–110. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00045-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jo, H. J., Gotts, S. J., Reynolds, R. C., Bandettini, P. A., Martin, A., Cox, R. W., et al. (2013). Effective preprocessing procedures virtually eliminate distance-dependent motion artifacts in resting state FMRI. J. Appl. Math., 2013. doi: 10.1155/2013/935154

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Küper, M., Wünnemann, M. J. S., Thürling, M., Stefanescu, R. M., Maderwald, S., Elles, H. G., et al. (2014). Activation of the cerebellar cortex and the dentate nucleus in a prism adaptation fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 1574–1586. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loftus, A. M., Vijayakumar, N., and Nicholls, M. E. R. (2009). Prism adaptation overcomes pseudoneglect for the greyscales task. Cortex 45, 537–543. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luaute, J., Michel, C., Rode, G., Pisella, L., Jacquin-Courtois, S., Costes, N., et al. (2006). Functional anatomy of the therapeutic effects of prism adaptation on left neglect. Neurology 66, 1859–1867. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000219614.33171.01

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luauté, J., Schwartz, S., Rossetti, Y., Spiridon, M., Rode, G., Boisson, D., et al. (2009). Dynamic changes in brain activity during prism adaptation. J. Neurosci. 29, 169–178. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3054-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maguire, E. A., Burgess, N., Donnett, J. G., Frackowiak, R. S. J., Frith, C. D., and O’Keefe, J. (1998). Knowing where and getting there: A human navigation network. Science 280, 921–924. doi: 10.1126/science.280.5365.921

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McIntosh, R. D., Brown, B. M. A., and Young, L. (2019). Meta-analysis of the visuospatial aftereffects of prism adaptation, with two novel experiments. Cortex 111, 256–273. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McIntosh, A. R., and Gonzalez-Lima, F. (1994). Structural equation modeling and its application to network analysis in functional brain imaging. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2, 2–22. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460020104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McIntosh, R. D., Rossetti, Y., and Milner, A. D. and others. (2002). Prism adaptation improves chronic visual and haptic neglect: a single case study. Cortex, 38, pp.309–320, doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70662-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michel, C. (2016). Beyond the sensorimotor plasticity: cognitive expansion of prism adaptation in healthy individuals. Front. Psychol. 6:1979. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01979

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michel, C., Pisella, L., Halligan, P. W., Luauté, J., Rode, G., Boisson, D., et al. (2003). Simulating unilateral neglect in normals using prism adaptation: implications for theory. Neuropsychologia 41, 25–39. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00135-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miles, W. R. (1930). Ocular dominance in human adults. J. Gen. Psychol. 3, 412–430. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1930.9918218

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milner, A. D., Brechmann, M., and Pagliarini, L. (1992). To halve and to halve not: an analysis of line bisection judgements in normal subjects. Neuropsychologia 30, 515–526. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(92)90055-Q

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newport, R., and Schenk, T. (2012). Prisms and neglect: what have we learned? Neuropsychologia 50, 1080–1091. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.01.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panico, F., Rossetti, Y., and Trojano, L. (2020). On the mechanisms underlying prism adaptation: A review of neuro-imaging and neuro-stimulation studies. Cortex 123, 57–71. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panico, F., Sagliano, L., Grossi, D., and Trojano, L. (2016). Cerebellar cathodal tDCS interferes with recalibration and spatial realignment during prism adaptation procedure in healthy subjects. Brain Cogn. 105, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2016.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pisella, L., Michel, C., Gréa, H., Tilikete, C., Vighetto, A., and Rossetti, Y. (2004). Preserved prism adaptation in bilateral optic ataxia: strategic versus adaptive reaction to prisms. Exp. Brain Res. 156, 399–408. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1746-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pisella, L., Rode, G., Farnè, A., Tilikete, C., and Rossetti, Y. (2006). Prism adaptation in the rehabilitation of patients with visuo-spatial cognitive disorders. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 19, 534–542. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e328010924b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Price, L. R., Laird, A. R., Fox, P. T., and Ingham, R. J. (2009). Modeling dynamic functional neuroimaging data using structural equation modeling. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 16, 147–162. doi: 10.1080/10705510802561402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Redding, G. M., Rossetti, Y., and Wallace, B. (2005). Applications of prism adaptation: a tutorial in theory and method. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 431–444. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Redding, G. M., and Wallace, B. (2006). Prism adaptation and unilateral neglect: review and analysis. Neuropsychologia 44, 1–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reed, S. A., and Dassonville, P. (2014). Adaptation to leftward-shifting prisms enhances local processing in healthy individuals. Neuropsychologia 56, 418–427. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reid, A. T., Headley, D. B., Mill, R. D., Sanchez-Romero, R., Uddin, L. Q., Marinazzo, D., et al. (2019). Advancing functional connectivity research from association to causation. Nat. Neurosci. 22, 1751–1760. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0510-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rondi-Reig, L., and Burguière, E. (2005). Is the cerebellum ready for navigation? Prog. Brain Res. 148, 199–212. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(04)48017-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossetti, Y., Rode, G., Pisella, L., Farné, A., Li, L., Boisson, D., et al. (1998). Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect. Nature 395, 166–169. doi: 10.1038/25988

