Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 25 October 2023
Sec. Pathological Conditions
Volume 17 - 2023 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2023.1243572

Similarities and differences regarding acute anorexia nervosa and semi-starvation: does behavioral thermoregulation play a central role?

  • Human Performance Laboratory, Department of Kinesiology, School of Health Sciences, East Carolina University, Greenville, NC, United States

Objective: To clarify the association between acute anorexia nervosa (AN) and semi-starvation (SS) by focusing on similarities and differences in physiology, mood, and behavior.

Method: A comparison of published literature between these two groups.

Results: Both groups show similar hormonal and metabolic changes in response to caloric restriction and extreme weight loss (~25%). Associated changes result in a reduced body temperature (Tcore-low). Maintenance of body temperature within a specific range is crucial to survival. However, both groups cannot activate autonomic strategies to maintain their Tcore-low, such as increasing metabolic rate, constricting skin blood vessels, or shivering. Furthermore, Tcore-low increases the individuals’ “coldness sensations” throughout the body, hence the frequent reports from ANs and SSs of “feeling cold.” To eliminate these uncomfortable “coldness sensations” and, importantly, to maintain Tcore-low, ANs, and SSs “select” different thermoregulatory behavioral strategies. It is proposed that the primary differences between AN and SS, based on genetics, now manifest due to the “selection” of different thermo-regulatory behaviors. AN patients (ANs) “select” hyperactive behavior (HyAc), which increases internal metabolic heat and thus assists with maintaining Tcore-low; in harmony with hyperactive behavior is a lively mood. Also related to this elevated arousal pattern, ANs experience disrupted sleep. In contrast, SS individuals “select” a passive thermo-behavioral strategy, “shallow torpor,” which includes reduced activity, resulting in energy conservation. In addition, this inactivity aids in the retention of generated metabolic heat. Corresponding to this lethargic behavior, SS individuals display a listless mood and increased sleep.

Conclusion: Initial similarities between the two are attributable to physiological changes related to extreme weight loss. Differences are most likely attributable to genetically programmed “selection” of alternate thermoregulatory strategies, primarily to maintain Tcore-low. However, if acute AN is prolonged and evolves into a chronic condition, AN will more closely align with starvation and more precisely reflect SS symptomology.

1. Introduction

Anorexia Nervosa (AN) is a condition wherein an individual, usually 1–4% of females (Watson et al., 2019), self-imposes severe food restrictions and loses up to 25% of body weight. Over the decades, AN has been regarded principally as a psychogenic condition, but one with severe physiological consequences (Pirke, 1987; Dalle Grave et al., 2008; Treasure et al., 2015; Calugi et al., 2018; Watson et al., 2019). A related condition is semi-starvation (SS); this, too, manifests due to a striking reduction in food consumption and dramatic weight loss (Keys et al., 1950; Fichter et al., 1986). However, SS is typically in response to uncontrolled circumstances, such as famine, and is usually not willfully self-imposed. Furthermore, unlike AN, SS is regarded predominantly as a physiological condition with associated psychological and behavioral changes (Keys et al., 1950).

Many researchers have questioned whether AN represents a unique condition or whether AN reflects the sequelae associated with semi-starvation (Mecklenburg et al., 1974; Casper and Davis, 1977; Calugi et al., 2018; Phillipou et al., 2018; Frank et al., 2019; Casper et al., 2020a). Casper and Davis (1977) suggested that “the effects of starvation are more intimately involved in the symptoms of anorexia nervosa than has heretofore been described.” According to Garner (Garner, 1997), “One of the most important advancements in the understanding of eating disorders is the recognition that severe and prolonged dietary restriction can lead to serious physical and psychological complications. Many of the symptoms once thought to be primary features of anorexia nervosa are actually symptoms of starvation.”

To better understand and thus improve the treatment of AN, Phillipou et al., (2018) have suggested that it is crucial to determine which signs and symptoms are explicitly related to AN and which represent the condition of semi-starvation. The present paper will attempt to unravel these conditions by assessing the similarities and differences between AN and SS (Casper and Davis, 1977) and determining whether acute AN represents a unique condition with distinctive psychological and physiological parameters, whether it reflects semi-starvation and its sequelae, or is a combination of these two conditions (Tyszkiewicz-Nwafor et al., 2020).

The investigation of AN focuses primarily on published literature on acute-restrictive AN (Casper and Davis, 1977; Carrera et al., 2012), implying that the individual is close to the time of onset of significant weight loss (Casper, 2018). However, it is difficult to verify when exactly the AN condition manifested since, typically, many months or even years pass between the earliest changes in eating, the dramatic weight loss, and the formal diagnosis (Kay, 1953; Scolnick and Mostofsky, 2014; Frostad and Bentz, 2022).

Regarding SSs, the emphasis will be on the classic Minnesota Study of Semi-Starvation (MinnSS) conducted in 1943, during World War II (WWII). Keys and colleagues (Keys et al., 1950) selected 36 average-weight, healthy, intelligent, psychologically well-adjusted, young white males who could get along reasonably well under trying conditions. The study consisted of:

• a 3-month control period during which subjects maintained average body weight,

• a 6-month semi-starvation period (~50% of previous caloric intake), and

• a 3-month weight recovery period.

This paper will focus on the 6-month semi-starvation period, during which subjects lost ~25% of body weight, similar to losses seen in AN. A team of medical doctors monitored these subjects to understand behavioral, physiological, psychological, and social changes. In addition to the dramatic reduction in food intake, subjects were required to participate in the same scheduled activities for the entire year; the purpose was to simulate wartime conditions.

Many notable differences exist between the MinnSS study and AN (Keys et al., 1950). However, this remains the most comprehensive “control group” available. It provides impressive information and could not be conducted nowadays for ethical reasons.

2. The initiation of the starvation response in ANs and SSs: leptin

Leptin is a hormone synthesized and released from fat tissue with its main action occurring in the CNS/hypothalamus (Jequier, 2002; Muller et al., 2009; Hebebrand et al., 2022). Blood levels are usually proportional to the amount of body fat. In this manner, it acts as a messenger molecule to inform the brain of the status of fat energy reserves in the body (Jequier, 2002; Muller et al., 2009).

At the onset of caloric restriction and weight loss, blood leptin levels decrease rapidly and dramatically (Seoane-Collazo et al., 2020); these decreased levels play a major role in adapting the organism to starvation (Ahima et al., 1996; Hebebrand et al., 2023). Reduced leptin levels signal the hypothalamus of imminent “danger” related to reduced food availability (Ahima et al., 1996; Muller et al., 2009; Hebebrand et al., 2022). The hypothalamus-pituitary-target organ axes, in addition to other changes (Hebebrand et al., 2019), now increase or decrease the synthesis and secretion of many energy-intensive hormones (Misra and Klibanski, 2014; Schorr and Miller, 2017):

• It decreases the synthesis of reproductive hormones, thus suppressing reproductive-related activities (Casper and Davis, 1977; Misra and Klibanski, 2014).

• It decreases the release of thyroid-stimulating hormone and the activity of the sympathetic nervous system, which, among other functions, reduces metabolic rate (Fichter et al., 1986; Misra and Klibanski, 2014; Schorr and Miller, 2017).

• It reduces the synthesis of insulin-like growth factor, which then inhibits growth and development (Fichter et al., 1986; Schorr and Miller, 2017) in the face of food scarcity and thus assists with extending survival (Devlin, 2011).

These reduced energy needs for growth and reproduction diminish the organisms’ need to draw on the primary energy source, stored fat reserves.

Regarding ANs, leptin levels are consistently lower, by as much as ~75% compared to HC (ECKERT et al., 1998; HAAS et al., 2005; Dostalova et al., 2007; Karageorgiou et al., 2020). Also, depending on the degree and severity of the caloric deficit, leptin levels may be undetectable (Hebebrand et al., 1997).

Hebebrand et al. (2022) suggest that off-label treatment with recombinant human leptin (metreleptin) may alleviate many of the emotional, cognitive, and associated behavioral symptoms reflective of AN. They also propose that this treatment could possibly break the vicious cycle that sustains this condition (Hebebrand et al., 2023). Milos et al. (2020) treated three severely anorexic patients; this short-term treatment proved partially successful.

However, the use of this treatment remains controversial. Casper (2022) acknowledges that short-term high doses of metreleptin mitigated feelings of depression, inner tension, intrusive thoughts of food, and the urge to be physically active. Yet when treatment was terminated, this had little influence on the patients’ personal commitment to remain at a low weight. Fraga et al. (2020), using rats, argued that ambient temperature was more effective in reducing the amount of running and weight loss than metreleptin. Placebo-controlled clinical trials are needed to confirm or refute the usefulness of metreleptin in treating AN (Hebebrand et al., 2019).

Regarding SS: there is a similar significant decrease in blood leptin levels in obese subjects, and normal-weight males and females due to famine, fasting, or willful caloric restriction. This decrease varies from 44 to 83% (Boden et al., 1996; Considine et al., 1996; Chan et al., 2003; Muller M.J. et al., 2015).

In summary, weight loss in ANs and SSs rapidly decreases blood leptin levels, altering the hormonal profile and preparing the body to withstand starvation. Specifically, there is a decrease in hormones that typically promote increased energy utilization, such as growth and reproduction. It has also been proposed that administering a leptin analog may induce psychological and behavioral changes to assist in recovery; however, this awaits clarification.

3. The maintenance of body temperature in HCs, ANs, and SSs

3.1. Healthy controls

Most Healthy controls (HCs) maintain a body core temperature (Tcore) of 37°C regardless of race, gender, and geographic location. Sustaining this level within a narrow range is critical to survival as it optimizes bodily functions and prevents cellular damage (Rising et al., 1992). Sustaining Tcore involves the use of internal metabolic heat, which is a by-product of all energy-producing processes (Landsberg, 2012). This heat is then retained in the body by constricting surface/skin blood vessels and insulating body fat. If sufficient metabolic heat is not produced and retained, “emergency” strategies may become activated. These include shivering, activation of specialized heat-producing brown adipose tissue, and piloerection (goosebumps). However, these emergency strategies are energy demanding. Thus, HCs will initially preferably engage in a more straightforward strategy, thermoregulatory behavior. Such behaviors include wearing a sweater or turning up a thermostat, to name a few. In endotherms, thermo-behavior is the most effective and frequently utilized means of maintaining Tcore (Romanovsky, 2007; Flouris, 2011). Thermo–behavior is our first line of thermal defense to prevent changes in core temperature (Schlader, 2014).

3.2. An and Tcore-low

Unlike HCs, numerous studies have demonstrated that ANs have a significantly lower Tcore. These low values range from 35.4°C (Mecklenburg et al., 1974; Rising et al., 1992) to 36.6°C (Davies et al., 1979); this will be referred to as Tcore-low instead of hypothermia since hypothermia implies a Tcore below 35°C. It is also suggested that this Tcore-low represents a new set point for the ANs; it does not threaten survival but is crucial for them to maintain this level to prevent further reductions, which may become life-threatening (Smith, 2021).

