Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 20 May 2022
Sec. Positive Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Positive Educational Approaches to Teaching Effectiveness and Student Well-being View all 17 articles

Contribution of Academic Satisfaction Judgments to Subjective Well-Being

\r\nMauricio F. Zalazar-Jaime*Mauricio F. Zalazar-Jaime1*Luciana S. Moretti,Luciana S. Moretti1,2Leonardo A. Medrano,\r\nLeonardo A. Medrano1,2
  • 1Secretaría de Investigación, Universidad Siglo 21, Córdoba, Argentina
  • 2Departamento de Psicología, Pontificia Universidad Católica Madre y Maestra, Santiago de los Caballeros, Dominican Republic

The role of academic satisfaction (AS) on persistence and successful academic behavior has been the focus of research for decades. Nevertheless, driven by positive educational psychology, subjective well-being has been highlighted as another central feature in the academic path of students. Studies aimed at identifying the variables that contribute to explain different aspects of academic performance have been widely investigated, although studies aimed at identifying the determinants of subjective well-being are still limited. The present paper examined the contribution of AS judgments on subjective well-being (SWB). To this end, it was hypothesized that SWB levels depend on the balance between positive/negative emotions and life satisfaction judgments. Furthermore, it was stipulated that AS has an indirect contribution on SWB, through life satisfaction, whereas the balance of emotions influences both AS judgments and life satisfaction. Using an analysis strategy based on structural equation modeling, the results indicated that the model fitted satisfactorily, explaining 32% of the variance of SWB. Particularly, it was observed that AS judgments contributed to life satisfaction judgments (β = 0.34). Although no direct contribution of AS on SWB was reported, a total contribution partially mediated by life satisfaction judgments was revealed (total β = 0.19). These findings support the importance of academic satisfaction judgments, not only because of their importance in academic terms, but also because of their impact on university students’ subjective well-being and health.

Introduction

Contribution of Academic Satisfaction Judgments to Subjective Well-Being

Positive education promotes the development” of educational environments that allow students to learn an established educational curriculum, while acquiring the skills necessary to develop their own well-being and that of others (Oades et al., 2011; Riedel et al., 2020). This is a fundamental key point because the development of positive teaching practices and methods can contribute to achieving learning potential, developing positive attitudes toward higher education and skills for future employment of university students (Nilson, 2010; Eryılmaz, 2015).

The pursuit of happiness and achieving “a good life” are considered to be one among the elemental missions of psychology (Povedano-Diaz et al., 2020). However, the scientific study of well-being does not aim to prescribe what a good life entails, but to analyze the factors that lead people to positively evaluate their lives according to their own parameters. This evaluation is called “Subjective well-being” (SWB) and has been shown to be a phenomenon of importance for physical and mental health. Different research indicates that SWB is beneficial for health (Brummett et al., 2009; Segerstrom and Sephton, 2010) and longevity (Lyubomirsky et al., 2005; Howell et al., 2007; Chida and Steptoe, 2008). In addition, SWB is associated with supportive social relationships, citizenship, work performance, and resilience (Lyubomirsky et al., 2005; Diener and Chan, 2011).

SWB is a desirable goal for the society as a whole and in particular for adolescent and young adults (D’Agostino et al., 2019). During this distinctive stage, biological, psychological, cognitive and social changes can affect the levels of SWB, so this variable can be considered as an indicator of how they cope with these changes (Povedano-Diaz et al., 2020). SWB also works as a protective factor, people with higher SWB are more likely to develop health behaviors such as exercising, not smoking and drinking less alcohol (Diener et al., 2018). Also, higher levels of SWB are associated with the ability to form positive relationships, establish strong self-esteem, effectively express feelings and regulate emotions, persevere, and positively engage in challenging tasks (Shoshani and Slone, 2017; Bradshaw, 2019). Different studies also indicate that SWB is related to positive academic outcomes, and a better adaptation to the demands and characteristics of the university system (Casas, 2011; Fernandes Ferreira Lima and Araujo de Morais, 2018; Tomás et al., 2020).

Subjective Well-Being and Academic Satisfaction in University Students

The most popular model on SWB is the one proposed by Diener et al. (2018), which “includes people’s appraisals and evaluations of their own lives” (p. 1). This conceptualization differentiates, on the one hand, cognitive judgments such as those linked to life satisfaction and on the other hand, an emotional component which involves a balance between positive affect (pleasant and desirable feelings and moods) and negative affect (unpleasant and undesirable feelings and moods). In this model, the role of satisfaction judgments is key. According to Diener et al. (2018) when people reflect on their life, they make both general (considering the totality of their life) and specific judgments (they evaluate more specific domains such as satisfaction with studies, work or family). Goal satisfaction theory (Diener et al., 2018) assumes that the satisfaction of key needs, desires and goals will produce to high levels of SWB, and thus the dissatisfaction of them will produce to low levels of SWB. Various experimental and non-experimental studies indicate that vital satisfaction assessments are strongly associated with SWB indices (Schimmack, 2008) and changes in satisfaction assessments generate significant changes in SWB (Gamble and Gärling, 2012).

