Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Commun., 02 February 2023
Sec. Psychology of Language
Volume 8 - 2023 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2023.1072880

Reconciling the cognitive and social approaches to describing teacher talk in second language classrooms: The contribution of systemic functional linguistics

  • School of Foreign Languages and Cultures, Guangdong University of Finance, Guangzhou, China

Teacher talk plays an important role in second-language classroom interaction. Studies are informed by multiple theories and yet could be classified under two general approaches, i.e., cognitive and social. The two approaches provide different but complementary perspectives on the role of teacher talk in interaction, with a focus on either learners' cognitive change or their social participation. A conversation between them is called for in the academic field to understand their interdependent relationship as well as the loss and gain in the respective approach. However, the conversation is difficult to launch because the cognitive and social approaches have developed distinct perspectives on what constitutes language and how learning evolves, leading to seemingly incompatible descriptive paradigms. With reference to systemic functional linguistics (SFL), this article argues for reconciling the two approaches in the following aspects. First, the meaning-oriented view of language in SFL expands the learning scope beyond language forms and offers both approaches an angle to reconsider the focus of the interaction. Second, the semiotic view of learning in SFL blurs the boundary of cognition and language use and provides a perspective for understanding the mediated role of language in the cognitive and social processes of learning. Finally, the functions of scaffolding in teacher talk revealed by SFL based on a linguistic analysis may not only enrich the description of each approach but also enable findings across the two approaches to be comparable. It is anticipated that SFL would create new spaces for the conversation between the two approaches.

1. Introduction

Teacher talk plays an important role in second-language classroom interaction in terms of providing students with input, eliciting output, and shaping their language production (Long, 1983, 1996; Krashen, 1985; Swain, 1985, 2005; van Lier, 1996, 2004). A systematic description of teacher talk is, therefore, central to understanding teachers' roles in classroom teaching and learning and has long been a research focus in second language acquisition (SLA) studies. Although studies are informed by multiple theories, they could be classified under two general approaches, namely cognitive and social (Firth and Wagner, 1997, 2007; Zuengler and Miller, 2006; Larsen-Freeman, 2007, 2018; Ellis, 2010). The cognitive approach emphasizes an individual learner's internal mental processing of language (Kasper, 1997; Long, 1997; Gass, 1998), perhaps leading to Doughty and Long (2003, p. 4) categorization of SLA as “a branch of cognitive science.” The social approach refers to a collective enterprise that espouses the essential effect of social factors on language learning (van Lier, 1996; Kramsch, 2002; Lantolf and Thorne, 2006; Lantolf, 2011). Whereas the cognitive approach has been dominant for years in the SLA field, the social approach has received increasing attention since the publication of Firth and Wagner (1997) article, followed by a number of commentaries (e.g., Kasper, 1997; Liddicoat, 1997; Long, 1997; Gass et al., 2007). Both approaches value the role of teachers in classroom interaction, but each shows its own concern for language learning. The cognitive side aims at understanding what type of teacher talk is effective for learners' cognitive processing of language; learning is measured by learners' correction of errors in language structures (Gass, 2003; Long, 2006). Instead, the social side perceives teacher talk as a resource for scaffolding interaction in meaningful activities; learners' participation in these activities is both the product and process of learning (van Lier, 1996; Lantolf and Thorne, 2006; Larsen-Freeman, 2007, 2018). The theoretical distinction has resulted in constant debate. The cognitive approach criticizes the social approach for not answering any question of acquisition (Long, 1997; Gass, 1998; Gass et al., 2007), whereas the social approach is unsatisfied with the cognitive approach for overlooking actual language use (Firth and Wagner, 1997; Kramsch and Whiteside, 2007). However, language learning is both a cognitive and social process; actually, there is a space for social participation in the cognitive approach and vice versa. Adherents to the cognitive approach commonly believe that learner participation in the conversation is essential to the cognitive processing of language (Long, 1996; Gass, 1997, 2003; Loewen and Sato, 2018); similarly, under the social approach, the socioculturalists maintain the mediated role of social interactions in learners' cognitive development (Vygotsky, 1978; van Lier, 1996; Lantolf and Thorne, 2006; Lantolf, 2011). Conversations are, thus, as necessary as debates because they are conducive to understanding how the two approaches are complementary as well as specifying what is gained and lost in the respective approach (Kramsch, 2002; Zuengler and Miller, 2006; Ortega, 2012).

