Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 18 December 2020
Sec. Neuro-Otology
This article is part of the Research Topic Vestibular Contributions to Health and Disease, Volume II - Dedicated to Bernard Cohen View all 26 articles

Responses of Neurons in the Medullary Lateral Tegmental Field and Nucleus Tractus Solitarius to Vestibular Stimuli in Conscious Felines

  • 1Department of Otolaryngology, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
  • 2Department of Neuroscience, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
  • 3Department of Pharmacology and Toxicology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, United States

Considerable evidence shows that the vestibular system contributes to adjusting sympathetic nervous system activity to maintain adequate blood pressure during movement and changes in posture. However, only a few prior experiments entailed recordings in conscious animals from brainstem neurons presumed to convey baroreceptor and vestibular inputs to neurons in the rostral ventrolateral medulla (RVLM) that provide inputs to sympathetic preganglionic neurons in the spinal cord. In this study, recordings were made in conscious felines from neurons in the medullary lateral tegmental field (LTF) and nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS) identified as regulating sympathetic nervous system activity by exhibiting changes in firing rate related to the cardiac cycle, or cardiac-related activity (CRA). Approximately 38% of LTF and NTS neurons responded to static 40° head up tilts with a change in firing rate (increase for 60% of the neurons, decrease for 40%) of ~50%. However, few of these neurons responded to 10° sinusoidal rotations in the pitch plane, in contrast to prior findings in decerebrate animals that the firing rates of both NTS and LTF neurons are modulated by small-amplitude body rotations. Thus, as previously demonstrated for RVLM neurons, in conscious animals NTS and LTF neurons only respond to large rotations that lead to changes in sympathetic nervous system activity. The similar responses to head-up rotations of LTF and NTS neurons with those documented for RVLM neurons suggest that LTF and NTS neurons are components of the vestibulo-sympathetic reflex pathway. However, a difference between NTS/LTF and RVLM neurons was variability in CRA over time. This variability was significantly greater for RVLM neurons, raising the hypothesis that the responsiveness of these neurons to baroreceptor input is adjusted based on the animal's vigilance and alertness.

Introduction

The neural pathways through which baroreceptor and other inputs affect sympathetic nervous system activity have been determined in a variety of mammalian species (13). A caveat is that most neurophysiologic experiments examining changes in activity of neurons in key brainstem areas that participate in regulating cardiovascular function were conducted in anesthetized or decerebrate animals (4). An exception is the rostral ventrolateral medulla (RVLM), an area of the reticular formation that plays a key role in controlling the firing rate of sympathetic preganglionic neurons that regulate heart rate and constriction of vascular smooth muscle (5, 6). Three studies reported the activity of RVLM neurons in conscious felines and how it differs from that in decerebrate animals (79). These studies showed that RVLM neurons whose activity is correlated with the cardiac cycle, and presumably participate in controlling sympathetic nervous system activity, have spontaneous firing rates that vary over time. They also compared peak and trough firing rates of RVLM neurons between cardiac cycles and determined that they can also fluctuate during a recording session. It was postulated that these changes in spontaneous firing rate and cardiac-related activity (CRA) of RVLM neurons were dependent on supratentorial inputs and related to the animal's alertness, vigilance, and attentiveness to its environment (4, 7, 9).

Studies have shown that much larger changes in body position are required to modulate the activity of RVLM neurons in conscious animals than in decerebrate animals (8). Inputs from the vestibular system participate in regulating sympathetic nervous system activity during changes in body position (10). In decerebrate felines, vestibular inputs elicited by small (<10°) sinusoidal head rotations were sufficient to alter the activity of sympathetic efferent fibers in peripheral nerves as well as the firing rate of RVLM neurons (8, 11). However, in conscious animals the firing rates of RVLM neurons were unaffected by 10° sinusoidal body rotations (8, 9), although the activity of a third of the neurons was modulated by static 40° head-up tilts, with an increase in activity in ~50% of units, and a decrease in the other 50% (9). These changes in unit activity were not correlated with changes in heart rate, suggesting that they were not due to engagement of the baroreceptor reflex. Gating of vestibular inputs to RVLM neurons such that these neurons only respond to large-amplitude static tilts has physiologic relevance, as head-up tilts <40° in amplitude do not appreciably affect the distribution of blood in the body requiring a compensatory change in sympathetic nervous system activity (12). This is unlike other vestibular-elicited responses, such as vestibulo-ocular reflexes, which are engaged during changes in head position <1° in magnitude (13).

Baroreceptor inputs are relayed to RVLM neurons through a pathway that includes neurons in nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS), which receive direct inputs from baroreceptor afferents (13). NTS neurons relay baroreceptor signals to the RVLM through connections in the medullary reticular formation (13). In felines, neurons in the dorsolateral medullary reticular formation, a region termed the lateral tegmental field (LTF), also play a key role in relaying baroreceptor signals to the RVLM (1417). There is also evidence that both NTS and LTF neurons transmit vestibular signals to RVLM neurons. NTS (1820) and LTF (21) receive direct projections from the vestibular nuclei. Projections from the vestibular nuclei to NTS are glutamatergic (20), but the neurotransmitters used by projections from the vestibular nuclei to LTF in felines are unknown. Experiments in decerebrate animals showed that NTS (22) and LTF (23) neurons respond to small-amplitude sinusoidal rotations of the animal's body, including low-frequency rotations that primarily activate otolith organs. To our knowledge, the activity of NTS neurons with baroreceptor inputs has not been recorded in conscious animals. Although one prior study considered the responses of LTF neurons to vestibular stimuli in conscious animals, the focus of this work was neurons that lacked CRA that presumably were part of the “vomiting center,” which also is located in the dorsolateral medullary reticular formation (24).

The present study examined CRA and responses to 10° sinusoidal rotations and 40° static tilts of NTS and LTF neurons in conscious felines. The major goal was to ascertain whether NTS and LTF neurons are likely components of the vestibulo-sympathetic pathway in conscious animals, with responses to vestibular stimuli similar to those of RVLM neurons presumed to transmit these signals to sympathetic preganglionic neurons in the spinal cord (9). In addition, comparing the responses to whole-body tilts of NTS and LTF neurons with those of RLVM neurons provides insights into the role of neurons at each stage of the vestibulo-sympathetic reflex pathway in transforming signals from the inner ear. As noted above, changes in sympathetic nervous system activity are elicited only during large-amplitude changes in body position that can result in decreased venous return to the heart and diminished cardiac output (4, 12). A gating of signals thus occurs in the vestibulo-sympathetic circuitry to suppress responses to vestibular inputs elicited during small-amplitude movements. It is informative to compare how NTS and LTF neurons respond to small- and large-amplitude rotations in conscious animals, to determine if they are components of this gating mechanism.