As proposed by Diener (1984, 1994) the general assessment of life satisfaction depends successively on the extent of satisfaction within a group of specific domains. In addition, at different points in life, priorities and life circumstances change, and thus we will expect that the predictors of happiness can also change (Jebb et al., 2020). In this sense, it is expected that satisfaction with the academic experience constitutes a domain of great importance for people during their university studies (Lent, 2004).

Academic satisfaction (AS) is a high priority satisfaction domain in the lives of college students (Lent, 2004; Lent et al., 2017). Several studies sustain the importance of AS on well-being and life satisfaction (Garriott et al., 2015; Sheu et al., 2016). A review by Suldo et al. (2006) refers to a series of studies suggesting that students’ feelings and attitudes toward their university are significantly related to their level of life satisfaction. Likewise, studies developed within the framework of social cognitive career theory highlight the contribution of AS on overall life satisfaction, along with other relevant constructs such as self-efficacy beliefs and perception of social support (Lent et al., 2009, 2012; Ojeda et al., 2011; Sheu et al., 2017).

On the other hand, it is important to note that academic dissatisfaction can also be a source of subjective discomfort. During their formative years, university students are exposed to numerous academic demands, which can lead to stress, loss of confidence and demotivation. AS constitutes a protective factor against stress and helps students to cope more healthily with the challenges of academic life (Tessema et al., 2012; Lent et al., 2013; Kong and Yan, 2014). In contrast, it has been observed that students with low levels of AS present greater vulnerability to stress and lower academic performance, which negatively affects their well-being (Kuo et al., 2014).

It should also be noted that the conceptual delimitation of AS has not been without controversy. AS is often understood as a subjective and global cognitive assessment by students of their learning experience at university. AS judgments therefore derive from the comparison students make between their expectations and their perceived academic experience, the conformation of those assessments being supported by the knowledge available within the autobiographical memory (Bickart and Schwarz, 2001). As pointed out by Lent et al. (2007), there is a parallel conceptualization that understands AS as “the level of enjoyment that students perceive when carrying out experiences linked to their role as students” (p. 87). However, this definition is considered inadequate since positive affect would be a variable closely linked to, but different from satisfaction judgments. Positive affect influences the shaping of satisfaction judgments (Schoefer, 2008) and would also increase as a consequence of the favorable evaluation that people make (Tessema et al., 2012). This close link between positive affect and satisfaction judgments leads to the fact that they are often used interchangeably, when in fact they are associated but different variables. In the present study, AS is considered as a cognitive construct linked to, but distinct from, affect.

Despite the relevance of AS on student well-being, most of the studies conducted have focused on investigating the role of AS on academic behavior (e.g., academic performance and persistence). In fact, no SWB studies specifically addressing the contribution of AS to SWB were found within the literature. For this reason, the objective of this study was to evaluate the impact of academic satisfaction on the SWB of university students. Specifically, and according to the model proposed by Diener, we hypothesized that: (a) levels of subjective well-being depend on life satisfaction judgments and the balance of positive/negative emotions; (b) domain-specific judgments (academic satisfaction) have a direct and indirect contribution, through life satisfaction, on SWB; and (c) the balance of emotions influences academic satisfaction and life satisfaction judgments (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Contribution of academic satisfaction on the subjective well-being model.

Methodology

Participants

The sample was constituted by 326 university students between 17 and 46 years old (M = 21.76; SD = 1.61), of both sexes (men = 38.7%; women = 61.3%). The participants were from public (68%) and private (32%) universities, distributed in the following careers: social sciences (48%), natural sciences (29%), technology (21%) and arts and humanities (2%). The sampling was non-probabilistic and accidental. That is, the possibility of participation was limited to those students who were on the university campus and gave their consent. In addition, as this was a cross-sectional study, no dropouts were recorded.

Instruments

Subjective Well-Being

To measure this variable, Subjective Happiness Scale (SHS; Lyubomirsky and Lepper, 1999) was used. In order to achieve a measure of overall “Subjective Well-being,” Lyubomirsky and Lepper (1999) have designed the SHS to measure the levels of Well-being in a comprehensive and global sense. This measure is a subjective assessment of whether a person is happy or unhappy. This is a 4-item scale, two of the items ask participants to characterize themselves in terms of statements such as “in general I consider myself….” on a 7-point Likert scale, where 1 is not very happy and 7 is very happy. The other two items provide a brief description of happy and unhappy individuals and ask participants to say the extent to which they identify with each description, for example “some people are very happy in general and enjoy life come what may, exploiting life to the full. To what extent does this characterization describe you? where 1 is not at all and 7 is to a large extent. Vera-Villarroel et al. (2011) reported studies on psychometric properties with adequate internal factor structure and consistency (α values between 0.73 and 0.87) for populations of adults, adolescents and university students; r-values obtained for depression and neuroticism scales in studies of discriminant validity were between –0.24 and –0.63 and significant positive correlations with measures of optimism and extroversion between 0.27 and 0.55. It should be noted that in this context the concepts of subjective happiness and well-being are equivalent (Lyubomirsky and Lepper, 1999).