Nonetheless, a real conversation is likely to be challenging, because the two approaches have developed distinct perspectives on what constitutes language and how learning evolves. This leads to their different descriptive paradigms of teacher talk (Lee, 2013). Wherever a conversation comes to a deadlock, an unlocking tool might be a theory from outside. Learning theories, either cognitive or social, focus on learning processes; however, the teacher talk involved in these processes is also a discourse behavior that can be examined systematically within a linguistic framework. This article attempts to reconcile the two approaches with reference to systemic functional linguistics (SFL) (Halliday, 1978, 2014), a meaning-oriented linguistic theory that has developed productive analytical frameworks for discourse analysis, with no exception for teacher talk (e.g., Christie, 2002; Schleppegrell, 2004; Christie and Derewianka, 2008; Rose, 2014). Before discussing what SFL may contribute to reconciling the two approaches in Section 4, we will conduct a brief review of the cognitive and social approaches in Sections 2 and 3.

2. Description of the cognitive approach

The cognitive approach evolves based on the development of formal linguistics and cognitive science. The formal view of language tends to dualize form and meaning: learning a language means acquiring language forms, including phonological, lexical, morphological, and syntactic forms. Meanings are attached to and conveyed by these forms from one mind to another; the role of language in shaping cognition is overlooked (Painter, 1999; Halliday and Burns, 2006). A privileging change in classroom research in comparison with behaviorism is an emphasis on learners as cognitive beings who actively get involved in the interaction (Larsen-Freeman, 2007). Teachers, as competent interlocutors, can facilitate the acquisition by providing comprehensible input and triggering learners' adjustments (Krashen, 1985; Swain, 1985, 2005; Long, 1996).

In teacher talk description, instances of teacher talk are classified into pre-specified categories and examined in relation to their effectiveness on acquisition (Larsen-Freeman, 2007; Ellis, 2010). A dichotomy is often adopted, such as display and referential questions (Long and Sato, 1983; Brock, 1986; Lee, 2006; McNeil, 2012), prompt and recast (Mackey and Goo, 2007; Lyster and Saito, 2010; Brown, 2016), explicit and implicit feedback (Panova and Lyster, 2002), or input-providing and output-promoting feedback (Ellis, 2008). The pre-determined categories provide a common basis for researchers to select empirical data and interpret them. Identifying empirical cases that include the properties of a given category becomes a key descriptive task (Lee, 2013).

These pre-given categories ensure the uniformity and integrity of research (Norris and Ortega, 2003); however, those umbrella terms are likely to incorporate a variety of features and cause interpretive ambiguity in empirical studies. Take recast as an illustration. The same concept may not refer to the same discourse phenomenon in different studies (Nicholas et al., 2001; Ellis and Sheen, 2006). Long (2006) defines recast as a reformulation that focuses on the meaning and does not interrupt the flow of communication; form-focused didactic reformulations are excluded. Sheen (2006), however, adopts a more general concept: provided that the overall purpose is communicative, recasts can be meaning-focused or form-focused. Ellis and Sheen (2006) use the following example to show their possible divergence in data coding.

Example 1

L: Korean is more faster.

T: Is Faster.

L: Is faster than English.

(Ellis and Sheen, 2006, p. 581).

According to Long (2006), the teacher's reformulation might not be qualified as a recast because the teacher appeared to understand what the learner meant, but corrected the grammatical error; language was temporarily treated as an object. According to Sheen (2006), however, the teacher's reformulation was a recast because the interaction as a whole was intended to be communicative despite the momentary focus on form.

Ellis and Sheen (2006) then contend that defining recasts based on teachers' intentions is problematic. They propose to subcategorize recasts depending on distinguishable linguistic features and maintain that taxonomical delicacy is fundamental to examine the effectiveness of recasts (also see Nicholas et al., 2001). Ellis and Sheen (2006) claim that there is no available theory to guide the sub-division. They then resort to the acquisition, in particular uptake. However, researchers seem satisfied with those general categories, which can be evidenced by a number of meta-analyses and review articles (e.g., Li, 2010; Lyster and Saito, 2010; Brown, 2016; Nassaji, 2020).

A lack of a linguistic tool might be a reason why Ellis and Sheen (2006) suggestion has not been widely adopted. Although acquisition, such as uptake, can help locate effective types of teacher talk, the linguistic features of teacher talk detected are likely to be partial. Moreover, it seems that teacher talk affects acquisition rather than the other way around. A detailed discourse analysis of teacher talk might contribute to building a stronger causal relationship between teacher talk and acquisition.