The primary cause of a reduced Tcore-low is a significantly lower metabolic rate, which is a key provider of body heat and thus helps maintain internal body temperature. The basal metabolic rate for ANs is reduced by approximately 21% (Polito et al., 2000; Onur et al., 2005) to 32% (Bossu et al., 2007) compared to HCs; thus, they cannot produce sufficient heat to maintain the normal 37°C.

Furthermore, Oishi et al. (2013) using mice, have demonstrated that fasting and the induction of ketosis, in the presence of hypothermia, may further impact body temperature. Since ANs are in metabolic ketosis (McCue, 2010) in the presence of Tcore-low, it is possible that this further exacerbates a reduction in body temperature.

In addition to the reduced ability to produce body heat, ANs cannot effectively retain this heat due to their inability to constrict surface blood vessels and due to having less insulating fat (Chudecka and Lubkowska, 2016). Furthermore, they cannot activate secondary backup thermoregulatory responses such as shivering, piloerection, or activation of brown fat (Vigersky et al., 1977; Nishita et al., 1986; Bredella et al., 2012).

However, like most humans, they can initiate the crucial tactic of thermoregulatory behavior ; many different thermo-behavioral strategies are available. It has been proposed that the primary thermo-behavioral strategy employed by ANs is the generation of internal metabolic heat through muscle movement (Gutierrez and Vazquez, 2001; Smith, 2021). This behavior is most likely a result of genetic programming; AN is now considered a psycho-metabolic condition, with as much as 76% of this condition being genetically determined (Bulik et al., 2019).

In support of the hypothesis that ANs generate heat through movement, is extensive evidence demonstrating that most ANs engage in excessive amounts of physical activity (Beumont et al., 1994; Davis et al., 1994, 1997; Alberti et al., 2013; El Ghoch et al., 2013; Gummer et al., 2015; Casper, 2018). The generated metabolic heat (Vargas et al., 2019) assists with maintaining or increasing Tcore-low; it also aids the perfusion of warm blood to the extremities, especially the hands (Vigersky et al., 1976), a primary area for the detection of cold sensations (Smith, 2021).

Although many have claimed that the main reason for excessive movement in AN is to enhance weight loss (Kiezebrink et al., 2009), several rodent studies have suggested otherwise. They have demonstrated that in a rat model referred to as activity-based anorexia, excessive running, when paired with food restriction and weight loss, represents a drive to generate body heat to assist with maintaining Tcore-low (Hillebrand et al., 2005; Gutierrez et al., 2006, 2008). It is proposed that the ANs’ excessive activity represents a similar function (Gutierrez and Vazquez, 2001; Smith, 2021). This thermo-behavioral strategy will be referred to as hyperactivity (HyAc); a more detailed discussion will follow (Section 5.1).

3.3. SSs and Tcore-low

Like ANs, SSs have a reduced Tcore and metabolic rate (Keys et al., 1950; Taylor and Keys, 1950; Tucker, 2006); furthermore, they too are in a state of metabolic ketosis (McCue, 2010); if hypothermic, this too could lower body temperature (Oishi et al., 2013). It is assumed that they cannot generate the backup thermal responses of shivering, piloerection, and activation of brown adipose tissue, although this has not been investigated. But, like ANs and other humans, they can “select” specific thermoregulatory behaviors. It is proposed that their central strategy is the converse of ANs. SSs “select” a more passive strategy that focuses on retaining whatever internal heat has been generated and passively gaining external environmental heat. This is verified by Keys et al. (1950) focusing on the behavior of their semi-starved subjects and using anecdotal observations from famine and prisoner-of-war camps. This behavioral pattern will be called shallow torpor (See Section 6.1).

In summary, it is hypothesized that the primary difference between ANs and SSs is their “selection” of different thermo-behavioral strategies (Barakat et al., 2023) most likely genetically determined. So, while ANs engage in HyAc to generate internal metabolic heat, SSs “select” a more inactive strategy that focuses on retaining whatever internal heat has been generated and passively gaining external environmental heat.

4. “Feeling cold” initiates engagement in thermoregulatory behavior

4.1. ANs and SSs “feel cold”

“Feeling cold” has been a prominent complaint for ANs (Swenne and Engstrom, 2005; Calugi et al., 2018) and SSs (Keys et al., 1950). Surprisingly, this does not appear to be consciously related to changes in the body Tcore-low (Jacquot et al., 2014; Vargas et al., 2018). Instead, feelings of body coldness are derived from sensations in the peripheral skin (Romanovsky, 2014). These “unpleasant” perceptions are then transmitted to the brain/hypothalamus, motivating engagement in thermo-behavior to lessen this discomfort (Flouris, 2011; Jacquot et al., 2014; Vargas et al., 2018).

4.2. ANs and cold sensations

Many researchers have reported that cold intolerance is a common complaint among ANs and is usually unrelated to ambient temperature (Lampert and Lau, 1976; Luck and Wakeling, 1980; Schwabe et al., 1981; Silverman, 1983; Crisp, 1984a; Bhanji and Mattingly, 1991; Kurklinsky et al., 2011; Carrera et al., 2012; Das and Maiti, 2013). In addition to these overall feelings of coldness, ANs complain especially of having cold hands (Nishita et al., 1986). “Fingers, toes, hands, and feet were usually blue and cold” (Lampert and Lau, 1976).

Recently it has been demonstrated that the hand skin temperature of ANs is the only skin surface area significantly lower (~2°C) than that of HCs. Surprisingly, all other surface areas of the body are the same or warmer than that of HCs, implying that metabolic heat cannot be effectively retained (Chudecka and Lubkowska, 2016). Thus, the hands are most likely the primary source for generating overall body thermal sensations (Vargas et al., 2018; Smith, 2021), with the skin temperature of the wrist being the best predictor of thermal sensations and of motivation to initiate thermoregulatory behavior (Jacquot et al., 2014).

4.3. SSs and cold sensations

Like ANs, SSs suffer from extreme sensations of coldness. Keys et al. (1950) reported that cold temperatures were poorly tolerated during the starvation phase of the MinSS, despite the room’s adequate heating. “Many subjects slept under heavy blankets and wore extra clothing during the day in hot summer weather.” In addition, subjects always requested that their food, coffee, and tea be served unusually hot. To feel warm, “all the men had learned to have long, hot showers “(Franklin et al., 1948), and when possible, would bask in the sun (Tucker, 2006), and gather about the steam radiators (Keys et al., 1950).

In another food-restricted study, the Carnegie study (Keys et al., 1950), the researchers commented on the subjects’ extreme sensitivity to cold. They noted that these men’s hand temperature, like ANs, was approximately 2°C lower than that of HCs.

So, both ANs and SSs experience profound sensations of coldness, especially in the extremities, which act as “bait” to induce thermo-behavior and thus reduce this discomfort. Simultaneously, and most importantly, this thermo-behavior assists with maintaining Tcore-low. It will now be proposed that the “selection” of thermo-behavior differentiates ANs from SSs. However, before discussing these differences, it should be noted that both groups display additional behaviors that are similar, especially those related to hunger and eating (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Additional similarities related to hunger, eating behavior, self-mutilation, and libido.

5. ANs and “selection” of thermo-behavior which coordinates with mood and sleep

5.1. ANs and hyperactivity

It is well established that as many as 78% (Davis et al., 1994) of ANs engage in excessive amounts of physical activity during the acute phase of the disorder (Beumont et al., 1994; Davis et al., 1994, 1997; Alberti et al., 2013; El Ghoch et al., 2013; Gummer et al., 2015; Casper, 2018). This surprising display of stamina continues even after significant weight loss (Vigersky et al., 1976; Casper et al., 2020). Many researchers have claimed that the primary reason for this HyAc is due to the relentless pursuit of weight loss (Kiezebrink et al., 2009); however, this has been disputed (Gutierrez and Birmingham, 2020).

To illustrate this excessive engagement in physical activity, Davis et al. (1994) administered questionnaires to 45 ANs and reported the following: 78% described their physical activity during the acute phase of their disorder as “excessive;” 93% of the patients described their exercise as “out of control” and “compulsive;” 75% reported that during the period of maximum weight loss, physical activity increased as their weight and food intake decreased. There were frequent comments from the ANs, such as “All I was doing was exercising” and “The lower my weight got, the more energy I had.” Furthermore, this increased urge to move continued despite increased levels of physical fatigue (Casper et al., 2020).

Many terms have been used to describe this excessive activity (Davis et al., 1999; Meyer et al., 2008, 2011). However, the terminology used here, hyperactivity (HyAc), will reflect the total daily activity of ANs (Smith, 2021) and includes three overlapping categories:

• Excessive Exercise (ExEx), involves “formal” exercise such as walking/jogging, weight training, volleyball, yoga, etc.

• Non-Exercise Activity Thermogenesis (NEAT) includes all movements during awake periods (Levine, 2007), such as performing daily chores, attending school/work, etc.

• Spontaneous Physical Activity (SPA) involves fidgety, restless behavior (Belak et al., 2017).

ANs find many ways of producing daily movement in excess. The execution of all these movements produces body heat to varying degrees. The generated metabolic heat now assists with Tcore-low (Gutierrez and Vazquez, 2001; Smith, 2021).

In addition to HyAc to maintain Tcore-low, ANs use other means to maintain/increase body temperature. This may include wearing extra clothing, sitting close to heaters, and drinking warm fluids (Strumia, 2013).

Furthermore, HyAc may produce added benefits (Garland et al., 2011; Giel et al., 2013). These include reduced appetite, decreased anxiety, prevention or reduction of depressive-like behavior, and increased feelings of well-being (Scheurink et al., 2010; Hicks et al., 2019).

Although it has been stressed that HyAc is primarily a strategy to maintain Tcore-low (Gutierrez and Vazquez, 2001; Smith, 2021) many view HyAc in ANs as an ancient behavioral pattern aimed at encouraging foraging or migration during times of food shortages (Casper et al., 1991; Guisinger, 2003; Scolnick and Mostofsky, 2014). However, engaging in HyAc for foraging and maintaining Tcore-low are not mutually exclusive. If the ancient AN felt compelled to move, this would have maintained Tcore-low and enhanced her chances of finding food.

Using a well-established rodent model, the activity-based anorexia model (ABA), it has been demonstrated that providing heat to the “anorexic” ABA-rodent reduces activity and weight loss (Routtenberg and Kuznesof, 1967; Epling and Pierce, 1996; Hillebrand et al., 2005; Gutierrez et al., 2006; Vazquez et al., 2006; Roura et al., 2020).

Applying translational reasoning, it has been hypothesized that providing heat to ANs, may reduce the compulsion to move and thus assist with weight gain (Gutierrez and Vazquez, 2001; Smith, 2021). However, the effectiveness of heat application in human ANs, remains equivocal.

Gull (1997) was the first to report that warming the spine, wearing warm clothing, and being kept in a warm bed for a time would promote recovery in ANs. In 2001 Gutierrez and Vasquez (Gutierrez and Vazquez, 2001) successfully treated three hyperactive ANs on an outpatient basis using three different heat treatments. All three patients showed an immediate reduction in overactivity. When assessed 30 months later, BMI for all three had improved dramatically, and activity patterns remained reduced. However, these were case reports with no control group. Additionally, compliance could not be assessed because the study was conducted on an outpatient basis.