Positive and Negative Affect Scale

This is a 20-item scale in which 10 items evaluate states of positive affect (for example “active,” “strong,” “inspired”) and the remaining 10, states of negative affect (such as “guilty,” “scared,” “hostile”; Watson et al., 1988). Participants were asked to indicate on a 5-point Likert scale the extent to which they experience each one of the mentioned affective states, 1 signifying very seldom or never and 5 very frequently or almost always. In his original version, Watson et al. (1988) reported studies with an internal consistency of between 0.86 and 0.90 for Positive Affect and between 0.84 and 0.87 for Negative Affect. In a study adapted to Argentina, Medrano et al. (2015) were able to replicate the structure of two factors and indicated acceptable indices of internal consistency (α = 0.83 for Negative Affect and α = 0.82 for Positive Affect).

Satisfaction With Life Scale

This 5-item scale investigates the level of satisfaction with situations related to life in general Diener et al. (1985); participants respond on a 7-point Likert scale where 1 is completely disagree and 7 is entirely agree. Diener et al. (1985) reported satisfactory internal structure, test-retest reliability and internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.87) for the scale’s psychometric properties. For the present study the version translated into Spanish by Gómez et al. (2007) was used, in which there are 5 response options, with 1 representing total disagreement and 5 total agreement. For the psychometric properties the Spanish version showed adequate values for internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.84), internal structure (exploratory factor analysis) and convergent-discriminant validity (Atienza et al., 2000).

Academic Satisfaction

Since the basic aim of the current paper was to evaluate the contribution of AS to the model of SWB, it was decided to treat the former as a latent variable in order to arrive at a more concise measurement. Structural equation modeling (SEM) allows for the simultaneous use of several variables for a theoretical, unobservable construct, which ultimately leads to more valid conclusions on the construct level, helping to reduce measurement error. In the present study we applied three different AS scales. The first, AS1, is the scale developed by Lent et al. (2007) comprising 7 items in which respondents must evaluate their level of satisfaction with different aspects of their academic experience (for instance, “I enjoy my classes most of the time”). Respondents are asked to indicate the extent to which they agree to each statement on a 10-point Likert scale. The original psychometric studies suggested that the scale has a unidimensional factor structure and high internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.94). The version adapted by Medrano et al. (2014) was used, in which the internal structure and internal consistency structures are satisfactory (α = 0.85). The second tool (AS2) is the scale developed by Fernandes Sisto et al. (2008) comprising 11 items (statements such as “I like the classes”) to be rated on a 4-point Likert scale where 1 is never and 4 always. For the purpose of the present study, the version adapted by Medrano and Pérez (2010) was used. The psychometric results corroborated the unidimensional structure of the scale via exploratory factor analysis and its internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.84). The third instrument, AS3, is the global index of academic satisfaction (Argyle, 1987). This scale consists of a single item assessing academic satisfaction via a graphic representation of responses on a visual scale of 10 positions. Students were asked to respond on a scale of 1–10 to the item “based on how you feel about your studies, indicate where you would place yourself on the following diagram?” where 1 is “the worst learning experience I could ever have” and 10 is “the best learning experience I could ever have.

Procedure

After providing clear and explicit information to the university authorities concerning the nature and purpose of the study, questionnaires were delivered collectively during class under the supervision of the authors of the paper. The voluntary nature of participation was emphasized and the confidentiality of the data assured, stating clearly that the results would be used exclusively for research purposes and that the identity of the individual participants would remain confidential. Each participating student signed a consent form. The following questionnaires were then handed out over the next 12 min: Subjective Happiness (Lyubomirsky and Lepper, 1999), Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS; Watson et al., 1988), Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS; Diener et al., 1985) and the three measures of Academic Satisfaction (Argyle, 1987; Lent et al., 2007; Fernandes Sisto et al., 2008).

SEM was used to examine the relationships among variables. The main advantage of the SEM technique is that it allows for a more precise analysis of empirical data by taking into account latent variables and complex patterns of relationships between variables.

Results

Initial Descriptive and Exploratory Analysis of the Data

Atypical univariate cases were identified by calculating the Z ratings for each variable (Z > ± 3.29 were considered atypical) and multivariate cases by means of the Mahalanobis distance (p < 0.001; Tabachnick and Fidell, 2013). Twenty two atypical univariate cases were observed and 19 multivariate. Although atypical cases tend to distort the results it was decided to include them in the analysis since otherwise the data would fail to represent a segment of the population (Hair et al., 1999). The mean, standard deviation, asymmetry and kurtosis were calculated. Values between ± 1.00 were considered excellent for evaluating the asymmetry and kurtosis indices and values lower than ± 2.00 were considered adequate (George and Mallery, 2016). All asymmetry and kurtosis variables presented values between ± 1.00 with the exception of General Satisfaction, which showed an adequate level of asymmetry R (-1.11), higher than the kurtosis range (2.40; see Table 1). Multivariate normality was verified by Mardia’s coefficient (Mardia, 1970, 1974), giving a value of 12.06, which is below the critical value of 70 suggested by Rodríguez Ayán and Ruiz (2008) and thus approaches the standard normal distribution. The association between the different variables was verified using Pearson’s r correlation coefficient. All correlations were statistically significant with weak to moderate r-values, thus discarding any overlap between the variables (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2013; see Table 2).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for mean and standard deviation, asymmetry and kurtosis of the variables comprising the subjective well-being model.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Bivariate correlations between the variables comprising the subjective well-being model.