3. Description of the social approach

The social approach here refers to a collection of SLA studies informed by socially oriented theories or methodology, including Vygotskian sociocultural theory (Vygotsky, 1978; van Lier, 1996, 2004; Lantolf and Thorne, 2006; Lantolf, 2011), conversation analysis (Markee and Kasper, 2004; Seedhouse, 2005; Markee, 2008; Hall, 2019), and language socialization (Hymes, 1972; Kramsch, 2002; Watson-Gegeo and Nielsen, 2003; Duff and Talmy, 2011). Varied as these theories are, they foreground social and cultural contexts in understanding language and learning. Language development corresponds to the process of becoming socialized into effective participation in social activities; this socializing process is scaffolded by a more competent knower (Lantolf and Thorne, 2006; Lantolf, 2011). To quote Hall (2022, p. 100), “[i]ndividuals are socialized THROUGH language to USE language.”

An implication for classroom teaching is that teachers are the more competent knowers who create effectual interactional environments for learners to participate in and ultimately scaffold learners' language development (van Lier, 1996, 2004; Hall, 2022). The scaffolded assistance has been thoroughly discussed within the sociocultural theory (Wood et al., 1976; Vygotsky, 1978; Wertsch, 1985; van Lier, 1996, 2004; Lantolf and Thorne, 2006; Lantolf, 2011). McCormick and Donato (2000), e.g., examine six scaffolded roles of teacher questions following Wood et al. (1976) and Wertsch (1985): recruitment, reduction in degrees of freedom, direction maintenance, marking critical features, frustration control, and demonstration. McCormick and Donato (2000) describe how questions as dynamic discursive tools construct collaboration and facilitate comprehension. For example,

Example 2

T: (7-second wait) In other words, I guess what Sally is trying to ask is what were some of the flaws with the studies?

S: Yeah.

T: Can I say that? What were some of the flaws? (DM)

(teacher looks at students, moves toward the board) Flaws? (DM)

(writes the word flaw on board—Snulfkin enters class) Singular,

flaw. Hi Snulfkin. Does everybody know this word (DM)

Flaw? (DM)

(McCormick and Donato, 2000, p. 132).

Direction maintenance (DM) is the most pervasive scaffolded role of teacher talk found in the study (46%). The teacher used a series of questions to check students' understanding of the word flaw. The detailed transcription depicts what the teacher says and does in classroom interactions, such as writes the word flaw on the board. The analysis shows how the sequence of questions gets students involved in the task in order to maintain the direction of teaching toward “building student participation, comprehension and comprehensibility” (McCormick and Donato, 2000, p. 132). The interactional context of teaching goals is carefully considered. Similar to McCormick and Donato (2000), other branches of the social approach such as conversation analysis also prioritize the force of contexts in shaping teacher talk and language learning (see Seedhouse, 2005; Walsh, 2011; Waring, 2015; Kunitz et al., 2021).

Although the social approach acknowledges the shaping force of context in instances of interaction, the relationship between the two needs to be further strengthened (Hall, 2019, 2022). Walsh (2011) conducts a conversation analysis of classroom interaction. However, when discussing discourse features in the four types of contexts he classifies, i.e., managerial, material, skills and systems, and classroom, he refers to the general categorization of questions and feedback. For example, in the classroom context, teachers provide students with opportunities for genuine communication and are inclined to use referential questions and content feedback (Walsh, 2011). It appears difficult to mention those delicate language features identified in conversation analysis. The reason might be that those features are scattered across instances of interaction. It would be necessary to detect meaning patterns in order to consolidate the relationship between language features and context (Hasan, 2005; Byrnes, 2006; Hall, 2019, 2022).

4. The contribution of SFL

Sections 2 and 3 reviewed how teacher talk is described under the two approaches. The cognitivists bifurcate language forms and meaning and view learning as the result of cognition. Teacher talk is classified into pre-determined categories which are then examined regarding their relevance to acquisition. Subcategorization might be necessary to provide adequate evidence for how acquisition evolves out of interaction. The socialists highlight social and cultural contexts in interpreting language learning. The analysis looks carefully into how delicate language features in instances of interaction could scaffold learners' participation in social activities. The detection of meaning patterns might be required to consolidate the relationship between language features and contexts.

Systemic functional linguistics views language as a dynamic system that is constantly shaped by language use within social contexts (Halliday, 1978, 2014). In other words, SFL does not bifurcate form and meaning; rather, forms are naturally tied to meaning (Halliday, 1978, 2014). When applied to SLA, learning is conceived as a semiotic process in which learners develop meaning potentials through interpreting and making meaning in verbal and nonverbal interaction with others in social activities (Halliday, 1993; Matthiessen, 2009; Kress and van Leeuwen, 2020). Based on these views of language and learning, it is assumed that SFL can reconcile the two approaches in terms of expanding the learning scope, understanding the mediated role of language in learning, and offering a linguistic interpretation of scaffolding.