A secondary goal of these experiments was to establish whether anticipation of a passive movement elicits changes in NTS and LTF neuronal activity. It is well-established that feedforward cardiovascular responses are elicited prior to exercise and some other active movements (2527). A study that monitored changes in heart rate and cerebral blood flow in conscious felines before and during 60° head-up tilts preceded by a light cue demonstrated that no preparatory cardiovascular responses occur before such imposed changes in body position (28). The conclusion of this study was that preparatory cardiovascular responses occur before active movements, but not passive (imposed) movements requiring increases in sympathetic nervous system activity. This conclusion was supported by a study showing that no changes in RVLM neuronal activity occurred in conscious animals following a light cue and before the onset of head-up tilts (9). However, it is possible that changes in neuronal activity are present upstream in the vestibulo-sympathetic reflex pathway, but suppressed prior to being relayed to the RVLM. Thus, by determining whether the activity of LTF and NTS neurons changes preceding cued head-up tilts, the present study provided an additional test of the hypothesis that feedforward cardiovascular responses occur only before active movements, but are absent prior to expected passive changes in body position (28).

A preliminary report of these findings has been published on the bioRxiv preprint server (29).

Methods and Materials

All experimental procedures on animals followed the National Research Council's Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (30) and were approved by the University of Pittsburgh's Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Experiments were performed on five female antibody profile defined and specific pathogen free (APD/SPF) domestic shorthair cats obtained from Marshall BioResources (North Rose, New York, USA). Animals were 4–6 months of age when acquired from the vendor. Juvenile female animals were obtained for these studies, following our prior experience that they are more amenable than males to be acclimated for the 2 h restraint period required for data collection. Moreover, the much higher growth rate in the first year for male cats makes it difficult to maintain chronically implanted recording devices. Animals were provided commercial cat food and water ad libitum and were housed under 12 h light/dark cycles. Table 1 provides information about the animals and the parameters of the study.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Information about the animals used in the study, the length of data collection, and the number of units included in data analysis.

This study employed procedures similar to those used in our prior experiments (79, 28, 31), which entailed acclimating animals for restraint on a computer-controlled tilt table, implanting instrumentation for recording the electrocardiogram (ECG) and single unit neuronal activity from the brainstem, and subsequently collecting data over a period of 112–399 days. Following data collection, animals were euthanized, and unit locations were histologically reconstructed.

Surgical Procedures

To avoid changes in hormone levels that could affect physiological responses, animals were spayed by a veterinarian prior to the beginning of the study. After they were spayed, the animals were conditioned for body restraint in an animal holder mounted on a computer-controlled tilt table. The period of gradual acclimatization for restraint lasted about 2 months per animal but varied based on the animal's behavior. At the beginning of acclimatization, animals were initially restrained for only a few minutes per session, but as training continued, the period of restraint was slowly increased to 2 h.

After an animal was acclimated to 2 h of body restraint, a recovery surgery was performed in a dedicated operating suite using aseptic techniques. The recovery surgery entailed a craniectomy, attachment of a head fixation plate and recording chamber to the skull, and implantation of wire bundles beneath the thoracic skin to record the ECG. These procedures were described in detail in prior manuscripts (79, 28, 31). During surgeries, animals were initially anesthetized by an intramuscular injection of ketamine (20 mg/kg) and acepromazine (0.2 mg/kg), and anesthesia was maintained using 1–2% isoflurane vaporized in O2 provided through an endotracheal tube. Vital signs (heart rate, respiration rate, and blood oxygen saturation) were measured and recorded in the animal's anesthesia record every 15 min. Ringer lactate solution was provided intravenously and a heating lamp and pad were used to maintain body temperature near 38°C. Following the surgery, an antibiotic (amoxicillin, 50 mg/kg, orally twice per day) was provided for 10 days, and a transdermal fentanyl patch (25 μg/h) delivered analgesia for 72 h.

Recording Procedures

After 2 weeks of recovery from surgery, animals were acclimated for a month to head restraint by inserting a screw into the head-mounted fixation plate, and 40° whole-body tilts in the pitch plane provided by a servo-controlled hydraulic tilt table (Neurokinetics, Pittsburgh, PA USA), which was operated using a Micro1401 mk 2 data collection system and Spike-2 version 7 software (Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK). During experiments, the recording room was darkened, and black curtains were placed around the tilt table to mask any visual cues about the animal's position in space. An array of 10 LED lights generating a light intensity of 300 lumens was positioned 28 cm in front of the animal's face to provide a visual prompt prior to tilts. The light array was controlled by the Cambridge Electronic Design hardware and software, and illuminated for 2 s, from 8 to 10 s prior to the onset of tilts (see Figure 1). The light cue was presumably highly salient since the area surrounding the animal was darkened. The light cue was provided during every trial during the acclimation period, so animals had experienced several hundred tilts paired with a light cue prior to the onset of recordings. Animals were continuously monitored during recording sessions for indicators of distress, and to assure that they remained awake with eyes open.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Example of data collected during one trial. The top trace shows table position, the second shows discriminated activity for one unit, the third shows raw unit activity recorded by a microelectrode, and the bottom trace is a recording of the electrocardiogram (ECG). Initially, animals were positioned 20° nose-down for a variable amount of time. A light cue was presented (onset indicated by arrow) that lasted for 2 s. Ten seconds after the light cue was initiated, animals were tilted 40° head-up (20° nose-down to 20° nose-up) and remained in that position for 30 s prior to returning to the prone position. To determine if the light cue had any effect on a unit's activity, firing rate during the period 5 s before light onset (Pre-) was compared to that during the period 5–10 s prior to tilt onset (Post-2).

Brainstem neuronal recordings were initiated once the animals were acclimated for restraint on the tilt table. A hydraulic microdrive (model 650, David Kopf Instruments, Tujunga, CA USA) and x-y positioner, both attached to the recording chamber, allowed for epoxy-insulated tungsten microelectrodes (5 MΩ, Frederick Haer, Bowdoin, ME USA) to be inserted at defined coordinates into the brainstem. Recordings were performed over a 122–399 day period (see Table 1) and targeted to LTF and NTS neurons located 0–5 mm rostral to the obex. Units were localized using stereotaxic coordinates and physiological landmarks including the dorsal and ventral respiratory groups (which were evident as clusters of neurons with respiratory-related activity).