In a Randomized Control Trial (RCT), Bergh et al. (2002) assigned 13 ANs to a Treatment group. The treatment consisted of using a computerized device (a Mandometer) during lunchtime to monitor food intake, rate of eating, and satiety. In addition, they supplied external heat by having subjects rest in a warm room (up to 40°C) for 1 h after lunch and confined subjects to a wheelchair or allowed them to walk slowly in the clinic. The Control group (n = 16) received no treatment. The outcome was significantly different between the treated and control groups. Fourteen patients in the treatment group went into remission after a median of 14.4 months, and 75% remained in remission at 12 months. They concluded that their treatment had a beneficial effect but also suggested that it was unclear what contribution each modality made (Mandometer, heat, reduced movement?). In a subsequent study, Zandian et al. (2017) reported that applying heat (room at 32°C) for 30 min after lunch significantly reduced anxiety.

Birmingham et al. (2004) tested the hypothesis that heat would increase the rate of weight gain. They randomized 21 subjects to a Heat or Control Group and applied heat for ~4 h daily for 21 days using a heated vest. Subjects reported enjoying the heating experience but did not show an improvement in eating behavior. Surprisingly, exercise behavior was not monitored, so whether heat impacted physical activity is unknown. In addition, the average BMI of these subjects was 17.5, suggesting a borderline condition. Also, these subjects would be considered chronic ANs since AN had lasted for approximately 13.6 years. These and other design factors may have confounded their results.

Cerrato et al. (2012) hypothesized that ambient/seasonal temperature would impact the amount of hyperactivity in acute AN; they monitored daily movement using an accelerometer for 3 consecutive days. They assigned 15 ANs to a Warm Group between Spring and Summer (April – October), during which the mean ambient temperature was 16°C. Activity levels of the Cold group were monitored between Autumn and Winter (October–April) with a mean ambient temperature of 4.5°C. The results demonstrated that the Cold Group was significantly more physically active than the Warm Group (p = 0.003). They concluded that keeping patients warm may be a beneficial treatment option for reducing hyperactivity, which is typically refractory to treatment.

The following research design is proposed to address the question of whether applying heat 24/7 for an extended period (possibly 3 weeks–3 months) would assist with rehabilitation by increasing the ANs Tcore-low, reducing aspects of HyAc, and resulting in a spontaneous increase in food intake.

On admittance to a clinic, ANs would be randomly assigned to an experimental (EXP) or control (C) group. EXP ANs would engage in bed rest for most of the day/night, using an electric blanket and keeping the blanket’s temperature as high as is comfortable; ambient room temperature should also be elevated. In addition, they should be allowed to sit (with the heated blanket) and move around the clinic to a limited extent (partly to assess spontaneous movement and reduce musculoskeletal deterioration that occurs with bed rest). After baseline measurements are assessed on day 1, the following dependent variables should be continuously monitored in the EXP:

• Tcore using ingested capsules (Bongers et al., 2018).

• Wrist skin temperature using a surface thermistor (Jacquot et al., 2014).

• A multi-sensor monitor (Sensewear Mini Armband) to monitor sleep patterns (Martinez-Sanchez et al., 2020; Hebebrand et al., 2023).

• A pedometer to monitor the amount of spontaneous walking.

• A heart rate monitor to ensure that ANs do not surreptitiously engage in moderate/high-intensity exercise.

• A modified shoe-based accelerometer to monitor foot and hand fidgeting (Belak et al., 2017).

• Three meals a day and snacks should be provided, and amounts eaten be recorded for spontaneous ingestion.

• An indwelling catheter to monitor daily changes in circulating hormones such as leptin and ghrelin, as well as reproductive and metabolic hormones.

• Assessment of body weight.

• Use of appropriate psychological questionnaires.

The C would be exposed to a treatment regime typical for that particular clinic. In addition, daily, Tcore-low, wrist skin temperature, total amount of movement, and food intake should be recorded.

The statistical analyses could involve a one-way ANOVA of the EXP group as well as a correlational analysis and multiple regression using all dependent variables. A 2-way ANOVA could be used to compare the results of the EXP and C groups.

In summary, it is proposed that the primary purpose of HyAc is to maintain Tcore-low. In addition, other positive benefits, such as reduced appetite and increased anxiolysis, also accrue; this assists in reinforcing HyAc. However, when viewed in terms of ancient times, HyAc also represents foraging/migration behavior, increasing the ANs’ chance of locating food (Guisinger, 2003). Regarding the effectiveness of heat as a treatment for ANs, definitive research is needed to determine whether heat application 24/7 in human ANs, initially reported by Gutierrez and Vazquez (2001) would produce a similar outcome as is seen in the ABA model (Gutierrez et al., 2002; Birmingham et al., 2004; Hillebrand et al., 2005).

5.2. Mood/attitude and psychological comorbidities during the acute phase of AN

The buoyant mood and feelings of well-being of the ANs correspond to her hypermobility, although, such behavior appears disproportionate to such severe weight loss (Casper and Davis, 1977; Casper, 2006, 2016, 2018). ANs seem to lack awareness of fatigue and weakness in the acute phase and insist on not being tired (Crisp, 1967; Casper, 2006; Hebebrand et al., 2023). Most maintain they never felt better (Casper, 1998). Until weight loss is profound, they deny feeling ill or fatigued (Vigersky et al., 1976) and are contented with their physical state (Casper and Heller, 1991). They also lack concern regarding the seriousness of the health risks of extreme weight loss (Casper and Heller, 1991; Gull, 1997). They continue to function by attending school and work, and many become enthusiastically involved in numerous activities (Casper, 2018).

Many researchers/clinicians have ascribed psychological comorbidities, such as depression, as a precipitating factor in AN (Blinder et al., 2006). However, Crisp (Crisp, 1984b) suggests that the degree of depression in AN is about the same as that found in other populations and is well below that in depressive illness. This is supported by Delvenne et al. (1992) who concluded that there is no association between AN and depression.

Others have concluded that there is no neurosis specific to anorexia nervosa (Kay and Leigh, 1954) and no consistent precipitating psychological pathology before the onset of AN (Bergh et al., 2013). Furthermore, most ANs initially seek medical attention because of drastic weight loss and not for psychological support.

If any psychological symptoms manifest, these are preceded by weight loss (Scolnick, 2018). Sodersten et al. (2016b, 2019) suggest that any manifestation of psychological factors is related to the experience of semi-starvation since this also occurs in healthy individuals exposed to extreme food restrictions. “It is possible that there are some ANs who have co-morbid depression, anxiety, obsessions, compulsions, or delusions. However, none of the 100 s of ANs we have treated to recovery have had such co-morbid disorders – their mental symptoms resolve as their eating behavior normalizes” (Bergh et al., 2013). After full clinical recovery, most reach complete psychological recovery (Vigersky et al., 1977; Bergh et al., 2002; Bulik et al., 2007; Sodersten et al., 2016b).

In summary, if present, many co-morbid symptoms are predominantly a result of the starvation response; “there is no need to postulate that mental factors predispose individuals to the illness” (Ioakimidis et al., 2011). However, it is acknowledged that the question of co-morbidities associated with AN remains controversial (Misra and Klibanski, 2014; Barakat et al., 2023).

5.3. ANs and disrupted sleep

In keeping with their energetic behavior and animated mood, ANs are restless sleepers (Hebebrand et al., 2023). Sleep disturbances are part of their high arousal pattern (Crisp et al., 1971; Kleppe et al., 2023). They have trouble falling asleep (Crisp et al., 1971); they wake more frequently during the night (Crisp, 1967; Crisp et al., 1971); these awake periods are more prolonged than those of HC (Crisp et al., 1971; Lacey et al., 1976). They also have a lesser total amount of sleep (Crisp et al., 1971; Lacey et al., 1976; Delvenne et al., 1992; Malcolm et al., 2022) and have significantly less REM sleep (Crisp et al., 1971). Another striking feature is that they wake early (Crisp et al., 1971). This insomnia is associated with low body weight; weight gain significantly improves many aspects of sleep (Lacey et al., 1976). Unlike severely depressed patients, ANs rarely complain of sleep problems (Crisp, 1980).

In a narrative review, Hebebrand et al. (2023) supported the findings that the majority of individuals with eating disorders suffer from sleep disturbances. This was more pronounced with binging/purging type ANs than restrictive AN. In addition, the severity of the eating disorder and activity levels were associated with sleeping disorders. They further reported that administration of human recombinant leptin, metreleptin, to five ANs, substantially improved sleep quality within one to 2 days; however, more research is needed to verify its usefulness.

Interestingly, sleep is closely aligned with thermoregulation in healthy adults (Jequier, 2002; Parmeggiani, 2003; Szymusiak, 2018). Krauchi (2007) has demonstrated that HCs (mainly women) who have trouble falling asleep, frequently have cold hands; he refers to this as “vasospastic syndrome.” This may account for the ANs delay in falling asleep, as she, too, has cold hands (Chudecka and Lubkowska, 2016).

It is also suggested that frequent wakings, longer times remaining awake, and early morning arousal may also be related to thermoregulation and sensations of coldness (Krauchi, 2007; Sauchelli et al., 2016), as well as unrelenting hunger (Goldstein et al., 2018). Cold sensations would encourage movement to increase metabolic heat and thus bolster the all-important Tcore-low, while hunger sensations would further motivate the need to forage for food.

In summary, ANs are HyAc, with the primary survival focus being maintaining Tcore-low. Their mood is upbeat and reflects their energetic behavior; this is associated with a disrupted sleep pattern compared to HCs.

6. SSs and “selection” of thermoregulatory behavior, coordinates with mood, and sleep

6.1. SSs and shallow torpor

SSs from the MinnSS, like ANs, have a Tcore-low and experience extreme sensations of coldness. Like ANs, they need to reduce the discomfort of coldness and, in so doing, defend body temperature in the face of food deprivation. So, what behavioral thermoregulatory strategies do they adopt to satisfy these needs?

It is proposed that the primary behavioral thermoregulatory strategy “selected” by SSs is diametrically opposite to HyAc (Casper and Davis, 1977; Casper, 2022). SSs become tired, listless, and apathetic; these behaviors aim at conserving energy (Taylor and KEYS, 1950; Casper and Davis, 1977; Gorsky and Calloway, 1983; Hebebrand et al., 2022).

MinnSSs decreased the cost of daily activity from 1,567 to 451 Kcals per day, a 58% decrease (Franklin et al., 1948). During the first week of food reduction, SSs appeared fine; thereafter, overt movements became noticeably slower. “They climbed steps warily, one step at a time” (Taylor and Keys, 1950). One subject got stuck in the revolving doors of a downtown store and “never had the strength to push it open” (Taylor and Keys, 1950). In the later part of the 6-month fast, housekeeping chores and laboratory duties were neglected (Franklin et al., 1948). Participation in education programs for relief workers finally collapsed (Keys et al., 1950).