First, the meaning-oriented view of language in SFL extends the learning scope beyond language forms and offers both approaches an angle to reconsider the focus of the interaction. SFL conceives learning as learners' expansion of their language resources for meaning-making (Halliday, 1991, 1993; Feez, 2000; Matthiessen, 2009). Mohan and Beckett (2001) demonstrate the teacher's focus on meaning patterns by analyzing recasts in causal explanations in content-based language learning. Here is a selection of his analysis.

Example 3

S: To stop the brain's aging, we can use our bodies and heads

T: So, we can prevent our brain from getting weak

by being mentally and physically active?

(Mohan and Beckett, 2001, p. 137)

The student's articulation “we can use our bodies and heads” was grammatically correct and would be ignored by a form-focused analysis. The teacher, however, reformulated it into “by being mentally and physically active.” To be specific, the teacher reconstructed the structure from “to do X we can do Y” into “we can do X by doing Y,” thus making “doing Y” a dependent clause of means. From an SFL perspective, the recast is not a simple paraphrase, but a more academic expression of causal relationships based on the notion of grammatical metaphor, which is problematic across school levels, even for advanced learners (Achugar and Schleppegrell, 2005; Christie, 2012; Liardet, 2013; Schleppegrell, 2013; Gebhard et al., 2014; Humphrey and Macnaught, 2015). The earlier analysis does not imply that SFL overlooks grammatical rules in a meaning-oriented manner. The focus on meaning shifts the scaffolded role of teacher talk from correcting errors in rules to negotiating meaning patterns in discourse, providing a perspective to rethink the sphere of language learning.

Second, the semiotic view of learning in SFL provides a perspective for understanding the mediated role of language in learning and blurs the boundary of cognition and language use. Language is both an instance and a system, which are not independent but the same thing approached along a cline of instantiation (Halliday, 2014). Toward the instance pole are texts made of acts of meaning; toward the system pole lie meaning potentials emerging from acts. Between the two poles are intermediate levels of registers or text types (Halliday, 1991; Matthiessen, 2009). Learning occurs when a learner interacts with others through text, instantiating different registers and distilling meaning potential from acts of meaning (Halliday, 1991; Matthiessen, 2009). As an individual stretches and revises personalized meaning potential, the learner builds up a social semiotic for themselves (Halliday, 1974). Learning to mean is a personalized matter embedded in social processes. The learner's cognitive and social worlds, therefore, coalesce.

Finally, SFL could enrich the description of teacher talk in both approaches in terms of providing a linguistic interpretation of scaffolding. Within the SFL framework, the scaffolded role of teacher talk has been closely examined in relation to its multistratal and multifunctional system: the intersection of form, meaning, and contexts around the three metafunctions, i.e., ideational (constructing experiences), interpersonal (enacting relationships), and textual (organizing flows of information) (e.g., Christie, 2002; Rose and Martin, 2012; Gebhard et al., 2014; Rose, 2014; Macnaught, 2015; Brisk, 2016; Hood, 2017; Xie, 2021). Macnaught (2015), in particular, puts a linguistic interpretation on van Lier (1996, 2007) three levels of scaffolding concerning the curriculum, lesson, and task goal in writing classrooms. Take the task goal, for example, a prominent feature is the use of technical terminology to scaffold a writing task in the following aspects:

(a) labeling and identifying meanings as a type of meaning choice;

(b) increasing the specificity of expected meanings in setting up tasks;

(c) gathering more “like” responses to create a list of language options;

(d) relating multiple instances of language use to a general characteristic that they all share;

(e) labeling the function of text parts;

(f) exploring the reasoning and justification of language choices (Macnaught, 2015, p. 307).

Technical terms here are generalized based on their functions or meanings in interaction. These functions build up an intrinsic link between the linguistic choices of teacher talk and their scaffolded roles in recurring contexts. A functional analysis might serve as the basis to subcategorize teacher talk and specify the roles of scaffolding in the cognitive approach. The analysis also enables instances of teacher talk to be interwoven into a system based on their functions of scaffolding in the social approach (Hammond and Gibbons, 2005). Moreover, the sharing of the linguistic analysis of scaffolding, if possible, is likely to allow findings across the two approaches to be comparable.