Activity recorded from brainstem neurons was amplified by a factor of 10,000 and filtered with a band pass of 300–10,000 Hz using an AM Systems (Sequim, WA USA) model 1800 microelectrode AC amplifier. The output of the amplifier was sampled at 25,000 Hz using the Micro1401 mk 2 data collection system and Spike-2 version 7 software. The ECG signal was amplified by a factor of 1,000 and filtered with a bandpass of 10–10,000 Hz using an AM Systems model 1700 differential AC amplifier and sampled at 2,500 Hz. Voltages from potentiometers mounted on the tilt table that signaled table position were sampled at 100 Hz. An example of data recordings is shown in Figure 1.

When a presumed NTS or LTF neuron was isolated, the animal was tilted 20° nose-down. The animal remained in this position for 1–4 min to allow baseline recordings to be collected to determine if the cell exhibited cardiac-related activity (CRA). Subsequently, the light cue was provided, and the animal was tilted 20° nose-up (such that the table movement was 40°). Since the period when the animal was tilted nose-down varied between trials, animals were not able to predict the occurrence of the light cue or the following nose-up tilt. After the animals were tilted nose-up, they remained in this position for 30 s prior to returning to their initial prone position (see Figure 1). Multiple trials (up to 13) were conducted if unit activity remained stable. For most units responses to 10° (peak-to-peak) 0.5 Hz sinusoidal rotations in the pitch plane were also recorded.

Data Analysis Procedures

Typically, only one or two units were present in the recording field during trials. The spike detection and sorting algorithm of the Spike-2 software was used to isolate the activity of each unit in the recording field, and to verify that spike shape remained consistent between trials. If there was any doubt that data were not continually recorded from the same unit, the data were discarded. Event markers generated by the Spike-2 software indicated the timing of action potentials generated by each sampled neuron, as well as the peak ECG R-waves. These event markers were used in our subsequent data analyses.

Detection of CRA

Experimental procedures similar to those used in our prior studies were used to determine if a unit exhibited CRA (79). Trigger pulses coinciding with the R-wave of the ECG were used for the creation of post R-wave interval histograms (10 ms bin width) of unit activity (Datapac software, Run Technologies; Mission Viejo CA USA). Interval histograms between successive R-waves were constructed simultaneously. Trigger pulses were also used to generate an average of the peak ECG R-wave. The averages and histograms were generated using data segments of varying length (28–211 s). When multiple trials were conducted for each neuron, a separate analysis was performed for each trial. A ratio of peak-to-background counts was calculated for each histogram. To classify a neuron as having CRA, the ratio of peak-to-background counts in the histogram had to be greater than a value of 2.0 for the majority of analyses for each unit. Figure 2 shows examples of two analyses for an NTS unit.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A–D) Cardiac-related activity (CRA) evident in two trials for an NTS neuron (A,B are for one trial and C,D are for the second trial). Histograms were triggered by event markers demarking ECG R-waves; bin width is 10 ms. (A,C) show intervals between R-waves, and (B,D) show intervals between the unit's action potentials. Unit activity peaked in periods between R-waves. (E,F) plot of counts during peak and trough periods for the LTF (E) and NTS (F) neurons classified as exhibiting CRA.

Response of Units to Light Cue and 40° Tilts

The following analyses were conducted using MATLAB software (Mathworks, Natick, MA USA). Unit firing rate and heart rate were determined for a 30 s period prior to the light cue, while the animal was tilted nose-down, as well as when the animal was positioned nose-up. Furthermore, unit firing rate and heart rate were also determined for the time period 5 s prior to the light cue and during the two consecutive 5 s time periods prior to the onset of the 40° nose-up tilt (see Figure 1). Since the duration of the light cue was 2 s, the light remained illuminated during the first 2 s of Post-1 time interval indicated in Figure 1.

Subsequent statistical analysis of the data compared heart rate and unit firing rates when animals were positioned nose-up and nose-down, as well as during the pre- and post-light cue periods prior to the onset of the tilt. As established in a prior study (9), to provide adequate statistical power these analyses were only conducted for units in which five or more 40° tilts were delivered. Statistical analyses and plotting of experimental results were performed using Prism 8 software (GraphPad software, San Diego, CA USA). Confidence intervals are indicated as mean ± one standard deviation.

Response to Sinusoidal Rotations

We additionally tested the responses of most neurons to 10° (peak-to-peak amplitude) sinusoidal tilts at 0.5 Hz to determine if they responded to small dynamic vestibular stimuli as well as large amplitude tilts. During sinusoidal rotations, neural activity was binned (500 bins/cycle) for ~35 stimulus repetitions and fitted with a sine wave using a least-squares minimization technique described in other reports (8, 22, 24, 31, 32). Two criteria were used to determine if a unit's activity was significantly modulated by 10° sinusoidal rotations: a signal-to-noise ratio >0.5 and only one evident first harmonic. To ensure that a unit's response to the rotations was not a reflection of its rhythmic spontaneous firing rate at the stimulus frequency, the stimulus was periodically stopped and restarted to determine if peak firing rate remained aligned with the same phase of head movement. Units were discarded from analysis if they were determined to have rhythmic activity at the frequency of the stimulus, although such occurrences were rare.

Reconstruction of Unit Locations

Once data collection was completed in an animal, electrolytic lesions were made at designated coordinates by passing a 100 μA negative current for 60 s through a 0.5 MΩ tungsten electrode. Approximately 1 week following the lesions, animals were anesthetized by an intramuscular injection of ketamine (20 mg/kg) and acepromazine (0.2 mg/kg) followed by an intraperitoneal injection of pentobarbital sodium (40 mg/kg). Animals were then transcardially perfused with saline followed by 10% formalin. The brain was removed and placed in a solution of 10% formalin/30% sucrose for at least 2 weeks to allow for additional fixation. After the brain was post-fixed, it was sectioned transversely at 50 μm thickness using a freezing microtome. The sections were mounted in serial order on microscope slides and stained using thionine. Images of sections were captured using a spot camera and digital software (Spot Imaging, Sterling heights, MI USA) through a Nikon Eclipse E600 microscope. Photomontages of sections were created with Adobe Illustrator software (Adobe Inc., Mountain View, CA USA) and used to plot specific unit locations. The locations of individual units were reconstructed with respect to the position of the lesions, the relative coordinates of each unit and unit depths.