Evaluation of the Subjective Well-Being Model

The statistical software Mplus 6.12. was used to assess goodness of fit using the maximum likelihood estimator (ML), applying the following statistical tools to measure the fit of the model: Chi-squared, Comparative Fit Index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), and the Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR). CFI and TLI indices with values between ≥ 0.90 and 0.95 or higher are considered an acceptable to excellent fit and values of < 0.08 are expected for RMSEA and < 0.06 for SRMR (Hu and Bentler, 1999; Yu and Muthén, 2002). The results indicated a satisfactory fit (χ2 = 9.176; df = 5; p = 0.000; CFI = 0.99; TLI = 0.98; RMSEA = 0.05, [90% CI = 0.00, 0.10], SRMR = 0.020), explaining 32% of the subjective well-being variance.

Direct contributions of satisfaction with life (β = 0.48, p < 0.01) and positive/negative emotions (β = 0.17, p < 0.01) were observed. On the contrary, the relationship between academic satisfaction and subjective well-being was not corroborated (β = 0.03, p > 0.05). In order to adequately understand how one variable relates to another, indirect effects, understood as the product of the two standardized direct effects involved, must also be considered. By examining these effects it is verified that satisfaction with life modulates the relationship between positive/negative emotions and subjective well-being (indirect effect, β = 0.17, p < 0.01). Similar behavior was evident with respect to the contribution of academic satisfaction and subjective well-being through satisfaction with life (indirect effect, β = 0.16, p < 0.01). When examining the magnitude of the total effects (see Table 3), it can be seen that the variables that have the greatest contribution to subjective well-being are positive and negative emotions (β total = 0.34), and academic satisfaction (β total = 0.19, see Figure 2).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Total effects, direct and indirect, of the subjective well-being model.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Contribution of academic satisfaction on the subjective well-being model. Standardized path coefficients. **p < 0.01.

Discussion

Many universities are focused on the students’ performance, specifically in terms of good grades, without paying enough attention to other relevant aspects that should be part of the overall personal development (Tomás et al., 2020). According to Diener (1994), levels of well-being depend partially on the degree of satisfaction with different life domains (Argyle, 1992, 1994; Diener et al., 1999; Diener, 2000). The importance of each domain varies in accordance with its proximity and immediacy in a person’s life. AS judgments could thus be an important determinant of the level of well-being of university students; however, current scientific research has been centered on factors associated with academic performance and the specific contribution of AS has not been analyzed (Smith et al., 2003). Academic performance is of course a relevant construct, but high performing students with low SWB present lower levels of well-being (Lent et al., 2009; Tessema et al., 2012; Balkis, 2013; Kuo et al., 2014). This lack of studies taking into account the effect of academic satisfaction on mental health therefore led us to evaluate its contribution in the subjective well-being model proposed by Diener (1994) as applied to a sample of university students in Cordoba, Argentina.

Our findings corroborate the importance of AS judgments in well-being: it was observed that the greater the academic satisfaction, the more likely this was to favorably impact on students’ satisfaction with life in general. No direct and significant influence of satisfaction assessments on well-being was observed, possibly due to the effect of moderators such as age or the introductory/strictly theoretical nature of some subjects, which could bias satisfaction levels. Future studies should therefore control for these variables. Nonetheless it was observed that the total effect was slightly higher than the direct contribution of positive and negative emotions on well-being. These results are consistent with those reported in the literature. In a recent study, Lee and Shin (2022) evaluated a cross-cultural model of academic and life satisfaction, highlighting the contribution of positive affect on AS and the contribution of these constructs on life satisfaction.

In the context of efforts to promote the health and optimal academic behavior of students, Durkin and Joseph (2009) point out the crucial importance of looking more closely into the factors that promote well-being. Intervention strategies based on the theoretical framework of Lent’s Social Cognitive Career Theory (Lent, 2004) would contribute toward explaining how students develop these satisfaction judgments. For example, Lent (2004) reports that in self-efficacy beliefs about one’s ability to perform well, having positive outcome expectations and feeling supported, tend to affect academic satisfaction judgments. Measures to endorse self-efficacy beliefs should therefore be centered on incrementing the capacities involved: establishing and advancing toward a set of performance goals for each stage of academic study, predicting favorable results and perceiving the environment to be supportive.

As mentioned by Diener (1994, 2000) and Lent (2004) and in congruence with the triadic model (taking into account personal, behavioral and environmental factors) proposed by Bandura (1987), the findings of the present study underscore the fact that AS judgments not only affect the academic domain but also have an impact on satisfaction with life and indirectly, on well-being. These results highlight the importance of providing experiences that promote changes in AS. In the literature related to SCCT, it is observed that the most important promoter of satisfaction judgments is self-efficacy beliefs. Different studies (e.g., Wang et al., 2004; Tschannen-Moran and McMaster, 2009) point out the importance of focusing on the sources of information (mastery experience, social persuasion, vicarious learning, and physiological/emotional states), which contribute to such beliefs (Bandura, 1987). In this sense, Lam and Santos (2018) developed an intervention program aimed at modifying self-efficacy beliefs through the identification of appropriate models, anxiety management, recording of negative/destructive thoughts, small group discussions, consultations with the tutor and individual assignments (workbooks), among other aspects. This line of action, added to the recommendations made by Brown and Krane (2000) to modify such sources could be used as a focus to increase beliefs and, therefore, satisfaction judgments.