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saj, A., Cojan, Y., Vocat, R., Luauté, J., and Vuilleumier, P. (2013). Prism adaptation enhances activity of intact fronto-parietal areas in both hemispheres in neglect patients. Cortex 49, 107–119. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schenkenberg, T., Bradford, D. C., and Ajax, E. T. (1980). Line bisection and unilateral visual neglect in patients with neurologic impairment. Neurology 30, 509–517. doi: 10.1212/WNL.30.5.509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schintu, S., Chaumillon, R., Guillaume, A., Salemme, R., Reilly, K. T., Pisella, L., et al. (2020a). Eye dominance modulates visuospatial attention. Neuropsychologia 141:107314. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schintu, S., Freedberg, M., Alam, Z. M., Shomstein, S., and Wassermann, E. M. (2018). Left-shifting prism adaptation boosts reward-based learning. Cortex 109, 279–286. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.09.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schintu, S., Freedberg, M., Gotts, S. J., Cunningham, C. A., Alam, Z. M., Shomstein, S., et al. (2020b). Prism adaptation modulates connectivity of the Intraparietal sulcus with multiple brain networks. Cereb. Cortex 30, 4747–4758. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schintu, S., Kravitz, D. J., Silson, E. H., Cunningham, C. A., Wassermann, E. M., and Shomstein, S. (2022). Dynamic Changes in Spatial Representation within the Posterior parietal Cortex in Response to Visuomotor Adaptation. Cerebral Cortex. Accepted.

Google Scholar

Schintu, S., Martín-Arévalo, E., Vesia, M., Rossetti, Y., Salemme, R., Pisella, L., et al. (2016). Paired-pulse parietal-motor stimulation differentially modulates Corticospinal excitability across hemispheres when combined with prism adaptation. Neural Plast. 2016, 1–9. doi: 10.1155/2016/5716179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schintu, S., Patané, I., Caldano, M., Salemme, R., Reilly, K. T., Pisella, L., et al. (2017). The asymmetrical effect of leftward and rightward prisms on intact visuospatial cognition. Cortex 97, 23–31. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schintu, S., Pisella, L., Jacobs, S., Salemme, R., Reilly, K. T., and Farnè, A. (2014). Prism adaptation in the healthy brain: The shift in line bisection judgments is long lasting and fluctuates. Neuropsychologia 53, 165–170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, J., Pillai, A., Chen, K., and Horwitz, B. (2012). Effective connectivity modeling for fMRI: six issues and possible solutions using linear dynamic systems. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 5:104. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Striemer, C. L., and Danckert, J. A. (2010). Through a prism darkly: re-evaluating prisms and neglect. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 308–316. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szczepanski, S. M., and Kastner, S. (2013). Shifting Attentional priorities: control of spatial attention through hemispheric competition. J. Neurosci. 33, 5411–5421. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4089-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Talairach, J., and Tournoux, P. (1988). Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging. New York: Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Tsujimoto, K., Mizuno, K., Nishida, D., Tahara, M., Yamada, E., Shindo, S., et al. (2018). Prism adaptation changes resting-state functional connectivity in the dorsal stream of visual attention networks in healthy adults: A fMRI study. Cortex 119, 594–605. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.10.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J. X., Rogers, L. M., Gross, E. Z., Ryals, A. J., Dokucu, M. E., Brandstatt, K. L., et al. (2014). Targeted enhancement of cortical-hippocampal brain networks and associative memory. Science 345, 1054–1057. doi: 10.1126/science.1252900

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L., Mruczek, R. E. B., Arcaro, M. J., and Kastner, S. (2015). Probabilistic Maps of Visual Topography in Human Cortex. Cereb Cortex 25, 3911–3931. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu277

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiner, M. J., Hallett, M., and Funkenstein, H. H. (1983). Adaptation to lateral displacement of vision in patients with lesions of the central nervous system. Neurology 33, 766–772. doi: 10.1212/WNL.33.6.766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilf, M., Serino, A., Clarke, S., and Crottaz-Herbette, S. (2019). Prism adaptation enhances decoupling between the default mode network and the attentional networks. NeuroImage 200, 210–220. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.06.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Medical Association (2013). World medical association declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA 310, 2191–2194. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.281053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: visuospatial attention, visuomotor adaptation, parahippocampal gyrus, sensorimotor adaptation, posterior parietal cortex, navigation network, cerebellum

Citation: Schintu S, Gotts SJ, Freedberg M, Shomstein S and Wassermann EM (2022) Effective connectivity underlying neural and behavioral components of prism adaptation. Front. Psychol. 13:915260. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.915260

Received: 08 April 2022; Accepted: 04 August 2022;
Published: 02 September 2022.

Edited by:

Nicola Canessa, University Institute of Higher Studies in Pavia, Italy

Reviewed by:

Monika Halicka, Catholic University of Leuven, Belgium
Stephanie Clarke, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois (CHUV), Switzerland

Copyright © 2022 Schintu, Gotts, Freedberg, Shomstein and Wassermann. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Selene Schintu, selene.schintu@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.