Results

Activity was recorded from a total of 134 units with CRA whose locations were histologically confirmed to be in the LTF (97 neurons) and NTS (57 neurons). The locations of the units are shown in Figure 3. Additionally, unit activity was recorded from 51 units without CRA: 39 neurons in the LTF and 12 neurons in the NTS. Thus, 71% of the sampled LTF neurons and 83% of the sampled NTS neurons had CRA. Recordings from LTF neurons occurred in all experiments, whereas recordings of NTS activity were included in three experiments.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Locations of LTF and NTS neurons with CRA whose responses to 40° tilts were recorded during five or more trials. Red symbols designate units with significant differences in unit activity in the nose-down and nose-up position. Black symbols designate units without significant responses to tilts. The numbers above each section indicate the distance rostral to the obex in mm. LTF, lateral tegmental field; NTS, nucleus tractus solitarius; 5SN, spinal trigeminal nucleus; 5ST, spinal trigeminal tract; CUR, cuneate nucleus, rostral; CX, external cuneate nucleus; IO, inferior olivary nucleus; P, pyramid; VIN, inferior vestibular nucleus; RB, restiform body; VMN, medial vestibular nucleus; CD, dorsal cochlear nucleus.

Figures 2A–D shows examples of CRA in two trials for an NTS unit; Figure 2E compares the peak and trough activity for LTF neurons with CRA, and Figure 2F provides the same comparison for NTS neurons. Each point in Figures 2E,F is an average of all of the trials conducted for a cell. For every unit examined with five or more trials, a two-tailed paired t-test showed that on average peak counts were significantly greater (P < 0.05) than trough counts. For units located in the LTF, P-values ranged from <0.0001 to 0.02, with most units having P < 0.001, and only 5 having P-values between 0.002 and 0.02. On average, peak counts were 2.2 ± 0.5 (SD) times greater than trough counts. Similarly, for NTS units with five or more stimulus trials, P-values ranged from <0.0001 to 0.01, with most units having P < 0.0004, and only 7 having P-values between 0.001 and 0.01. On average, peak counts were 2.2 ± 0.4 (SD) times greater than trough counts. Even though CRA was present across trials for units that demonstrated this activity, the ratio for peak to trough counts often varied between trials, as illustrated in Figure 4 for eight units each in the LTF and NTS. For LTF units, the difference in peak-to-trough ratios for a particular unit (the largest ratio minus the smallest ratio across trials) ranged from 0.7 to 2.7; the median difference was 1.0. For NTS units, the difference in peak-to-trough ratios ranged from 0.3 to 2.9; the median difference was also 1.0. The variability in peak-to-trough ratios between runs was not significantly different for LTF and NTS neurons (P = 0.84, unpaired two-tailed t-test).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Peak-to-trough ratio of activity related to the cardiac cycle for 8 LTF and 8 NTS units where multiple trials were conducted. Data for each unit is depicted by different shapes and colors. For most units, the peak-to-trough ratio varied throughout the course of recording although all units exhibited CRA during every trial.

Responses to Changes in Head Position

Analyses of data focused on units for which multiple 40° tilts (≥5) were completed, so we could statistically ascertain whether firing rate differed in the nose-down and nose-up positions (9). A total of 45 LTF units (46% of sampled neurons; 32 with CRA) and 25 NTS units (44% of sampled neurons; 21 with CRA) were included in this statistical analysis, as indicated in Table 1. For the 32 LTF units with CRA, the following number of tilts were performed: 5 for 7 units, 6 for 7 units, 7 for 8 units, 8 for 6 units, 9 for 2 units, 10 for 1 unit, and 11 for 1 unit. For the 21 NTS units with CRA, the following number of tilts were performed: 5 trials for 8 units, 6 trials for 3 units, 7 trials for 3 units, 8 trials for 1 unit, 9 trials for 1 unit, 10 trials for 3 units, 11 trials for 1 unit, and 13 trials for 1 unit. The locations of the LTF and NTS units with CRA are indicated in Figure 3.

Responses to 40° Static Tilts

The activity of units when animals were positioned 20° nose-down was compared using a two-tailed paired t-test to that when animals were positioned 20° nose-up. Figure 1 illustrates the experimental paradigm, whereas Figure 5 illustrates the change in activity of an NTS neuron during 10 tilt trials; the activity of the unit when the animal was positioned nose-down was significantly lower than when the animal was nose-up (P < 0.0001). Of the 32 LTF units with CRA, the activity of 12 (38%) was significantly different (P < 0.05) between the nose-down and nose-up positions. Similarly, the activity of 8 of 21 NTS units with CRA (38%) was significantly different when animals were positioned nose-down and nose-up. Figure 6 shows the average percent change in firing rate during nose-up rotations for the neurons with significant responses to the change in body position. The firing of the majority of LTF (7/12) and NTS (5/8) neurons was higher when the animals were in the nose-up position. The mean absolute change in firing rate during 40° nose-up tilt was 51 ± 25% for LTF neurons and 51 ± 23% for NTS neurons. These changes in activity for NTS and LTF neurons were not significantly different (P = 0.94, unpaired two-tailed t-test). Figure 7 shows the activity during every trial for the LTF and NTS neurons that responded to tilts; P-values over each panel show the significance of the responses. In most cases, the changes in unit activity were similar between trials.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Change in activity for one NTS unit with CRA during 40° head-up tilt. (A) Unit activity during 1 s bins for 10 individual trials; each trial is demarked by lines of different colors as well as different symbols. (B) Histogram of averaged activity during 1 s bins for all trials. The red line designates table position; the transition from 20° nose-down to the 20° nose-up position (40° change in animal position) occurred 10 s after the light cue, which is demarked by a yellow arrow.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Percent change in firing rate when the animal was tilted nose-up (firing rate in nose-up position ÷ firing rate in the nose-down position) for the 12 units in the LTF and 8 units in the NTS with CRA whose activity was significantly modulated during 40° head-up tilts. Error bars indicate mean ± one standard deviation.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Firing rate during each trial for the 12 LTF and 8 NTS units with CRA whose activity was significantly modulated by 40° head-up tilt. Each panel depicts the firing rate for a single unit when the animal was positioned 20° nose-down (ND) and then subsequently positioned 20° nose-up (NU). P-values indicating the significance of the difference in firing rate in the ND and NU positions (paired t-test) are provided above each panel. The greatest significance level indicated is P < 0.01.