Regarding positive and negative emotions, the present study observed that students with positive emotions tend to be more satisfied with their lives, inducing a higher level of well-being. The literature linked to positive psychology highlights different intervention practices aimed at increasing positive affect (Sin and Lyubomirsky, 2009). For example, daily recording of gratitude (Emmons and McCullough, 2003; Froh et al., 2008) and recounting one’s own acts of kindness (Otake et al., 2006) has been associated with higher levels of positive affect, life satisfaction, and a decrease in negative affect. Similarly, Seligman et al. (2005) have shown that writing down three good things that went well each day and using the top strengths in a novel way each day for a week, increases well-being and decreases depressive symptoms. As mentioned by Fredrickson (2001) generating practices linked to positive emotions contributes to optimal functioning by increasing the thought-action repertoire, psychological resilience, and emotional well-being, thus minimizing persistent negative emotions.

Another modality of intervention lies in mindfulness practice. Burke (2014) argues that negative affect can be reduced as reactivity is reduced, as a consequence of adopting a non-judgmental perspective on internal experiences; whereas positive affect can increase as a consequence of engaging in the full experience of life, relating to oneself and third parties in a self-compassionate manner. On this point, there are intervention models based on the Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) paradigm adapted to educational contexts (see Broderick and Metz, 2009; Santorelli and Kabat-Zinn, 2009; Kaiser-Greenland, 2010; Semple and Lee, 2011, for examples), which have evidenced satisfactory results. As indicated by Diener et al. (2018) the different practices aimed at modifying SWB are characterized by persistent effects during moderate times and the effects of their effectiveness may vary depending on the practice performed by individuals. Based on these results, the development of intervention programs should be incorporated into the educational curriculum to be sustainable over time.

As for the limitations of the study, it should be noted that the sample is somewhat small, with a higher proportion of women, and the distribution of students in the different areas of knowledge is not homogeneous, with a greater representation of social sciences. These aspects should be considered for the generalization of the results. However, the review of the literature and, particularly, the theoretical foundations that support the relationships between the variables, suggest the suitability of the model and similar results can be found if the indicated limitations are overcome.

Despite the previous points, suggestions for future lines of research stand out. First, the construct of positive and negative affect could be complemented with an evaluation of the intensity and frequency of these emotions (Diener et al., 1985) to analyze the differential role of these variants. Second, as mentioned in several studies (Argyle, 1992, 1994; Diener et al., 1999; Diener, 2000), the particular circumstances of each student’s life, e.g., family, friends and social relationships, should be assessed to increase the explanatory power of the well-being model. Thirdly, in recent years, the presence of integrative models has been observed in the educational literature, which allow the combination between them, allowing the evaluation of multiple dimensions of educational behavior (Lent et al., 2013). In this sense, a line of action could include the constructs of positive emotions, engagement, relationships, meaning and accomplishment (PERMA) proposed by Seligman (2011) to assess well-being, to the proposed model with the aim of analyzing the interplay between the variables under study. As mentioned by Lee and Shin (2022) and, fourthly, empirical studies of a longitudinal nature are still limited compared to cross-sectional studies. This situation does not allow us to inspect both the temporal prevalence of the constructs and the possible bidirectional relationships. Therefore, longitudinal studies are needed to analyze these aspects. Finally, as a result of the development of these lines of action, it is necessary to carry out intervention programs through the development of practices and/or teaching methods, with the aim of contributing to positive education.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Comité Académico de ética en Investigación de la Facultad de Psicología (Universidad Nacional de Córdoba). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Argyle, M. (1987). The Psychology of Happiness. London: Methuen.

Google Scholar

Argyle, M. (1992). The Social Psychology of Everyday Life. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Argyle, M. (1994). The Psychology of Interpersonal Behaviour. London: Penguin.

Google Scholar

Atienza, F. L., Pons, D., Balaguer, I., and García Merita, M. (2000). Propiedades psicométricas de la Escala de Satisfacción con la Vida en adolescentes. Psicothema 12, 314–319.

Google Scholar

Balkis, M. (2013). Academic procrastination, academic life satisfaction and academic achievement: the mediation role of rational beliefs about studying. J. Cogn. Behav. Psychother. 13, 57–74.

Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1987). Pensamiento y Acción [Thought and Action]. Barcelona: Martinez Roca.

Google Scholar

Bickart, B., and Schwarz, N. (2001). Service experiences and satisfaction judgments: the use of affect and beliefs in judgment formation. J. Consum. Psychol. 11, 29–41. doi: 10.1207/S15327663JCP1101_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradshaw, J. (2019). Child subjective well-being studies. Some points for discussion. Rev. Politiq. Soc. Fam. 131, 219–229. doi: 10.3406/caf.2019.3361

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broderick, P. C., and Metz, S. (2009). Learning to BREATHE: a pilot trial of a mindfulness curriculum for adolescents. Adv. Sch. Ment. Health Promot. 2, 35–46. doi: 10.1080/1754730X.2009.9715696

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, S. D., and Krane, N. E. R. (2000). “Four (or five) sessions and a cloud of dust: old assumptions and new observations about career counseling,” in Handbook of Counseling Psychology, 3rd Edn, eds S. D. Brown and R. W. Lent (New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.), 740–766.