However, because of the requirements of the experimental condition, an overall level of activity had to be maintained. Subjects were obliged to walk to and from the mess hall, hike about 20 miles per week, and perform various other duties to simulate conditions of natural famine (Keys et al., 1950).

Although MinnSSs reported that physical exertion tired them and was avoided, occasionally, some exercised deliberately to lose weight to obtain extra bread rations or to prevent a reduction in food allocations because they had not lost enough weight (Keys et al., 1950). Some researchers have suggested that this additional activity implied that they remained “fairly active” (Epling and Pierce, 1996). On the contrary, all observations indicated that voluntary physical activity was markedly curtailed (Keys et al., 1950).

Similarly, decreased energy expenditure is a typical response to energy restriction in internment camps and during famines: “The first indications of a deficiency of food are languor, exhaustion, and general debility” (Franklin et al., 1948) “One can conclude that the natural tendency is to conserve energy during starvation by cutting down on all but a minimal level of essential activity” (Prentice et al., 1992). This decrease in energy expenditure reduces the depletion of stored fat reserves (Devlin, 2011).

Since SSs also need to maintain Tcore-low but do not generate internal metabolic heat through physical activity, as do ANs, how do SSs maintain Tcore-low? It is proposed that the primary thermoregulatory behavior adopted by most undernourished, emaciated humans focuses on retaining metabolic heat or passively gaining metabolic heat instead of actively generating internal heat.

SSs wear additional layers of clothing to retain metabolic heat, use blankets, wraps, and the like, and assume specific body postures that reduce environmental exposure, such as curling up (Hart et al., 1962). They also use shelters (houses, huts, igloos) to protect themselves from the elements (Wyndham and Morrison, 1958). To passively gain metabolic heat, SSs absorb heat from external sources such as basking in the sun, taking long hot showers, using hot-water bottles and heaters, turning up the thermostat, making a fire, and eating hot food and drinks (Tucker, 2006). The advantage of this inactive strategy is that less stored energy (fat) is used.

Numerous mammals and birds conserve energy by hibernating during food shortages (frequently compounded by low ambient temperatures). Hibernation involves drastically reducing Tcore, BMR, and movement. This behavior may last weeks to months, reducing the energy required to sustain life. “Daily torpor” or “shallow torpor” is a less intense form of energy conservation compared to hibernation (Schmidt, 2014). Shallow torpor requires periods of inactivity throughout the day. It has been defined as a temporary physiological state characterized by a controlled lowering of metabolic rate, body temperature, and physical activity below what is considered normal (Melvin and Andrews, 2009). It is an evolved behavioral strategy to facilitate survival in response to food shortages (Berger, 1984) and is “the most effective means for energy conservation available to endotherms” (Geiser, 2013).

It is now proposed that SSs and other semi-starved victims engage in shallow torpor when confronted with food shortages and weight loss, frequently compounded by low ambient temperatures. Shallow torpor serves two functions: firstly, it reduces energy needs and thus draws less on stored body fat reserves. Secondly, it enables individuals to engage in passive heat-retention/acquisition behaviors.

6.2. SSs mood/psychological state

The mood of the MinnSSs corresponded with their reduced activity levels; they became depressed and apathetic, although individual differences were marked (Keys et al., 1950). Emotional and personality changes started developing within 2 months of the semi-starvation period. The humor and high spirits, which had been an outstanding quality of the group during the initial three-month Control Phase, gradually disappeared. They became irritable; some had temper outbursts and would sulk; a few had violent urges; they lacked self-discipline and self-control. They were indecisive, sensitive to noise, and unable to concentrate. They began spending more time alone, saying it became “too tiring” to contend with others. Some symptoms, such as moodiness and depression, became more severe as semi-starvation progressed. This occurred in subjects who were initially emotionally well-balanced and had “made as good an adjustment to life as people usually do.” (Keys et al., 1950).

Furthermore, Franklin et al. (1948) reported that certain subjects developed symptoms of “semi-starvation neurosis,” ranging in intensity from mild to severe. However, in most cases, these symptoms receded during the following rehabilitation period. Eventually, they returned to pre-starvation “normal,” although four of the 36 subjects were disqualified due to the development of semi-starvation-induced “experimental neurosis” (Kalm and Semba, 2005). This is supported by (Fessler, 2003).

6.3. SSs and sleep

The sleeping behavior of SSs also corresponded to their lethargic movements and mood. Although the length of sleep during the night was not changed in the MinnSS, “the men frequently took naps during the day” (Keys et al., 1950).

Keys et al. (1950) reported that in Prisoner of War Camps in WWII, the desire for sleep increased; the number of hours that an adult male would wish to remain in bed, partly dozing but mostly in genuine deep sleep, steadily rose from 8 h to 16 h or more out of the 24 h. During sleep, BMR and body temperature decrease, and there is reduced movement; these adaptations further decrease energy needs (Schmidt, 2014; Monnard et al., 2017).

The following example illustrates how a group of present-day Russian peasants has learned to deal with food shortages and cold ambient temperatures; this custom has existed since time immemorial (Human Hibernation, 2000): “At the first fall of snow, the whole family gathers around the stove, lies down, ceases to wrestle with the problems of human existence, and quietly goes to sleep. Once a day, everyone wakes up to eat a piece of hard bread, of which an amount sufficient to last 6 months has providently been baked in the previous autumn. When the bread has been washed down with the draught of water, everyone goes to sleep again. The members of the family take … turn [s] to watch and keep the fire alight. After 6 months of this reposeful existence, the family wakes up, shakes itself, goes out to see if the grass is growing, and by-and-by sets to work at summer tasks.”

In summary: to deal with the two issues of feeling cold/maintenance of Tcore/low and reduced food availability, SSs focus on being inactive; this behavioral pattern is referred to as shallow torpor. In keeping with this strategy, SSs demonstrate a depressive mood and extended hours of sleep (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Proposed sequence of events leading to AN and SS, and the subsequent “selection” of thermo-behaviors. AN, anorexia nervosa; SS, semi-starvation; BMR, basal metabolic rate; Tcore-low, reduced body temperature; HyAc, hyperactivity.www.frontiersin.org = increase. www.frontiersin.org = decrease.

7. Vignette

The following hypothetical scenario is presented to contextualize the above ideas.

From about 2 million years ago to about 12,000 years ago, hunter-gatherer culture was humans’ only way of life (Fessler, 2002; Guisinger, 2003). ANs and SSs thermo-behavioral “strategies” were most likely operational during these times (Fessler, 2002; Guisinger, 2003).

To illustrate responses to reduced food availability and cold temperatures, suppose, ~14,000 years ago, a Clan of hunter-gatherers lived in a temperate region of the Northern Hemisphere with hot summers and cold winters (Guisinger, 2003; Vazquez et al., 2006b; Scolnick and Mostofsky, 2015). This Clan possibly consisted of ~70 individuals, including 30 females.

To obtain food, males hunted larger game, and women and children foraged for berries, fruits, bulbs, and the like (Fessler, 2002). As a result, everyone was well-fed in summer and most likely built-up excess body fat in preparation for winter shortages.

As winter approached and ambient temperatures declined, hunted, and foraged food became scarce. The men ventured out less frequently as game became sparser; the group became more reliant on the 30 women, the primary foragers (Fessler, 2002). Since foraged food was gradually depleted in nearby areas, the women were obliged to walk greater distances but collected less food.

Foraging was an energy-intensive activity (Pontzer et al., 2012). Thus, all 30 females began losing weight over subsequent weeks and months due to increased energy expenditure and reduced caloric consumption. Eventually, all 30 females lost approximately 25% of body weight; associated with this weight loss was a reduction in metabolic rate, Tcore, and related changes (Hebebrand et al., 2022). All 30 females now faced two primary survival challenges: reduced availability of food and maintenance of Tcore-low, probably exacerbated by decreases in ambient temperatures (McCue, 2010).

With this weight loss, it is proposed that foraging became unsustainable for 29/30 women (~99%). Most became lethargic and adopted the strategy of shallow torpor. They winterized shelters, remained in caves, used animal skins to keep warm, built fires, and slept for extended periods. Most of their energy was derived from stored body fat.

The 30th woman (1%), with a genetic predisposition to AN, adopted a diametrically opposite strategy: she became hyperactive. This hyperactivity would help maintain her Tcore-low and increase her chances of locating food. The problem with this restless behavior was that it was highly energy-consuming, necessitating increased foraging and, in some cases, migrating to warmer regions where more food was available (Guisinger, 2003). AN migration most likely contributes to the fact that female DNA is more diverse in many groups than male DNA (Seielstad et al., 1998).

With the onset of warmer weather and food availability, SSs and ANs would regain lost weight. In ANs, this would reduce HyAc, normalize Tcore-low, and re-establish mood and sleep patterns. In SSs, shallow torpor would diminish, and they, too, would re-establish “normal” behaviors. When weight was regained, possibly more fat would accumulate in both groups (Dulloo et al., 1996; El Ghoch et al., 2014); this would better prepare these females for a possible repeat scenario. From this perspective, changes in ANs and SSs may be regarded as seasonal (Gutierrez et al., 2013; Scolnick and Mostofsky, 2014, 2015).

In modern-day ANs, despite the frequent abundance of food and availability of heat, ANs inappropriately express HyAc behavior. It is proposed that this behavior remains a vestigial substitute for temperature regulation and foraging. In contrast, SSs do not adopt a strategy of shallow torpor in the presence of warmth and copious food.

8. Conclusion

In answer to Phillipou et al.’s (2018) question of whether psychologists are treating AN or SS, it is proposed that the initial physiological response to reduced caloric intake and weight loss in both groups are similar. The organism does not distinguish between weight loss due to willful food refusal or externally imposed circumstances such as famine. These physiological alterations aim at extending survival (Devlin, 2011).

It is proposed that these changes represent homeorhesis (as opposed to homeostasis). Homeorhesis reflects an orchestrated change during a specific physiological state, such as during a 9-month pregnancy (Bauman and Currie, 1980) or in association with injury and/or infection and the manifestation of “sickness behavior” (Shattuck and Muehlenbein, 2015). “The body adopts alternate strategies to promote optimal functioning during atypical circumstances.” (Bauman and Currie, 1980). When the cause for these changes is eliminated (in the case of ANs and SSs this would be increased caloric consumption and weight gain), the body reverts to its “standard” homeostatic state (Bauman and Currie, 1980).

The primary differences between ANs and SSs represent different strategies that each adopts to promote the maintenance of body temperature. For ANs this includes hyperactive behavior, an energetic mood, and reduced sleep. For SSs, behavior becomes lethargic; they display a depressive mood and an extensive increase in sleep. These thermo-behavioral “selections” are most likely genetically based, with the genetic component in ANs ranging from 32 to 76% (Bulik et al., 2019; Steiger and Booij, 2020; Barakat et al., 2023). Such changes could involve central re-programming of brain neurotransmitters, neuromodulators, and associated receptors (Pirke et al., 1984; Fessler, 2002; Levine and Kotz, 2005; Teske et al., 2008; Sodersten et al., 2016b; Zup et al., 2022; Barakat et al., 2023). Although there is considerable information on central changes in ANs, there is less information on these factors in SSs, possibly since their response to semi-starvation seems to be a “natural” adjustment (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Schematic diagram suggesting that if acute AN becomes chronic, symptoms will more closely resemble SS. AN, anorexia nervosa; SS, semi-starvation.