LTF and NTS units with CRA whose firing rates were significantly altered by 40° head-up tilt were intermixed with neurons whose firing rates were unaffected by the tilts, as indicated in Figure 3. LTF neurons that responded to tilts were located on average 2.2 ± 1.3 mm rostral to the obex and 3.0 ± 1.0 mm lateral to the midline, while unresponsive cells were located 2.5 ± 1.2 mm rostral to the obex and 2.6 ± 0.9 mm lateral to the midline. A two-way ANOVA analysis showed that distances from the obex (P = 0.74) and midline (P = 0.60) were not significantly different for LTF neurons whose activity was significantly modulated or unaffected by the 40° nose-up tilts. NTS neurons that responded to tilts were located on average 1.9 ± 0.56 mm rostral to the obex and 2.7 ± 0.48 mm lateral to the midline, while unresponsive units were located 2.1 ± 0.47 mm rostral to the obex and 2.5 ± 0.5 mm lateral to the midline. A two-way ANOVA analysis revealed that distances from the obex (P = 0.37) and midline (P = 0.26) were not significantly different for NTS units that responded to or were unaffected by the 40° nose-up tilts.

We also considered whether the firing rates of LTF units lacking CRA were significantly modulated during 40° changes in body position. Out of the 13 LTF cells lacking CRA with five or more tilt trials, seven (54%) responded significantly to 40° head-up tilts. P-values ranged from 0.0004 to 0.02, with five responses having P < 0.01. Activity decreased in four units and increased in the other three during head-up tilts. The average absolute change in firing rate for the seven units that responded significantly to 40° head-up tilts was 26 ± 21%. A two-sided Fisher's exact test failed to reveal any significant difference in the proportion of LTF units with and without CRA that responded to tilts (P = 0.31). A similar analysis was not conducted for NTS neurons, as the firing rate of only four neurons lacking CRA was recorded during five or more tilts. One of these neurons responded significantly to head-up rotations.

Heart rate was also measured during 40° head-up tilts. For the 53 units with CRA (32 LTF and 21 NTS cells) tested using 5 or more trials, heart rate decreased in 68% and increased in 32% of the trials. However, these changes in heart rate were statistically significant for only 13 of the 53 units (25%). In 12 of these 13 cases, heart rate was lower (by an average of 5.1 ± 1.4%) when animals were positioned head-up. Heart rate changed significantly (P < 0.05) during rotations for only 2/12 (17%) LTF units with CRA that responded to 40° tilts and for 5/20 (25%) of the units with CRA whose activity was not significantly modulated by 40° tilts. In 6/7 (85%) of these cases, heart rate decreased during head-up tilts, and the magnitude of the heart rate changes was small: 3.5 ± 0.10% for units that responded to tilts and 3.4 ± 5.9% for those without significant responses to the rotations. Significant changes in heart rate occurred during rotations for 3/8 (38%) of the neurons in the NTS that responded to 40° head-up tilts and 3/13 (23%) that failed to respond these movements. Heart rate decreased in all of these cases during head-up tilt. On average, heart rate decreased by 5.4 ± 0.41% for trials in which the firing rates of NTS neurons were modulated by 40° head-up tilts and decreased by 6.0 ± 1.2% for trials in which no significant changes in firing rate occurred during the rotations. The changes in heart rate during tilts that altered or failed to alter the firing rate of cells in the NTS were not significantly different (P = 0.32), as indicated by a two-tailed t-test.

Responses to 10° Sinusoidal Tilts

Responses were also recorded during 10° sinusoidal rotations in the pitch plane at 0.5 Hz for the 12 LTF units and 8 NTS units that responded to static head-up tilts. Only two of the LTF units (17%) and one unit in the NTS (13%) had significant responses to these rotations, in accordance with established criteria (8, 22, 24, 31, 32). The responses of two LTF units to 10° sinusoidal rotations are illustrated in Figure 8. All 3 units that responded significantly to 10° sinusoidal rotations also had large (>30%) changes in activity during 40° head-up tilts.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Averaged responses (~30 trials) of two LTF units with CRA to 10° sinusoidal rotations in the pitch plane at 0.5 Hz. Bin width was 4 ms (500 bins/trace). A sine wave fitted to responses is shown by black lines. The signal-to-noise (SN) ratio and gain for each unit's response is indicated. ND, nose-down; NU, nose-up.

Responses to Light Cue Preceding Static Tilts

Throughout the course of experimentation, a light cue was provided 10 s prior to the onset of each 40° head-up tilt, and we determined whether the activity of LTF or NTS units changed after this cue and before the onset of tilts (see Figure 1). As indicated in Figure 9, two-tailed paired t-tests revealed that only 3/32 LTF (9%) units with CRA, and just one that responded to head-up tilt, exhibited significant (P < 0.05) changes in activity in the 5 s time period prior to tilt onset (Post-2 period indicated in Figure 1) compared to the 5 s time period prior the light cue (Pre-period indicated in Figure 1). Only 1/21 NTS units with CRA, and none that responded to head-up tilts, had a significant increase in firing rate during the Post-2 period. A linear regression analysis failed to show a strong correlation between the significance of responses to the light cue and head-up tilts for LTF (P = 0.11, R2 = 0.23) or NTS (P = 0.03, R2 = 0.21) neurons.

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. Comparison of P-values (two-tailed t-test) for responses to head-up tilt (Tilt P) and responses to the light cue preceding tilts (Light P, pre-tilt vs. post-2 period indicated in Figure 1). Data for neurons with significant responses (P < 0.05) to head-up tilts are indicated by red symbols. Lines show the best-fit of the data with a linear regression analysis.

Discussion

These experiments included the first recordings in conscious animals from NTS and LTF neurons with changes in firing rate correlated with the cardiac cycle, which presumably participate in regulating cardiovascular responses mediated by the sympathetic nervous system (4). They showed that the activity of an appreciable fraction (38%) of both NTS and LTF neurons with CRA was significantly modulated by 40° static head-up tilts, with most (60%) having higher firing rates in the head-up position. The firing rates of NTS and LTF neurons with CRA changed on average ~50% during the 40° head-up tilts.

Such responses are similar to those of RVLM neurons during 40° static head-up tilts: 31% responded to these rotations, with an average change in firing rate of 34% (9). Approximately half of the RVLM neurons that responded to 40° head-up tilts had higher firing rates in the head-up position, whereas firing rates decreased in the other half. There was not a significant difference in the magnitude of responses to head-up tilts for NTS, LTF, and RVLM neurons (p = 0.1, one-way ANOVA). Since RVLM neurons receive direct and/or multisynaptic inputs from NTS (13) and LTF (1417), and the vestibular nuclei project to both regions (1821), these data are consistent with the hypothesis that LTF and NTS neurons are components of the vestibulo-sympathetic reflex pathway.