Google Scholar

Brummett, B. H., Boyle, S. H., Kuhn, C. M., Siegler, I. C., and Williams, R. B. (2009). Positive affect is associated with cardiovascular reactivity, norepinephrine level, and morning rise in salivary cortisol. Psychophysiology 46, 862–869. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00829.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, C. (2014). “An exploration of the effects of mindfulness training and practice in association with enhanced well-being for children and adolescents: theory, research, and practice,” in Interventions and Policies to Enhance Well-being, eds F. A. Huppert and C. L. Cooper (Chichester: Wiley Blackwell), 141–184. doi: 10.4324/9781315563862-11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casas, F. (2011). Subjective social indicators and child and adolescent well-being. Child Indic. Res. 4, 555–575. doi: 10.1007/s12187-010-9093-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chida, Y., and Steptoe, A. (2008). Positive psychological well-being and mortality: a quantitative review of prospective observational studies. Psychosom. Med. 70, 741–756. doi: 10.1097/psy.0b013e31818105ba

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Agostino, A., Grilli, G., and Regoli, A. (2019). The determinants of subjective well-being of young adults in Europe. Appl. Res. Qual. Life 14, 85–112. doi: 10.1007/s11482-017-9582-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychol. Bull. 95, 542–575.

Google Scholar

Diener, E. (1994). Assessing subjective well-being: progress and opportunities. Soc. Indic. Res. 31, 103–157. doi: 10.1007/BF01207052

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E. (2000). Subjective well-being: the science of happiness and a proposal for a national index. Am. Psychol. 55, 34–43. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.34

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E. D., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., and Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. J. Pers. Assess. 49, 71–75. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., and Chan, M. Y. (2011). Happy people live longer: subjective well-being contributes to health and longevity. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being 3, 1–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1758-0854.2010.01045.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., Oishi, S., and Tay, L. (2018). Advances in subjective well-being research. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2, 253–260. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0307-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diener, E., Suh, E., Lucas, R., and Smith, H. (1999). Subjective well-being: three decades of progress. Psychol. Bull. 125, 276–302. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durkin, J., and Joseph, S. (2009). Growth following adversity and its relation with Subjective well-being and psychological well-being. J. Loss Trauma 14, 228–234. doi: 10.1080/15325020802540561

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emmons, R. A., and McCullough, M. E. (2003). Counting blessings versus burdens: an experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective well-being in daily life. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 377–389. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eryılmaz, A. (2015). Positive psychology in the class: the effectiveness of a teaching method based on subjective well-being and engagement increasing activities. Int. J. Instr. 8, 17–32. doi: 10.12973/iji.2015.822a

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandes Ferreira Lima, R., and Araujo de Morais, N. (2018). Bienestar subjetivo de niños/as y adolescentes: revisión integradora. Cien. Psicol. 12, 249–260.

Google Scholar

Fernandes Sisto, F., Muniz, M., Bartholomeu, D., Vítola Pasetto, S., Francisca de Oliveira, A., and Guimarães Lopes, M. W. (2008). Estudo para a Construção de uma Escala de Satisfação acadêmica para universitários. Avaliação Psicol. 7, 45–55.

Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: the broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Am. Psychol. 56, 218–226. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Froh, J. J., Sefick, W. J., and Emmons, R. A. (2008). Counting blessings in early adolescents: an experimental study of gratitude and subjective well-being. J. Sch. Psychol. 46, 213–233. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2007.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gamble, A., and Gärling, T. (2012). The relationships between life satisfaction, happiness, and current mood. J. Happiness Stud. 13, 31–45. doi: 10.1007/s10902-011-9248-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garriott, P. O., Hudyma, A., Keene, C., and Santiago, D. (2015). Social cognitive predictors of first-and non-first-generation college students’ academic and life satisfaction. J. Couns. Psychol. 62, 253–263. doi: 10.1037/cou0000066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

George, D., and Mallery, P. (2016). IBM SPSS Statistics 23 Step by Step: A Simple Guide and Reference. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Gómez, V., Villegas, C., Barrera, F., and Cruz, J. (2007). Factores predictores de bienestar subjetivo en una muestra colombiana. Rev. Latinoam. Psicol. 39, 311–325.

Google Scholar

Hair, J., Anderson, R., Tatham, R., and Black, W. (1999). Análisis Multivariante, 4a Edn. México: Prentice Hall.

Google Scholar

Howell, R. T., Kern, M. L., and Lyubomirsky, S. (2007). Health benefits: meta-analytically determining the impact of well-being on objective health outcomes. Health Psychol. Rev. 1, 83–136. doi: 10.1080/17437190701492486

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L. T., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Modeling 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jebb, A. T., Morrison, M., Tay, L., and Diener, E. (2020). Subjective well-being around the world: trends and predictors across the life span. Psychol. Sci. 31, 293–305. doi: 10.1177/0956797619898826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaiser-Greenland, S. (2010). The Mindful Child. New York, NY: Free Press.