Although behavioral differences are proposed during acute AN, it is suggested that if this condition continues for an extended period, acute AN will progress into a chronic state (Crisp, 1984b; De Filippo et al., 2016). At this point, psychologists are probably treating SS and associated symptoms as well as inappropriate habit formation (Uniacke et al., 2018). If this assumption is correct, this would stress the importance of early detection and intervention (Kaye and Bulik, 2021; Koreshe et al., 2023).

Finally, it is suggested that the “adoption” of shallow torpor by SSs may be a more successful survival strategy than ANs’ hyperactive strategy. The rationale for attesting to this is that the number of females who “practiced” shallow torpor during times of food shortages and wintry weather appears to be much larger (96–99.09%) than ANs who adopted the strategy of HyAc (0.91–4%; Watson et al., 2019).

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Funding

This work was supported by the East Carolina University, an institutional partner, funds 50%.

Acknowledgments

Thank you to the following individuals for reviewing this manuscript. Joseph A, Houmard, Professor, Sport, Exercise, Recreation and Kinesiology, Director, Human Performance Laboratory, East Carolina University. Andrew McKune, Professor, Research Institute for Sport and Exercise, The University of Canberra. Much appreciation to Tami Marie Santeramo, Interlibrary Loan Coordinator, Laupus Health Sciences Library, East Carolina Univer.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Abbreviations

MinnSS(s), Minnesota Study of Semi-Starvation (subjects); AN(s), anorexia nervosa (patients); SS(s), semi-starved (individuals); HCs, healthy controls; HyAc, hyperactivity.

References

Ahima, R. S., Prabakaran, D., Mantzoros, C., Qu, D., Lowell, B., Maratos-Flier, E., et al. (1996). Role of leptin in the neuroendocrine response to fasting. Nature 382, 250–252. doi: 10.1038/382250a0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alberti, M., Galvani, C., El Ghoch, M., Capelli, C., Lanza, M., Calugi, S., et al. (2013). Assessment of physical activity in anorexia nervosa and treatment outcome. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 45, 1643–1648. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e31828e8f07

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barakat, S., McLean, S. A., Bryant, E., Le, A., Marks, P., National Eating Disorder Research Consortium, et al. (2023). Risk factors for eating disorders: findings from a rapid review. J. Eat. Disord. 11, 8–4. doi: 10.1186/s40337-022-00717-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bauman, D. E., and Currie, W. B. (1980). Partitioning of nutrients during pregnancy and lactation: a review of mechanisms involving homeostasis and homeorhesis. J. Dairy Sci. 63, 1514–1529. doi: 10.3168/jds.s0022-0302(80)83111-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belak, L., Gianini, L., Klein, D. A., Sazonov, E., Keegan, K., Neustadt, E., et al. (2017). Measurement of fidgeting in patients with anorexia nervosa using a novel shoe-based monitor. Eat. Behav. 24, 45–48. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2016.11.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berger, R. J. (1984). Slow wave sleep, shallow torpor and hibernation: homologous states of diminished metabolism and body temperature. Biol. Psychol. 19, 305–326. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(84)90045-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergh, C., Brodin, U., Lindberg, G., and Sodersten, P. (2002). Randomized controlled trial of a treatment for anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 99, 9486–9491. doi: 10.1073/pnas.142284799

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergh, C., Callmar, M., Danemar, S., Holcke, M., Isberg, S., Leon, M., et al. (2013). Effective treatment of eating disorders: results at multiple sites. Behav. Neurosci. 127, 878–889. doi: 10.1037/a0034921

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beumont, P. J., Arthur, B., Russell, J. D., and Touyz, S. W. (1994). Excessive physical activity in dieting disorder patients: proposals for a supervised exercise program. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 15, 21–36. doi: 10.1002/1098-108x(199401)15:13.0.co;2-k

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beumont, P., Beumont, C., Touyz, S., and Williams, H. (1997). Nutritional counseling and supervised exercise. Handbook of treatment for eating disorders. 2nd Edn New York London: The Guilford Press, 178–187.

Google Scholar

Bhanji, S., and Mattingly, D. (1991). Acrocyanosis in anorexia nervosa. Postgrad. Med. J. 67, 33–35. doi: 10.1136/pgmj.67.783.33

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birmingham, C. L., Gutierrez, E., Jonat, L., and Beumont, P. (2004). Randomized controlled trial of warming in anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 35, 234–238. doi: 10.1002/eat.10246

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blinder, B. J., Cumella, E. J., and Sanathara, V. A. (2006). Psychiatric comorbidities of female inpatients with eating disorders. Psychosom. Med. 68, 454–462. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000221254.77675.f5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boden, G., Chen, X., Mozzoli, M., and Ryan, I. (1996). Effect of fasting on serum leptin in normal human subjects. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 81, 3419–3423. doi: 10.1210/jcem.81.9.8784108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bongers, C. C. W. G., Daanen, H. A. M., Bogerd, C. P., Hopman, M. T. E., and Eijsvogels, T. M. H. (2018). Validity, reliability, and inertia of four different temperature capsule systems. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 50, 169–175. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000001403

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bossu, C., Galusca, B., Normand, S., Germain, N., Collet, P., Frere, D., et al. (2007). Energy expenditure adjusted for body composition differentiates constitutional thinness from both normal subjects and anorexia nervosa. Am. J. Physiol. Endocrinol. Metab. 292, E132–7. doi: 10.1152/ajpendo.00241.2006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bredella, M. A., Fazeli, P. K., Freedman, L. M., Calder, G., Lee, H., Rosen, C. J., et al. (2012). Young women with cold-activated brown adipose tissue have higher bone mineral density and lower Pref-1 than women without brown adipose tissue: a study in women with anorexia nervosa, women recovered from anorexia nervosa, and normal-weight women. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 97:584. doi: 10.1210/jc.2011-2246

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bulik, C. M., Blake, L., and Austin, J. (2019). Genetics of eating disorders: what the clinician needs to know. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 42, 59–73. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2018.10.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bulik, C. M., Hebebrand, J., Keski-Rahkonen, A., Klump, K. L., Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., Mazzeo, S. E., et al. (2007). Genetic epidemiology, endophenotypes, and eating disorder classification. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 40:52. doi: 10.1002/eat.20398

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calugi, S., Chignola, E., El Ghoch, M., and Dalle Grave, R. (2018). Starvation symptoms in patients with anorexia nervosa: a longitudinal study. Eat. Disord. 26, 523–537. doi: 10.1080/10640266.2018.1471921

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrera, O., Adan, R. A., Gutierrez, E., Danner, U. N., Hoek, H. W., van Elburg, A. A., et al. (2012). Hyperactivity in anorexia nervosa: warming up not just burning-off calories. PLoS One 7:e41851. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0041851

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casper, R. C. (1998). Behavioral activation and lack of concern, core symptoms of anorexia nervosa? Int. J. Eat. Disord. 24, 381–393. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199812)24:43.0.CO;2-Q

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casper, R. C. (2006). The drive for activity and restlessness in anorexia nervosa: potential pathways. J. Affect. Disord. 92, 99–107. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.12.039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casper, R. C. (2016). Restless activation and drive for activity in anorexia nervosa may reflect a disorder of energy homeostasis. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 49, 750–752. doi: 10.1002/eat.22575

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casper, R. C. (2018). Not the function of eating, but spontaneous activity and energy expenditure, reflected in restlessness and a drive for activity appear to be dysregulated in anorexia nervosa: treatment implications. Front. Psychol. 9:2303. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casper, R. C. (2022). Restlessness and an increased urge to move (drive for activity) in anorexia nervosa may strengthen personal motivation to maintain caloric restriction and may augment body awareness and proprioception: a lesson from leptin Administration in Anorexia Nervosa. Front. Psychol. 13:885274. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.885274

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casper, R. C., and Davis, J. M. (1977). On the course of anorexia nervosa. Am. J. Psychiatry 134, 974–978. doi: 10.1176/ajp.134.9.974

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casper, R. C., and Heller, W. (1991). La douce indifference and mood in anorexia nervosa: neuropsychological correlates. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry. 15, 15–23. doi: 10.1016/0278-5846(91)90037-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casper, R. C., Kirschner, B., Sandstead, H. H., Jacob, R. A., and Davis, J. M. (1980). An evaluation of trace metals, vitamins, and taste function in anorexia nervosa. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 33, 1801–1808. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/33.8.1801

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casper, R. C., Schoeller, D. A., Kushner, R., Hnilicka, J., and Gold, S. T. (1991). Total daily energy expenditure and activity level in anorexia nervosa. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 53, 1143–1150. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/53.5.1143

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casper, R. C., Voderholzer, U., Naab, S., and Schlegl, S. (2020). Increased urge for movement, physical and mental restlessness, fundamental symptoms of restricting anorexia nervosa? Brain Behav. 10:e01556. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1556

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cerrato, M., Carrera, O., Vazquez, R., Echevarria, E., and Gutierrez, E. (2012). Heat makes a difference in activity-based anorexia: a translational approach to treatment development in anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 45, 26–35. doi: 10.1002/eat.20884

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, J. L., Heist, K., DePaoli, A. M., Veldhuis, J. D., and Mantzoros, C. S. (2003). The role of falling leptin levels in the neuroendocrine and metabolic adaptation to short-term starvation in healthy men. J. Clin. Invest. 111, 1409–1421. doi: 10.1172/JCI200317490

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chudecka, M., and Lubkowska, A. (2016). Thermal imaging of body surface temperature distribution in women with anorexia nervosa. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 24, 57–61. doi: 10.1002/erv.2388

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciwoniuk, N., Wayda-Zalewska, M., and Kucharska, K. (2022). Distorted body image and mental pain in anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 20:718. doi: 10.3390/ijerph20010718

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Considine, R. V., Sinha, M. K., Heiman, M. L., Kriauciunas, A., Stephens, T. W., Nyce, M. R., et al. (1996). Serum immunoreactive-leptin concentrations in normal-weight and obese humans. N. Engl. J. Med. 334, 292–295. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199602013340503

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crisp, A. H. (1967). The possible significance of some behavioural correlates of weight and carbohydrate intake. J. Psychosom. Res. 11, 117–131. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(67)90064-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crisp, A. H. (1980). Sleep, activity, nutrition and mood. Br. J. Psychiatry 137, 1–7. doi: 10.1192/bjp.137.1.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crisp, A. H. (1984a). The psychopathology of anorexia nervosa: getting the heat out of the system. Res. Publ. Assoc. Res. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 62, 209–234.

Google Scholar

Crisp, A. H. (1984b). “Treatment of anorexia nervosa:what can be the role of psychopharmacological agents?” in The psychobiology of anorexia nervosa. ed. K. P. Pirke (Berlin Heidelberg New York Tokyo: Springer-Verlag), 148–160.