Although the firing rate of over a third of NTS and LTF neurons with CRA was modulated by static head-up tilts, few of these neurons responded to 10° sinusoidal rotations of the body in the pitch plane. Studies in decerebrate animals have shown that an appreciable fraction of NTS neurons with baroreceptor inputs (54%) (22) and LTF neurons (~60%) (23) responded to sinusoidal body rotations <10° in magnitude. Thus, as for RVLM neurons (8), LTF and NTS neurons with CRA only responded to large-amplitude body rotations that could produce fluid shifts requiring compensatory changes in sympathetic nervous system activity. In contrast, neurons in caudal regions of the vestibular nuclei that provide inputs to LTF and NTS responded robustly to small-amplitude body rotations in conscious felines (31, 33). Considering that the caudal aspect of the vestibular nuclei has direct projections to NTS and LTF (1821), it is unclear how gating of labyrinthine signals occurs in vestibulo-sympathetic responses, such that sympathetic nerve activity only changes during large-amplitude body rotations. It is feasible that NTS and LTF neurons also receive convergent inputs from other regions of the nervous system that modify their responses to vestibular stimuli. It has been postulated that the caudal cerebellar uvula modulates vestibulo-sympathetic responses (10), and the present data raise the prospect that a pathway originating in the cerebellum or elsewhere in the nervous system alters the responsiveness of LTF and NTS neurons to vestibular stimuli. This hypothesis remains to be validated experimentally. However, a caveat is that sinusoidal rotations of the head activate both semicircular canal and otolith organ afferents, whereas static tilts only elicit otolith organ inputs (13). Although it is evident that LTF and NTS neurons respond differently to small-amplitude sinusoidal rotations in conscious and decerebrate animals, it is feasible that prolonged 10° static tilts would alter the activity of some LTF and NTS neurons.

We also evaluated whether the responses of NTS and LTF neurons to large-amplitude static tilts could be due to activation of the baroreceptor reflex. Heart rate changes related to tilts were small and significant during trials for only a small proportion of neurons with CRA whose neuronal activity changed appreciably during the rotations. In addition, significant heart rate changes were noted for approximately equal proportions of cells whose firing rates were and were not modulated by head-up rotations. These data suggest that the primary signals altering the firing rates of NTS and LTF neurons during whole-body tilts were not from baroreceptors.

The magnitude of CRA of LTF and NTS neurons, gauged by the ratio of peak to background counts time-locked to the cardiac cycle, varied between trials. A similar analysis for RVLM data (9) showed that the variability in peak-to-trough ratios ranged from 2.8 to 4.3; the median difference was 3.4 (mean of 3.5 ± 0.6). A one-way ANOVA combined with Dunnett's post-hoc test showed that the variability in the peak-to-trough ratios was significantly larger for RVLM neurons than either NTS or LTF neurons (P < 0.0001). This variability in peak-to-trough ratios did not appear to be related to unit spontaneous firing rate, which was not significantly different (p = 0.15, one-way ANOVA) for LTF (14 ± 6 spikes/s), NTS (11 ± 6 spikes/s), and RVLM (16 ± 11 spikes/s) neurons. The difference in CRA for RVLM and NTS/LTF neurons may be a reflection of the complexity of inputs to the RVLM, which include projections from a variety of brainstem and supratentorial structures (4). These data suggest that factors such as alertness and vigilance have less of an impact on the excitability of LTF and NTS neurons than RVLM neurons, although this prospect is yet to be examined experimentally.

A light cue preceded 40° head-up tilts in these experiments, but there was no evidence that the activity of LTF or NTS units changed after the light cue and prior to the whole-body rotations. A previous experiment provided similar results for RVLM neurons (9). An earlier study showed that heart rate and cerebral blood flow was unaffected by a light cue delivered before 60° head-up tilts in conscious felines (28). Collectively, these data provide strong evidence that feedforward (preparatory) cardiovascular responses are absent prior to imposed postural changes that can result in a change in blood distribution in the body. This is in contrast to exercise and some other active movements (or imagination of active movements) in which cardiovascular responses occur prior to movement onset (2527). There is considerable evidence that motor cortex has multisynaptic influences on sympathetic nervous system activity (3436), as do subcortical areas involved in motor control (27). One possibility is that feedforward cardiovascular responses prior to movement are elicited through connections of brain motor areas with RVLM neurons, such that anticipation of imposed movements that do not entail motor responses fail to trigger changes in heart rate or peripheral blood flow. Additional experiments are needed to examine this hypothesis.

LTF and NTS neurons with and without CRA had similar responses during whole-body rotations in these studies. Studies in decerebrate and anesthetized animals have shown that neurons in these regions with respiratory-related activity and inputs from gastrointestinal receptors, as well as those with baroreceptor inputs, responded to vestibular stimuli (19, 22, 23). Head-up body rotations require changes in the activity of respiratory muscles to compensate for gravitational loading on these muscles (37), and both LTF and NTS neurons have been proposed as components of brainstem circuity that produces motion sickness (38). It is thus not surprising that NTS and LTF neurons lacking CRA responded to vestibular stimuli. However, the design of these experiments did not permit a determination of the physiologic role of neurons lacking CRA.

In summary, these experiments included the first recordings in conscious animals from NTS and LTF neurons with CRA that presumably participate in regulating cardiovascular responses mediated through the sympathetic nervous system. The studies showed that the responses of these neurons to whole-body rotations that activate the vestibular system were similar to those documented for RVLM neurons (9). In combination with other findings, these data support the conclusion that LTF and NTS neurons are components of the brainstem pathway that mediates vestibulo-sympathetic responses. However, variability in CRA from trial to trial was smaller for NTS and LTF neurons than for RVLM neurons. This observation suggests that RVLM neurons may receive stronger inputs from supratentorial regions than those in NTS and LTF, such that their excitability is modified more powerfully in accordance with an animal's vigilance and alertness. A caveat is that brainstem regions in addition to LTF and NTS, such as the caudal ventrolateral medulla (CVLM), convey both vestibular and baroreceptor signals to the RVLM, as indicated in Figure 10. This neural circuit diagram is based on the findings of both anatomical (1821, 3942) and electrophysiologic (21, 43) studies that mapped connections in the vestibulo-sympathetic pathway. Projections from the caudal aspect of the vestibular nuclei to the CVLM contain imidazole-4-acetic acid-ribotide, a putative neurotransmitter prevalent in brainstem regions that are involved in blood pressure control (39), whereas projections from the vestibular nuclei to NTS are glutamatergic (20). These observations raise the potential that responses of CVLM neurons to vestibular and baroreceptor stimuli could differ from those of NTS or LTF neurons, but this is not presently known as recordings from CVLM neurons have not yet been conducted in conscious animals.