Google Scholar

Kong, H., and Yan, Q. (2014). The relationship between learning satisfaction and career competencies. Int. J. Hosp. Manag. 41, 133–139. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhm.2014.05.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuo, Y. C., Walker, A. E., Schroder, K. E., and Belland, B. R. (2014). Interaction, Internet self-efficacy, and self-regulated learning as predictors of student satisfaction in online education courses. Intern. High. Educ. 20, 35–50. doi: 10.1016/j.iheduc.2013.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lam, M., and Santos, A. (2018). The impact of a college career intervention program on career decision self-efficacy, career indecision, and decision-making difficulties. J. Career Assess. 26, 425–444. doi: 10.1177/1069072717714539

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, E. S., and Shin, Y.-J. (2022). Cross-sectional and longitudinal tests of social cognitive model of well-being in Korean College students. J. Career Assess. 30, 110–133. doi: 10.1177/10690727211026187

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lent, R. (2004). Toward a unifying theoretical and practical perspective on well-being and psychosocial adjustment. J. Couns. Psychol. 51, 482–509. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.51.4.482

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lent, R. W., do Céu Taveira, M., and Lobo, C. (2012). Two tests of the social cognitive model of well-being in Portuguese college students. J. Vocat. Behav. 80, 362–371. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2011.08.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lent, R. W., Miller, M. J., Smith, P. E., Watford, B. A., Lim, R. H., Hui, K., et al. (2013). Social cognitive predictors of adjustment to engineering majors across gender and race/ethnicity. J. Vocat. Behav. 83, 22–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2013.02.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lent, R. W., Singley, D., Sheu, H. B., Schmidt, J. A., and Schmidt, L. C. (2007). Relation of social-cognitive factors to academic satisfaction in engineering students. J. Career Assess. 15, 87–97. doi: 10.1177/1069072706294518

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lent, R. W., Taveira, M. D. C., Figuera, P., Dorio, I., Faria, S., and Gonçalves, A. M. (2017). Test of the social cognitive model of well-being in Spanish college students. J. Career Assess. 25, 135–143. doi: 10.1177/1069072716657821

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lent, R., do Céu Taveira, M., Sheu, H., and Single, D. (2009). Social cognitive predictors of academic adjustment and life satisfaction in Portuguese college students: a Longitudinal analysis. J. Vocat. Behav. 74, 190–198. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2008.12.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyubomirsky, S., and Lepper, H. (1999). A measure of subjective happiness: preliminary reliability and construct validation. Soc. Indic. Res. 46, 137–155. doi: 10.1023/A:1006824100041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., and Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive affect: does happiness lead to success? Psychol. Bull. 131, 803–855. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.803

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mardia, K. V. (1970). Measures of multivariate skewness and kurtosis with applications. Biometrika 57, 519–530. doi: 10.2307/2334770

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mardia, K. V. (1974). Applications of some measures of multivariate skewness and kurtosis in testing normality and robustness studies. Sankhya Ser. B 36, 115–128.

Google Scholar

Medrano, L. A., Fernández Liporance, M., and Pérez, E. (2014). Computarized assessment system for academic satisfaction (ASAS) for first-year university student. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 12, 541–562.

Google Scholar

Medrano, L. A., Flores-Kanter, E., Trógolo, M., Ríos, M., Curerello, A., and González, J. (2015). Adaptación de la Escala de Afecto Positivo y Negativo (PANAS) para la población de Estudiantes Universitarios de Córdoba [Adaptation of the Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS) to student university population from Córdoba]. Anu. Investig. Fac. Psicol. 2, 22–36.

Google Scholar

Medrano, L., and Pérez, E. (2010). Adaptación de la escala de satisfacción académica a la población universitaria de Córdoba [Adaptation of Academic Satisfaction Scale to University Population from Córdoba]. Summa Psicol. 7, 5–14. doi: 10.4067/s0718-50062013000600003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nilson, L. (2010). Teaching at its Best: A Research-Based Resource for College Instructors, Third Edn. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Google Scholar

Oades, L., Robinson, P., Green, S., and Spence, G. B. (2011). Towards a positive university. J. Posit. Psychol. 6, 432–439. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2011.634828

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ojeda, L., Flores, L. Y., and Navarro, R. L. (2011). Social cognitive predictors of Mexican American college students’ academic and life satisfaction. J. Couns. Psychol. 58, 61–71. doi: 10.1037/a0021687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Otake, K., Shimai, S., Tanaka-Matsumi, J., Otsui, K., and Fredrickson, B. L. (2006). Happy people become happier through kindness: a counting kindnesses intervention. J. Happiness Stud. 7, 361–375. doi: 10.1007/s10902-005-3650-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Povedano-Diaz, A., Muñiz-Rivas, M., and Vera-Perea, M. (2020). Adolescents’ life satisfaction: the role of classroom, family, self-concept and gender. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17, 1–12. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17010019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riedel, R., Vialle, W., Pearson, P., and Oades, L. G. (2020). Quality learning and positive education practice: the student experience of learning in a school-wide approach to positive education. Int. J. Appl. Posit. Psychol. 5, 53–75. doi: 10.1007/s41042-020-00029-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez Ayán, M., and Ruiz, M. (2008). Atenuación de la asimetría y de la curtosis de las puntuaciones observadas mediante transformaciones de variables: incidencia sobre la estructura factorial. Psicológica 29, 205–227.