Google Scholar

Crisp, A. H., Hsu, L. K., and Harding, B. (1980). The starving hoarder and voracious spender: stealing in anorexia nervosa. J. Psychosom. Res. 24, 225–231. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(80)90012-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crisp, A. H., Stonehill, E., and Fenton, G. W. (1971). The relationship between sleep, nutrition and mood: a study of patients with anorexia nervosa. Postgrad. Med. J. 47, 207–213. doi: 10.1136/pgmj.47.546.207

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalle Grave, R., Calugi, S., and Marchesini, G. (2008). Compulsive exercise to control shape or weight in eating disorders: prevalence, associated features, and treatment outcome. Compr. Psychiatry 49, 346–352. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2007.12.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Das, S., and Maiti, A. (2013). Acrocyanosis: an overview. Indian J. Dermatol. 58, 417–420. doi: 10.4103/0019-5154.119946

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davies, C. T., Fohlin, L., and Thoren, C. (1979). Temperature regulation in anorexia nervosa patients during prolonged exercise. Acta Med. Scand. 205, 257–262. doi: 10.1111/j.0954-6820.1979.tb06042.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, C., Katzman, D. K., Kaptein, S., Kirsh, C., Brewer, H., Kalmbach, K., et al. (1997). The prevalence of high-level exercise in the eating disorders: etiological implications. Compr. Psychiatry 38, 321–326. doi: 10.1016/s0010-440x(97)90927-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, C., Katzman, D. K., and Kirsh, C. (1999). Compulsive physical activity in adolescents with anorexia nervosa: a psychobehavioral spiral of pathology. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 187, 336–342. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199906000-00002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, C., Kennedy, S. H., Ravelski, E., and Dionne, M. (1994). The role of physical activity in the development and maintenance of eating disorders. Psychol. Med. 24, 957–967. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700029044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Filippo, E., Marra, M., Alfinito, F., Di Guglielmo, M. L., Majorano, P., Cerciello, G., et al. (2016). Hematological complications in anorexia nervosa. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 70, 1305–1308. doi: 10.1038/ejcn.2016.115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delvenne, V., Kerkhofs, M., Appelboom-Fondu, J., Lucas, F., and Mendlewicz, J. (1992). Sleep polygraphic variables in anorexia nervosa and depression: a comparative study in adolescents. J. Affect. Disord. 25, 167–172. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(92)90002-n

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devlin, M. J. (2011). Why does starvation make bones fat? Am. J. Hum. Biol. 23, 577–585. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.21202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dostalova, I., Bartak, V., Papezova, H., and Nedvidkova, J. (2007). The effect of short-term exercise on plasma leptin levels in patients with anorexia nervosa. Metab. Clin. Exp. 56, 497–503.

Google Scholar

Dulloo, A. G., Jacquet, J., and Girardier, L. (1996). Autoregulation of body composition during weight recovery in human: the Minnesota experiment revisited. Int. J. Obes. Relat. Metab. Disord. 20, 393–405.

Google Scholar

Eckert, E. D., Pomeroy, C., Raymond, N., Kohler, P. F., Thuras, P., and Bowers, C. Y. (1998). Leptin in anorexia nervosa. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 83, 791–795. doi: 10.1210/jc.83.3.791

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

El Ghoch, M., Calugi, S., Lamburghini, S., and Dalle Grave, R. (2014). Anorexia nervosa and body fat distribution: a systematic review. Nutrients 6, 3895–3912. doi: 10.3390/nu6093895

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

El Ghoch, M., Calugi, S., Pellegrini, M., Milanese, C., Busacchi, M., Battistini, N. C., et al. (2013). Measured physical activity in anorexia nervosa: features and treatment outcome. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 46, 709–712. doi: 10.1002/eat.22140

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Epling, WF, and Pierce, WD. (1996). An overview of activity anorexia. In W.a.P. Epling, WD, Activity anorexia: theory, research and treament. (pp. 3–12). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Publishers.

Google Scholar

Fessler, D. M. (2002). Dimorphic foraging behaviors and the evolution of hominid hunting. Riv. Biol. 95, 429–453.

Google Scholar

Fessler, D. M. T. (2003). The implications of starvation induced psychological changes for the ethical treatment of hunger strikers. J. Med. Ethics 29, 243–247. doi: 10.1136/jme.29.4.243

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fichter, M. M., Pirke, K. M., and Holsboer, F. (1986). Weight loss causes neuroendocrine disturbances: experimental study in healthy starving subjects. Psychiatry Res. 17, 61–72. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(86)90042-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flouris, A. D. (2011). Functional architecture of behavioural thermoregulation. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 111, 1–8. doi: 10.1007/s00421-010-1602-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fraga, A., Carreira, M. C., Gonzalez-Izquierdo, A., Dieguez, C., Lopez, M., and Gutierrez, E. (2020). Temperature but not leptin prevents semi-starvation induced hyperactivity in rats: implications for anorexia nervosa treatment. Sci. Rep. 10:5300-z. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-62147-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frank, G. K. W., Shott, M. E., and DeGuzman, M. C. (2019). Recent advances in understanding anorexia nervosa. F1000Research 8:e2019. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.17789.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franklin, J. C., Schiele, B. C., Brozak, J., and Keys, A. (1948). Observations on human behavior in experimental semistarvation and rehabilitation. J. Clin. Psychol. 4, 28–45. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(194801)4:1<28::aid-jclp2270040103>3.0.co;2-f

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frostad, S., and Bentz, M. (2022). Anorexia nervosa: outpatient treatment and medical management. World J. Psychiatr. 12, 558–579. doi: 10.5498/wjp.v12.i4.558

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garfinkel, P. E. (1974). Perception of hunger and satiety in anorexia nervosa. Psychol. Med. 4, 309–315. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700042999

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garland, T., Schutz, H., Chappell, M. A., Keeney, B. K., Meek, T. H., Copes, L. E., et al. (2011). The biological control of voluntary exercise, spontaneous physical activity and daily energy expenditure in relation to obesity: human and rodent perspectives. J. Exp. Biol. 214, 206–229. doi: 10.1242/jeb.048397

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garner, D. M. (1997). “Psychoeducational principals in treatment” in Handbook of treatment for eating disorders. Second addition. ed. D. M. G. Garner Paul (New York, London: The Guilford Press), 145–177.

Google Scholar

Garner, D. M., Olmsted, M. P., Bohr, Y., and Garfinkel, P. E. (1982). The eating attitudes test: psychometric features and clinical correlates. Psychol. Med. 12, 871–878. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700049163

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geiser, F. (2013). Hibernation. Curr. Biol. 23:188.

Google Scholar

Giel, K. E., Kullmann, S., Preissl, H., Bischoff, S. C., Thiel, A., Schmidt, U., et al. (2013). Understanding the reward system functioning in anorexia nervosa: crucial role of physical activity. Biol. Psychol. 94, 575–581. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.10.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldstein, N., Levine, B. J., Loy, K. A., Duke, W. L., Meyerson, O. S., Jamnik, A. A., et al. (2018). Hypothalamic neurons that regulate feeding can influence sleep – wake states based on homeostatic need. Curr. Biol. 28, 3736–3747.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.09.055

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorsky, R. D., and Calloway, D. H. (1983). Activity pattern changes with decreases in food energy intake. Hum. Biol. 55, 577–586.

Google Scholar

Guisinger, S. (2003). Adapted to flee famine: adding an evolutionary perspective on anorexia nervosa. Psychol. Rev. 110, 745–761. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gull, W. W. (1997). Anorexia nervosa. Obes. Res. 5, 498–502. doi: 10.1002/j.1550-8528.1997.tb00677.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gummer, R., Giel, K. E., Schag, K., Resmark, G., Junne, F. P., Becker, S., et al. (2015). High levels of physical activity in anorexia nervosa: a systematic review. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev 23, 333–344. doi: 10.1002/erv.2377

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutierrez, E., Baysari, M. T., Carrera, O., Whitford, T. J., and Boakes, R. A. (2006). High ambient temperature reduces rate of body-weight loss produced by wheel running. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 59, 1196–1211. doi: 10.1080/17470210500417688

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutierrez, E., and Birmingham, C. L. (2020). Editorial: new perspectives to unlock the current impasse in treating anorexia nervosa. Front. Psychol. 11:207. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00207

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutierrez, E., Carrera, O., Vazquez, R., and Birmingham, C. L. (2013). Climate might be considered as a risk factor for anorexia nervosa? A hypothesis worth another look. Eat. Behav. 14, 278–280. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.05.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutierrez, E., Cerrato, M., Carrera, O., and Vazquez, R. (2008). Heat reversal of activity-based anorexia: implications for the treatment of anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 41, 594–601. doi: 10.1002/eat.20535

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutierrez, E., and Vazquez, R. (2001). Heat in the treatment of patients with anorexia nervosa. Eat. Weig. Disord. 6, 49–52. doi: 10.1007/bf03339752

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutierrez, E., Vazquez, R., and Beumont, P. J. (2002). Do people with anorexia nervosa use sauna baths? A reconsideration of heat-treatment in anorexia nervosa. Eat. Behav. 3, 133–142. doi: 10.1016/s1471-0153(01)00051-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haas, V., Onur, S., Paul, T., Nutzinger, D. O., Bosy-Westphal, A., Hauer, M., et al. (2005). Leptin and body weight regulation in patients with anorexia nervosa before and during weight recovery. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 81, 889–896. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/81.4.889

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hart, J. S., Sabean, H. B., Hildes, J. A., Depocas, F., Hammel, H. T., Andersen, K. L., et al. (1962). Thermal and metabolic responses of coastal Eskimos during a cold night. J. Appl. Physiol. 17, 953–960. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1962.17.6.953

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hebebrand, J., Blum, W. F., Barth, N., Coners, H., Englaro, P., Juul, A., et al. (1997). Leptin levels in patients with anorexia nervosa are reduced in the acute stage and elevated upon short-term weight restoration. Mol. Psychiatry 2, 330–334. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000282

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hebebrand, J., Denecke, S., and Antel, J. (2023). The role of leptin in rodent and human sleep: a transdiagnostic approach with a particular focus on anorexia nervosa. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 149:105164. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105164

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hebebrand, J., Exner, C., Hebebrand, K., Holtkamp, C., Casper, R. C., Remschmidt, H., et al. (2003). Hyperactivity in patients with anorexia nervosa and in semistarved rats: evidence for a pivotal role of hypoleptinemia. Physiol. Behav. 79, 25–37. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(03)00102-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hebebrand, J., Hildebrandt, T., Schlogl, H., Seitz, J., Denecke, S., Vieira, D., et al. (2022). The role of hypoleptinemia in the psychological and behavioral adaptation to starvation: implications for anorexia nervosa. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 141:104807. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104807

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hebebrand, J., Milos, G., Wabitsch, M., Teufel, M., Fuhrer, D., Buhlmeier, J., et al. (2019). Clinical trials required to assess potential benefits and side effects of treatment of patients with anorexia nervosa with recombinant human leptin. Front. Psychol. 10:769. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00769

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hicks, S. D., Jacob, P., Perez, O., Baffuto, M., Gagnon, Z., and Middleton, F. A. (2019). The transcriptional signature of a Runners high. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 51, 970–978. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000001865

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillebrand, J. J., de Rijke, C. E., Brakkee, J. H., Kas, M. J., and Adan, R. A. (2005). Voluntary access to a warm plate reduces hyperactivity in activity-based anorexia. Physiol. Behav. 85, 151–157. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.03.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Human Hibernation (2000). BMJ England. 320, 1245A. [Reprint].