FIGURE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 10. Brainstem regions that relay vestibular signals to the RVLM. This neural circuit diagram is based on the findings of both anatomical (1821, 3942) and electrophysiologic (21, 43) studies that mapped connections in the vestibulo-sympathetic pathway. Red text and arrows indicate afferent inputs, and blue text and arrows denote efferent (output) pathways from the central nervous system. CVLM, caudal ventrolateral medulla; IML, sympathetic intermediolateral cell column; LTF, lateral tegmental field; NTS, nucleus tractus solitarius; RVLM, rostral ventrolateral medulla.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by University of Pittsburgh Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.

Author Contributions

JB, ND, JS, AM, DM, PA, CM, and BY conducted the experiments. JB, ND, JS, AM, DM, PA, CM, SB, and BY analyzed the data, generated figures, revised the manuscript, and approved the final version. JB and BY wrote the original manuscript draft. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by NIH grants R01-DC013788 and R01-DC018229. AM received support from NIH grant K08-DC013571 and DM received support from NIH grant F32-DC015157.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Rutha Chivate, Asmita Joshi, Emmanuel Kambouroglos, Robert Khorami, John Lausch, Christian Molfetto, Bridget Perry, Sophie Tayade, Anisha Venkatesh, and Samuel Wittman for their assistance with data collection and analysis. A preliminary report of these findings has been published on the bioRxiv preprint server (29).

References

1. Spyer KM. Annual review prize lecture - central nervous mechanisms contributing to cardiovascular control. J Physiol. (1994) 474:1–19. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1994.sp019997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Guyenet PG, Koshiya N, Huangfu D, Baraban SC, Stornetta RL, Li YW. Role of medulla oblongata in generation of sympathetic and vagal outflows. In: Holstege Bandler GR, CB Saper, editors. Emotional Motor System. Amsterdam: Elsevier (1996). p. 127–44.

Google Scholar

3. Dampney RA. Functional organization of central pathways regulating the cardiovascular system. Physiol Rev. (1994) 74:323–64. doi: 10.1152/physrev.1994.74.2.323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Barman SM, Yates BJ. Deciphering the neural control of sympathetic nerve activity: status report and directions for future research. Front Neurosci. (2017) 11:730. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00730

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Reis DJ, Ruggiero DA, Morrison SF. The C1 area of the rostral ventrolateral medulla oblongata a critical brainstem region for control of resting and reflex integration of arterial pressure. Am J Hypertens. (1989) 2:363S−74. doi: 10.1093/ajh/2.12.363S

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Stornetta RL. Neurochemistry of bulbospinal presympathetic neurons of the medulla oblongata. J Chem Neuroanat. (2009) 38:222–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jchemneu.2009.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Barman SM, Sugiyama Y, Suzuki T, Cotter LA, DeStefino VJ, Reighard DA, et al. Rhythmic activity of neurons in the rostral ventrolateral medulla of conscious cats: effect of removal of vestibular inputs. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. (2011) 301:R937–46. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00265.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Destefino VJ, Reighard DA, Sugiyama Y, Suzuki T, Cotter LA, Larson MG, et al. Responses of neurons in the rostral ventrolateral medulla to whole body rotations: comparisons in decerebrate and conscious cats. J Appl Physiol. (2011) 110:1699–707. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00180.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Miller DM, Joshi A, Kambouroglos ET, Engstrom IC, Bielanin JP, Wittman SR, et al. Responses of neurons in the rostral ventrolateral medulla of conscious cats to anticipated and passive movements. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. (2020) 318:R481–92. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00205.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Yates BJ, Bolton PS, Macefield VG. Vestibulo-sympathetic responses. Compr Physiol. (2014) 4:851–87. doi: 10.1002/cphy.c130041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Yates BJ, Goto T, Bolton PS. Responses of neurons in the rostral ventrolateral medulla of the cat to natural vestibular stimulation. Brain Res. (1993) 601:255–64. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(93)91718-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Wilson TD, Cotter LA, Draper JA, Misra SP, Rice CD, Cass SP, et al. Vestibular inputs elicit patterned changes in limb blood flow in conscious cats. J Physiol. (2006) 575:671–84. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.112904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Goldberg JM, Wilson VJ, Cullen KE, Angelaki DE, Broussard DM, Büttner-Ennever JA, et al. The Vestibular System: A Sixth Sense. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (2012).

Google Scholar

14. Gebber GL, Barman SM. Lateral tegmental field neurons of cat medulla: a potential source of basal sympathetic nerve discharge. J Neurophysiol. (1985) 54:1498–512. doi: 10.1152/jn.1985.54.6.1498

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Orer HS, Gebber GL, Phillips SW, Barman SM. Role of the medullary lateral tegmental field in reflex-mediated sympathoexcitation in cats. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. (2004) 286:R451–64. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00569.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Orer HS, Barman SM, Gebber GL, Sykes SM. Medullary lateral tegmental field: an important synaptic relay in the baroreceptor reflex pathway of the cat. Am J Physiol. (1999) 277:R1462–75. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1999.277.5.R1462

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Barman SM, Gebber GL, Orer HS. Medullary lateral tegmental field: an important source of basal sympathetic nerve discharge in the cat. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. (2000) 278:R995–1004. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.2000.278.4.R995

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Balaban CD, Beryozkin G. Vestibular nucleus projections to nucleus tractus solitarius and the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus nerve: potential substrates for vestibulo-autonomic interactions. Exp Brain Res. (1994) 98:200–12. doi: 10.1007/BF00228409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Yates BJ, Grelot L, Kerman IA, Balaban CD, Jakus J, Miller AD. Organization of vestibular inputs to nucleus tractus solitarius and adjacent structures in cat brain stem. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. (1994) 267:R974–83. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1994.267.4.R974

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Gagliuso AH, Chapman EK, Martinelli GP, Holstein GR. Vestibular neurons with direct projections to the solitary nucleus in the rat. J Neurophysiol. (2019) 122:512–24. doi: 10.1152/jn.00082.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Yates BJ, Balaban CD, Miller AD, Endo K, Yamaguchi Y. Vestibular inputs to the lateral tegmental field of the cat: potential role in autonomic control. Brain Res. (1995) 689:197–206. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)00569-C