Google Scholar

Santorelli, S. F., and Kabat-Zinn, J. (2009). Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) Professional Education and Training: MBSR Curriculum and Supporting Materials. Amherst, MA: University of Massachusetts Medical Center.

Google Scholar

Schimmack, U. (2008). “The structure of subjective well-being,” in The Science of Subjective Well-Being, eds M. Eid and R. J. Larsen (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 97–123.

Google Scholar

Schoefer, K. (2008). The role of cognition and affect in the formation of customer satisfaction judgements concerning service recovery encounters. J. Consum. Behav. Int. Res. Rev. 7, 210–221. doi: 10.1002/cb.246

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Segerstrom, S. C., and Sephton, S. E. (2010). Optimistic expectancies and cell-mediated immunity: the role of positive affect. Psychol. Sci. 21, 448–455. doi: 10.1177/0956797610362061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seligman, M. E. P. (2011). Flourish: A Visionary New Understanding of Happiness and Well-Being. New York, NY: Free Press.

Google Scholar

Seligman, M. E. P., Steen, T. A., Park, N., and Peterson, C. (2005). Positive psychology progress: empirical validation of interventions. Am. Psychol. 60, 410–421. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.5.410

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Semple, R. J., and Lee, J. (2011). Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for Anxious Children. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.

Google Scholar

Sheu, H. B., Liu, Y., and Li, Y. (2017). Well-being of college students in China: testing a modified social cognitive model. J. Career Assess. 25, 144–158. doi: 10.1177/1069072716658240

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheu, H. B., Mejia, A., Rigali-Oiler, M., Primé, D. R., and Chong, S. S. (2016). Social cognitive predictors of academic and life satisfaction: measurement and structural equivalence across three racial/ethnic groups. J. Couns. Psychol. 63, 460–474. doi: 10.1037/cou0000158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shoshani, A., and Slone, M. (2017). Positive education for young children: effects of a positive psychology intervention for preschool children on subjective well being and learning behaviors. Front. Psychol. 8:1866. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sin, N. L., and Lyubomirsky, S. (2009). Enhancing well-being and alleviating depressive symptoms with positive psychology interventions: a practice-friendly meta-analysis. J. Clin. Psychol. 65, 467–487. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, C. M., Plant, M., Carney, R. N., Stein Arnold, C., Jackson, A., Johonson, L. S., et al. (2003). Productivity of educational psychologists in educational psychology journals, 1997-2001. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 28, 422–430. doi: 10.1016/S0361-476X(02)00044-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suldo, S. M., Riley, K. N., and Shaffer, E. J. (2006). Academic correlates of children and adolescents’ life satisfaction. Sch. Psychol. Int. 27, 567–582. doi: 10.1177/0143034306073411

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tabachnick, B. G., and Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using Multivariate Statistics, 6th Edn. Boston, MA: Pearson.

Google Scholar

Tessema, M. T., Ready, K., and Yu, W. C. (2012). Factors affecting college students’ satisfaction with major curriculum: evidence from nine years of data. Int. J. Hum. Soc. Sci. 2, 34–44.

Google Scholar

Tomás, J. M., Gutiérrez, M., Pastor, A. M., and Sancho, P. (2020). Perceived social support, school adaptation and adolescents’ subjective well-being. Child Indic. Res. 13, 1597–1617. doi: 10.1007/s12187-020-09717-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tschannen-Moran, M., and McMaster, P. (2009). Sources of self-efficacy: four professional development formats and their relationship to self-efficacy and implementation of a new teaching strategy. Elem. Sch. J. 110, 228–245. doi: 10.1086/605771

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vera-Villarroel, P., Celis-Atenas, K., and Córdova-Rubio, N. (2011). Evaluación de la felicidad: análisis psicométrico de la escala de felicidad subjetiva en población chilena. Terapia Psicol. 29, 127–133. doi: 10.4067/S0718-48082011000100013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L., Ertmer, P. A., and Newby, T. J. (2004). Increasing preservice teachers’ self-efficacy beliefs for technology integration. J. Res. Technol. Educ. 36, 231–250. doi: 10.1080/15391523.2004.10782414

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, C. Y., and Muthén, B. (2002). “Evaluation of model fit indices for latent variable models with categorical and continuous outcomes,” in Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0194739

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: subjective well-being, academic satisfaction, satisfaction judgments, university students, structural equation modeling

Citation: Zalazar-Jaime MF, Moretti LS and Medrano LA (2022) Contribution of Academic Satisfaction Judgments to Subjective Well-Being. Front. Psychol. 13:772346. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.772346

Received: 08 September 2021; Accepted: 02 May 2022;
Published: 20 May 2022.

Edited by:

Mirna Nel, North-West University, South Africa

Reviewed by:

Andreja Brajsa Zganec, Institute of Social Sciences Ivo Pilar (IPI), Croatia
Victoria L. Bowers, Saybrook University, United States

Copyright © 2022 Zalazar-Jaime, Moretti and Medrano. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mauricio F. Zalazar-Jaime, mfzalazar@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.