Google Scholar

Ioakimidis, I., Zandian, M., Ulbl, F., Bergh, C., Leon, M., and Sodersten, P. (2011). How eating affects mood. Physiol. Behav. 103, 290–294. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.01.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacquot, C. M. C., Schellen, L., Kingma, B. R., van Baak, M. A., and van Marken Lichtenbelt, W. D. (2014). Influence of thermophysiology on thermal behavior: the essentials of categorization. Physiol. Behav. 128, 180–187. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.01.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jequier, E. (2002). Leptin signaling, adiposity, and energy balance. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 967, 379–388. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2002.tb04293.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalm, L. M., and Semba, R. D. (2005). They starved so that others be better fed: remembering Ancel Keys and the Minnesota experiment. J. Nutr. 135, 1347–1352. doi: 10.1093/jn/135.6.1347

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karageorgiou, V., Furukawa, T. A., Tsigkaropoulou, E., Karavia, A., Gournellis, R., Soureti, A., et al. (2020). Adipokines in anorexia nervosa: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 112. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.104485

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kay, D. W. (1953). Anorexia nervosa: a study in prognosis. Proc. R. Soc. Med. 46, 669–674.

Google Scholar

Kay, D. W., and Leigh, D. (1954). The natural history, treatment and prognosis of anorexia nervosa, based on a study of 38 patients. J. Ment. Sci. 100, 411–431. doi: 10.1192/bjp.100.419.411

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaye, W. H., and Bulik, C. M. (2021). Treatment of patients with anorexia nervosa in the US-A crisis in care. JAMA Psychiatry 78, 591–592. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.4796

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keys, A., Brozek, J., Henschel, A., Mickelsen, O., and Taylor, HL. (1950). The biology of human starvation. 2. Minneapolis: The University of Minnesota Press.

Google Scholar

Keys, A., Brozek, J., Henschel, A., Mickelsen, O., and Taylor, H.L. (1950). The biology of human starvation. Volume 1. Minneapolis: The University of Minnesota Press.

Google Scholar

Keys, A., Henschel, A., and Brožek, J. (1950). The biology of human starvation, vol. II. 1st Edn Minneapolis USA: The University of Minnesota Press.

Google Scholar

Kiezebrink, K., Campbell, D., Mann, E., and Blundell, J. (2009). Similarities and differences between excessive exercising anorexia nervosa patients compared with DSM-IV defined anorexia nervosa subtypes. Eat. Weig. Disord. 14:199. doi: 10.1007/BF03325117

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleppe, M. M., Kessler, U., Rekkedal, G. A., Skjakodegard, H. F., and Danielsen, Y. S. (2023). Differences in sleep patterns between patients with anorexia nervosa and healthy controls: a cross-sectional study. J. Eat. Disord. 11, 76–78. doi: 10.1186/s40337-023-00799-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koreshe, E., Paxton, S., Miskovic-Wheatley, J., Bryant, E., Le, A., Maloney, D., et al. (2023). Prevention and early intervention in eating disorders: findings from a rapid review. J. Eat. Disord. 11, 38–33. doi: 10.1186/s40337-023-00758-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krauchi, K. (2007). The thermophysiological cascade leading to sleep initiation in relation to phase of entrainment. Sleep Med. Rev. 11, 439–451. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2007.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurklinsky, A. K., Miller, V. M., and Rooke, T. W. (2011). Acrocyanosis: the flying Dutchman. Vascul. Med. 16, 288–301. doi: 10.1177/1358863X11398519

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lacey, J. H., Crisp, A. H., Kalucy, R. S., Hartmann, M., and Chien, C. (1976). Study of EEG sleep characteristics in patients with anorexia nervosa before and after restoration of matched population mean weight consequent on ingestion of a normal diet. Postgrad. Med. J. 52, 45–49. doi: 10.1136/pgmj.52.603.45

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lampert, F., and Lau, B. (1976). Bone marrow hypoplasia in anorexia nervosa. Eur. J. Pediatr. 124, 65–71. doi: 10.1007/bf00452416

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landsberg, L. (2012). Core temperature: a forgotten variable in energy expenditure and obesity? Obes. Rev. 13, 97–104. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2012.01040.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levine, J. A. (2007). Nonexercise activity thermogenesis – liberating the life-force. J. Intern. Med. 262, 273–287. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2007.01842.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levine, J. A., and Kotz, C. M. (2005). NEAT – non-exercise activity thermogenesis – egocentric & geocentric environmental factors vs biological regulation. Acta Physiol. Scand. 184, 309–318. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-201X.2005.01467.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, P., and Wakeling, A. (1980). Altered thresholds for thermoregulatory sweating and vasodilatation in anorexia nervosa. Br. Med. J. 281, 906–908. doi: 10.1136/bmj.281.6245.906

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malcolm, A., Toh, W. L., Crocker, K., and Phillipou, A. (2022). The contribution of sleep to anorexia nervosa severity. Eat. Weig. Disord. 27, 1563–1568. doi: 10.1007/s40519-021-01286-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinez-Sanchez, S. M., Martinez-Garcia, T. E., and Munguia-Izquierdo, D. (2020). Clinical, psychopathological, physical, and sleep evolution in adolescents with restrictive anorexia nervosa participating in a day hospital program. Psychiatry Investig. 17, 366–373. doi: 10.30773/pi.2020.0016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCue, M. D. (2010). Starvation physiology: reviewing the different strategies animals use to survive a common challenge. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 156, 1–18. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpa.20

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mecklenburg, R. S., Loriaux, D. L., Thompson, R. H., Andersen, A. E., and Lipsett, M. B. (1974). Hypothalamic dysfunction in patients with anorexia nervosa. Medicine 53, 147–159. doi: 10.1097/00005792-197403000-00003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melvin, R. G., and Andrews, M. T. (2009). Torpor induction in mammals: recent discoveries fueling new ideas. Trends Endocrinol Metab 20, 490–498. doi: 10.1016/j.tem.2009.09.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, C., Taranis, L., Goodwin, H., and Haycraft, E. (2011). Compulsive exercise and eating disorders. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 19, 174–189. doi: 10.1002/erv.1122

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, C., Taranis, L., and Touyz, S. (2008). Excessive exercise in the eating disorders: a need for less activity from patients and more from researchers. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 16, 81–83. doi: 10.1002/erv.863

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miao, D., Young, S. L., and Golden, C. D. (2015). A meta-analysis of pica and micronutrient status. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 27, 84–93. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.22598

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milos, G., Antel, J., Kaufmann, L., Barth, N., Koller, A., Tan, S., et al. (2020). Short-term metreleptin treatment of patients with anorexia nervosa: rapid on-set of beneficial cognitive, emotional, and behavioral effects. Transl. Psychiatry 10, 303–301. doi: 10.1038/s41398-020-00977-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Misra, M., and Klibanski, A. (2014). Endocrine consequences of anorexia nervosa. Lancet. Diabet. 2, 581–592. doi: 10.1016/S2213-8587(13)70180-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monnard, C. R., Fares, E., Calonne, J., Miles-Chan, J. L., Montani, J., Durrer, D., et al. (2017). Issues in continuous 24-h Core body temperature monitoring in humans using an ingestible capsule telemetric sensor. Front. Endocrinol. 8:130. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2017.00130

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muller, M. J., Enderle, J., Pourhassan, M., Braun, W., Eggeling, B., Lagerpusch, M., et al. (2015). Metabolic adaptation to caloric restriction and subsequent refeeding: the Minnesota starvation experiment revisited. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 102, 807–819. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.115.109173

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muller, T. D., Focker, M., Holtkamp, K., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., and Hebebrand, J. (2009). Leptin-mediated neuroendocrine alterations in anorexia nervosa: somatic and behavioral implications. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 18, 117–129. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2008.07.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muller, T. D., Nogueiras, R., Andermann, M. L., Andrews, Z. B., Anker, S. D., Argente, J., et al. (2015). Ghrelin. Mol. Metabol. 4, 437–460. doi: 10.1016/j.molmet.2015.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishita, J. K., Knopes, K. D., Ellinwood, E. H., and Rockwell, W. J. K. (1986). Hypothermia and abnormalities in thermoregulation in patients with anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 5, 713–725.

Google Scholar

Nuttall, F. Q., Almokayyad, R. M., and Gannon, M. C. (2016). The ghrelin and leptin responses to short-term starvation vs a carbohydrate-free diet in men with type 2 diabetes: a controlled, cross-over design study. Nutr. Metabol. 13:47-e2016. doi: 10.1186/s12986-016-0106-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oishi, K., Yamamoto, S., Uchida, D., and Doi, R. (2013). Ketogenic diet and fasting induce the expression of cold-inducible RNA-binding protein with time-dependent hypothermia in the mouse liver. FEBS Open Bio 3, 192–195. doi: 10.1016/j.fob.2013.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olson, B., Marks, D. L., and Grossberg, A. J. (2020). Diverging metabolic programmes and behaviours during states of starvation, protein malnutrition, and cachexia. J. Cachexia. Sarcopenia Muscle 11, 1429–1446. doi: 10.1002/jcsm.12630

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onur, S., Haas, V., Bosy-Westphal, A., Hauer, M., Paul, T., Nutzinger, D., et al. (2005). L-tri-iodothyronine is a major determinant of resting energy expenditure in underweight patients with anorexia nervosa and during weight gain. Eur. J. Endocrinol. 152, 179–184. doi: 10.1530/eje.1.01850

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parmeggiani, P. L. (2003). Thermoregulation and sleep. Front. Biosci. 8:557. doi: 10.2741/1054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillipou, A., Rossell, S. L., and Castle, D. J. (2018). Anorexia nervosa or starvation? Eur. J. Neurosci. 48, 3317–3318. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14158

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pirke, K. P. (1987). “Biology of human starvation” in Handbook of eating disorders: Part 1: Anorexia and bulimia nervosa. eds. P. J. V. Beumont, G. D. Burrpws, and R. C. Casper (Amsterdam New York Oxford: Elsevier), 79–102.

Google Scholar

Pirke, K. M., Spyra, B., Warnhoff, M., Kuderling, I., Dorsch, G., and Gramsch, C. (1984). “Effect of starvation on central neurotransmitter systems and on endocrine regulation” in The psychobiology of anorexia nervosa. ed. K. P. Pirke (Berlin Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag), 46–57.