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Sugiyama Y, Suzuki T, DeStefino VJ, Yates BJ. Integrative responses of neurons in nucleus tractus solitarius to visceral afferent stimulation and vestibular stimulation in vertical planes. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. (2011) 301:R1380–90. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00361.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Moy JD, Miller DJ, Catanzaro MF, Boyle BM, Ogburn SW, Cotter LA, et al. Responses of neurons in the caudal medullary lateral tegmental field to visceral inputs and vestibular stimulation in vertical planes. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. (2012) 303:R929–40. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00356.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. McCall AA, Moy JD, DeMayo WM, Puterbaugh SR, Miller DJ, Catanzaro MF, et al. Processing of vestibular inputs by the medullary lateral tegmental field of conscious cats: implications for generation of motion sickness. Exp Brain Res. (2013) 225:349–59. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3376-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Fadel PJ, Raven PB. Human investigations into the arterial and cardiopulmonary baroreflexes during exercise. Exp Physiol. (2012) 97:39–50. doi: 10.1113/expphysiol.2011.057554

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Gandevia SC, Killian K, McKenzie DK, Crawford M, Allen GM, Gorman RB, et al. Respiratory sensations, cardiovascular control, kinaesthesia and transcranial stimulation during paralysis in humans. J Physiol. (1993) 470:85–107. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1993.sp019849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Waldrop TG, Eldridge FL, Iwamoto GA, Mitchell JH. Central neural control of respiration and circulation during exercise. In: Rowell LB, Shepherd JT, editors. Handbook of Physiology, Section 12, Exercise: Regulation and Integration of Multiple Systems. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (1996). p. 334–80.

Google Scholar

28. Patel NM, Baker EAG, Wittman SR, Engstrom IC, Bourdages GH, McCall AA, et al. Cardiovascular adjustments during anticipated postural changes. Physiol Rep. (2018) 6:e13554. doi: 10.14814/phy2.13554

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Bielanin JP, Douglas NO, Shulgach JO, McCall AA, Miller DM, Amin PR, et al. Responses of neurons in the medullary lateral tegmental field and nucleus tractus solitarius to vestibular stimuli in conscious felines. bioRxiv [Preprint]. (2020). doi: 10.1101/2020.10.23.352542

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. National Research Council. Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. Washington, DC: National Academy Press (2011).

Google Scholar

31. McCall AA, Miller DM, DeMayo WM, Bourdages GH, Yates BJ. Vestibular nucleus neurons respond to hindlimb movement in the conscious cat. J Neurophysiol. (2016) 116:1785–94. doi: 10.1152/jn.00414.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Arshian MS, Hobson CE, Catanzaro MF, Miller DJ, Puterbaugh SR, Cotter LA, et al. Vestibular nucleus neurons respond to hindlimb movement in the decerebrate cat. J. Neurophysiol. (2014) 111:2423–32. doi: 10.1152/jn.00855.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Miller DM, Cotter LA, Gandhi NJ, Schor RH, Cass SP, Huff NO, et al. Responses of caudal vestibular nucleus neurons of conscious cats to rotations in vertical planes, before and after a bilateral vestibular neurectomy. Exp Brain Res. (2008) 188:175–86. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1359-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Dum RP, Levinthal DJ, Strick PL. Motor, cognitive, and affective areas of the cerebral cortex influence the adrenal medulla. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2016) 113:9922–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1605044113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Dum RP, Levinthal DJ, Strick PL. The mind-body problem: circuits that link the cerebral cortex to the adrenal medulla. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2019) 116:26321–28. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1902297116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Clancy JA, Johnson R, Raw R, Deuchars SA, Deuchars J. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the motor cortex increases sympathetic nerve activity. Brain Stimul. (2014) 7:97–104. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2013.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Cotter LA, Arendt HE, Jasko JG, Sprando C, Cass SP, Yates BJ. Effects of postural changes and vestibular lesions on diaphragm and rectus abdominis activity in awake cats. J Appl Physiol. (2001) 91:137–44. doi: 10.1152/jappl.2001.91.1.137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Yates BJ, Catanzaro MF, Miller DJ, McCall AA. Integration of vestibular and emetic gastrointestinal signals that produce nausea and vomiting: potential contributions to motion sickness. Exp Brain Res. (2014) 232:2455–69. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-3937-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Holstein GR, Friedrich VL Jr, Martinelli GP. Imidazoleacetic acid-ribotide in vestibulo-sympathetic pathway neurons. Exp Brain Res. (2016) 234:2747–60. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4725-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Holstein GR, Friedrich VL Jr, Martinelli GP. Projection neurons of the vestibulo-sympathetic reflex pathway. J Comp Neurol. (2014) 522:2053–74. doi: 10.1002/cne.23517

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Holstein GR, Friedrich VL Jr, Kang T, Kukielka E, Martinelli GP. Direct projections from the caudal vestibular nuclei to the ventrolateral medulla in the rat. Neuroscience. (2011) 175:104–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Stocker SD, Steinbacher BC, Balaban CD, Yates BJ. Connections of the caudal ventrolateral medullary reticular formation in the cat brainstem. Exp Brain Res. (1997) 116:270–82. doi: 10.1007/PL00005755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Steinbacher BC, Yates BJ. Processing of vestibular and other inputs by the caudal ventrolateral medullary reticular formation. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. (1996) 271:R1070–7. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1996.271.4.R1070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: vestibular, cardiovascular regulation, vestibulo-sympathetic responses, rostral ventrolateral medulla, nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS), lateral tegmental field, sympathetic nervous system, reticular formation

Citation: Bielanin JP, Douglas NO, Shulgach JA, McCall AA, Miller DM, Amin PR, Murphey CP, Barman SM and Yates BJ (2020) Responses of Neurons in the Medullary Lateral Tegmental Field and Nucleus Tractus Solitarius to Vestibular Stimuli in Conscious Felines. Front. Neurol. 11:620817. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.620817

Received: 23 October 2020; Accepted: 30 November 2020;
Published: 18 December 2020.

Edited by:

Michael Strupp, Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich, Germany

Reviewed by:

Andrei V. Derbenev, Tulane University, United States
Sergei B. Yakushin, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, United States

Copyright © 2020 Bielanin, Douglas, Shulgach, McCall, Miller, Amin, Murphey, Barman and Yates. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Bill J. Yates, byates@pitt.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.