Google Scholar

Polito, A., Fabbri, A., Ferro-Luzzi, A., Cuzzolaro, M., Censi, L., Ciarapica, D., et al. (2000). Basal metabolic rate in anorexia nervosa: relation to body composition and leptin concentrations. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 71, 1495–1502. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/71.6.1495

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pontzer, H., Raichlen, D. A., Wood, B. M., Mabulla, A. Z. P., Racette, S. B., and Marlowe, F. W. (2012). Hunter-gatherer energetics and human obesity. PLoS One 7:e40503. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040503

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prentice, A. M., Diaz, E., Goldberg,, Jebb, G. R., Coward, S. A., and Whitehead, W. A. (1992) in Famine and refeeding: adaptations in energy metabolism. ed. G. K. Anderson (San Diego, New York, Boston, London, Sydney, Tokyo, Toronto: Academic Press, Inc. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich), 245–267.

Google Scholar

Price, T., Zebitz, M., Giraldi, A., Lokind, T. S., Treasure, J., and Sjogren, J. M. (2020). Sexual function and dysfunction among women with anorexia nervosa: a systematic scoping review. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 53, 1377–1399. doi: 10.1002/eat.23299

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rising, R., Keys, A., Ravussin, E., and Bogardus, C. (1992). Concomitant interindividual variation in body temperature and metabolic rate. Am. J. Phys. 263:730. doi: 10.1152/ajpendo.1992.263.4.E730

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romanovsky, A. A. (2007). Thermoregulation: some concepts have changed. Functional architecture of the thermoregulatory system. Am. J. Phys. Regul. Integr. Comp. Phys. 292, R37–R46. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00668.2006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romanovsky, A. A. (2014). Skin temperature: its role in thermoregulation. Acta Physiol 210, 498–507. doi: 10.1111/apha.12231

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roura, I., Fraga, A., and Gutierrez, E. (2020). Differential effects of heat in the phases of the light-dark cycle in the activity-based anorexia model. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 53, 1826–1835. doi: 10.1002/eat.23363

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Routtenberg, A., and Kuznesof, A. W. (1967). Self-starvation of rats living in activity wheels on a restricted feeding schedule. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 64, 414–421. doi: 10.1037/h0025205

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauchelli, S., Jiménez-Murcia, S., Sánchez, I., Riesco, N., Custal, N., Fernández-García, J. C., et al. (2016). Orexin and sleep quality in anorexia nervosa: clinical relevance and influence on treatment outcome. Psychoneuroendocrinology 65, 102–108. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheurink, A. J., Boersma, G. J., Nergardh, R., and Sodersten, P. (2010). Neurobiology of hyperactivity and reward: agreeable restlessness in anorexia nervosa. Physiol. Behav. 100, 490–495. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.03.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlader, Z. (2014). The relative overlooking of human behavioral temperature regulation: an issue worth resolving. Temperature 1, 20–21. doi: 10.4161/temp.29235

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, M. H. (2014). The energy allocation function of sleep: a unifying theory of sleep, torpor, and continuous wakefulness. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 47, 122–153. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schorr, M., and Miller, K. K. (2017). The endocrine manifestations of anorexia nervosa: mechanisms and management. Nat. Rev. Endocrinol. 13, 174–186. doi: 10.1038/nrendo.2016.175

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwabe, A. D., Lippe, B. M., Chang, R. J., Pops, M. A., and Yager, J. (1981). Anorexia nervosa. Ann. Intern. Med. 94, 371–381. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-94-3-371

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scolnick, B. (2018). Hypothesis: clues from mammalian hibernation for treating patients with anorexia nervosa. Front. Psychol. 9:2159. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02159

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scolnick, B., and Mostofsky, D. I. (2014). Anorexia nervosa: a rogue hibernation? Med. Hypotheses 82, 231–235. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2013.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scolnick, B., and Mostofsky, D. I. (2015). Anorexia nervosa, seasonality, and polyunsaturated fatty acids. Med. Hypotheses 85, 380–382. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2015.05.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seielstad, M. T., Minch, E., and Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1998). Genetic evidence for a higher female migration rate in humans. Nat. Genet. 20, 278–280. doi: 10.1038/3088

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seoane-Collazo, P., Martinez-Sanchez, N., Milbank, E., and Contreras, C. (2020). Incendiary leptin. Nutrients 12, 1–34. doi: 10.3390/nu12020472

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shattuck, E. C., and Muehlenbein, M. P. (2015). Human sickness behavior: ultimate and proximate explanations. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 157, 1–18. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.22698

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silverman, J. A. (1983). “Medical consequence of starvation; the malnutrition of anorexia nervosa: caveat Medicus” in Anorexia nervosa: recent developments in research. eds. P. L. Darby, P. E. Garfinkel, D. M. Garner, and D. V. Coscina (New York, NY: Alan R. Liss, INC), 293–299.

Google Scholar

Smith, L. L. (2021). The central role of hypothermia and hyperactivity in anorexia nervosa: a hypothesis. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 15:700645. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.700645

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sodersten, P., Bergh, C., and Leon, M. (2016a). Commentary: new insights in anorexia nervosa. Front. Neurosci. 10:483. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00483

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sodersten, P., Bergh, C., Leon, M., and Zandian, M. (2016b). Dopamine and anorexia nervosa. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 60, 26–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.11.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sodersten, P., Brodin, U., Zandian, M., and Bergh, C. E. K. (2019). Verifying Feighner's hypothesis; anorexia nervosa is not a psychiatric disorder. Front. Psychol. 10:21. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02110

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Speakman, J. R., and Mitchell, S. E. (2011). Caloric restriction. Mol. Asp. Med. 32, 159–221. doi: 10.1016/j.mam.2011.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steiger, H., and Booij, L. (2020). Eating disorders, heredity and environmental activation: getting epigenetic concepts into practice. J. Clin. Med. 9:1332. doi: 10.3390/jcm9051332

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strumia, R. (2013). Eating disorders and the skin. Clin. Dermatol. 31, 80–85. doi: 10.1016/j.clindermatol.2011.11.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swenne, I., and Engstrom, I. (2005). Medical assessment of adolescent girls with eating disorders: an evaluation of symptoms and signs of starvation. Acta Paediatrica 94, 1363–1371. W434661225X8N425

Google Scholar

Szymusiak, R. (2018). Body temperature and sleep. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 156, 341–351. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-63912-7.00020-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, H. L., and Keys, A. (1950). Adaptation to caloric restriction. Science 112, 215–218. doi: 10.1126/science.112.2904.215

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teske, J. A., Billington, C. J., and Kotz, C. M. (2008). Neuropeptidergic mediators of spontaneous physical activity and non-exercise activity thermogenesis. Neuroendocrinology 87, 71–90. doi: 10.1159/000110802

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Treasure, J., Zipfel, S., Micali, N., Wade, T., Stice, E., Claudino, A., et al. (2015). Anorexia nervosa. Nat. Rev. Dis. Prim. 1:15074. doi: 10.1038/nrdp.2015.74

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tucker, T. (2006). The great starvation experiment. New York London Toronto Sydney: Free Press.

Google Scholar

Tyszkiewicz-Nwafor, M., Rybakowski, F., Dmitrzak-Weglarz, M., Skibinska, M., Paszynska, E., Dutkiewicz, A., et al. (2020). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor and oxytocin signaling in association with clinical symptoms in adolescent inpatients with anorexia nervosa-a longitudinal study. Front. Psych. 10:1032. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.01032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uniacke, B., Timothy Walsh, B., Foerde, K., and Steinglass, J. (2018). The role of habits in anorexia nervosa: where we are and where to go from here? Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 20, 61–65. doi: 10.1007/s11920-018-0928-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vargas, N. T., Chapman, C. L., Sackett, J. R., Johnson, B. D., Gathercole, R., and Schlader, Z. J. (2019). Thermal behavior differs between males and females during exercise and recovery. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 51, 141–152. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000001756

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vargas, N. T., Slyer, J., Chapman, C. L., Johnson, B. D., Temple, J. L., Mietlicki-Baase, E. G., et al. (2018). The motivation to behaviorally thermoregulate during passive heat exposure in humans is dependent on the magnitude of increases in skin temperature. Physiol. Behav. 194, 545–551. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.07.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vazquez, R., Carrera, O., Birmingham, L., and Gutierrez, E. (2006). Exploring the association between anorexia nervosa and geographical latitude. Eat Weigh. Disord. 11, e1–e8. doi: 10.1007/BF03327745

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vigersky, R. A., Andersen, A. E., Thompson, R. H., and Loriaux, D. L. (1977). Hypothalamic dysfunction in secondary amenorrhea associated with simple weight loss. N. Engl. J. Med. 297, 1141–1145. doi: 10.1056/NEJM197711242972103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vigersky, R. A., Loriaux, D. L., Andersen, A. E., and Lipsett, M. B. (1976). Anorexia nervosa: behavioural and hypothalamic aspects. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 5, 517–535. doi: 10.1016/s0300-595x(76)80034-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, H. J., Yilmaz, Z., Thornton, L. M., Hubel, C., Coleman, J. R. I., Gaspar, H. A., et al. (2019). Genome-wide association study identifies eight risk loci and implicates metabo-psychiatric origins for anorexia nervosa. Nat. Genet. 51, 1207–1214. doi: 10.1038/s41588-019-0439-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wyndham, C. H., and Morrison, J. F. (1958). Adjustment to cold of bushmen in the Kalahari Desert. J. Appl. Physiol. 13, 219–225. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1958.13.2.219

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yao, S., Kuja-Halkola, R., Thornton, L. M., Norring, C., Almqvist, C., D'Onofrio, B. M., et al. (2017). Risk of being convicted of theft and other crimes in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: a prospective cohort study in a Swedish female population. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 50, 1095–1103. doi: 10.1002/eat.22743

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yaryura-Tobias, J. A., Neziroglu, F. A., and Kaplan, S. (1995). Self-mutilation, anorexia, and dysmenorrhea in obsessive compulsive disorder. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 17, 33–38. doi: 10.1002/1098-108x(199501)17:13.0.co;2-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zandian, M., Holmstedt, E., Larsson, A., Bergh, C., Brodin, U., and Sodersten, P. (2017). Anxiolytic effect of warmth in anorexia nervosa. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 135, 266–267. doi: 10.1111/acps.12691

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zup, S. L., Park, J. H., and Dominguez, J. M. (2022). Editorial: intersection of hormones and neuropeptides in the brain. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16:886591. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.886591

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: hyperactivity (HyAc), shallow torpor, low body temperature (Tcore-low), cold sensations, foraging, evolution

Citation: Smith LL (2023) Similarities and differences regarding acute anorexia nervosa and semi-starvation: does behavioral thermoregulation play a central role? Front. Behav. Neurosci. 17:1243572. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2023.1243572

Received: 20 June 2023; Accepted: 22 September 2023;
Published: 25 October 2023.

Edited by:

Fabrizio Sanna, University of Cagliari, Italy

Reviewed by:

Barbara Scolnick, Boston University, United States
Emilio Gutierrez, Universidad de Santiago de Compostela, Spain

Copyright © 2023 Smith. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lucille Lakier Smith, lclakier@gmail.com

Download