5. Conclusion

This article first reviewed the cognitive and social approaches with respect to their descriptions of teacher talk based on their conceptualization of language and learning. The conversation between the two approaches is beset by bifurcations: form vs. meaning, psychological vs. sociocultural, and acquisition vs. participation. Next, the article proceeded to reconcile these bifurcations with reference to SFL. SFL appears naturally linked to the social approach in terms of its recognition of socialized learning; however, SFL may be conducive to specifying how teachers employ linguistic resources to scaffold the transition between acts of meaning and meaning potentials. In contrast, not all cognitivists agree with contextualized learning, or this is probably not their research focus; rather, they contribute robustly to understanding the teacher's facilitative role in learners' psychological processing of language. However, cognitive change evolves out of interaction (Long, 1996). SFL could at least offer a tool to demonstrate interactive functions of language and achieve taxonomical delicacy in teacher talk analysis. The linguistic interpretation would enrich the understanding of scaffolding that is involved in cognitive processes. In addition, language forms are essential to learning and have been examined in rigorous detail by cognitivists. SFL brings into view a more dynamic meaning perspective on forms. Hopefully, SFL would open up new spaces for the conversation between the cognitive and social approaches to SLA studies.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.

Funding

This research was sponsored by the Department of Education of Guangdong Province (Grant Nos: 2020WTSCX060, 2022WTSCX076, and 2022GXJK277) and Guangzhou Planning Office of Philosophy and Social Science (Grant No: 2020GZYB72).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Achugar, M., and Schleppegrell, M. J. (2005). Beyond connectors: the construction of cause in history textbooks. Linguist. Educ. 16, 298–318. doi: 10.1016/j.linged.2006.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brisk, M. (2016). “Genres in writing collaborative project,” in Exploring Literacies: Theory, Research and Practice, eds. H. de Silva Joyce, and S. Feez (Basingstoke, UK and New York: Palgrave Macmillan), 287–292.

Brock, C. A. (1986). The effects of referential questions on ESL classroom discourse. TESOL Q. 20, 47–59. doi: 10.2307/3586388

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, D. (2016). The type and linguistic foci of oral corrective feedback in the L2 classroom: a meta-analysis. Lang. Teach. Res. 20, 436–458. doi: 10.1177/13621688145632

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrnes, H., (ed.). (2006). Advanced Language Learning: The Contributions of Halliday and Vygotsky. London: Continuum.

Christie, F. (2002). Classroom Discourse Analysis. London: Continuum.

Christie, F. (2012). Language Education Throughout the School Years: A Functional Perspective. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.

Christie, F., and Derewianka, B. (2008). School Discourse: Learning to Write Across the Years of Schooling. London: Continuum.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Doughty, C. J., and Long, M. H. (2003). “The scope of inquiry and goals of SLA,” in The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition, eds. C. J. Doughty and M. H. Long (Malden, MA: Blackwell), 1–16.

Google Scholar

Duff, P., and Talmy, S. (2011). “Language socialization approaches to second language acquisition: social, cultural, and linguistic development in additional languages,” in Alternative Approaches to SLA, ed. D. Atkinson (London: Routledge), 95–116.

Google Scholar

Ellis, R. (2008). The Study of Second Language Acquisition (2nd edition). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Ellis, R. (2010). “Theoretical pluralism in SLA: Is there a way forward,” in Conceptualizing ‘Learning' in Applied Linguistics, eds. P. Seedhouse, S. Walsh, and C. Jenks (London: Palgrave Macmillan), 23–51.

Google Scholar

Ellis, R., and Sheen, Y. (2006). Reexamining the role of recasts in second language acquisition. Stud. Second Lang. Acquisit. 28, 575–600. doi: 10.1017/S027226310606027X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feez, S. (2000). “Heritage and innovation in second language acquisition,” in Genre in the Classroom: Multiple Perspectives, ed. A. M. Johns (Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 43–69.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Firth, A., and Wagner, J. (1997). On discourse, communication, and (some) fundamental concepts in SLA research. Mod. Lang. J. 81, 285–300. doi: 10.2307/329302

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Firth, A., and Wagner, J. (2007). Second/Foreign language learning as a social accomplishment: elaborations on a reconceptualized SLA. Mod. Lang. J. 91, 800–819. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4781.2007.00670.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gass, S. (1997). Input, Interaction, and the Development of Second Languages. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.

Google Scholar

Gass, S. (1998). Apples and oranges: Or, why apples are not orange and don't need to be—a response to Firth and Wagner. Mod. Lang. J. 82, 83–90. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4781.1998.tb02597.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gass, S. (2003). “Input and interaction,” in The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition, eds. C. J. Doughty and M. H. Long (Malden, MA: Blackwell), 224–256.

Google Scholar

Gass, S., Lee, J., and Roots, R. (2007). Firth and Wagner (1997): new ideas or a new articulation? Mod. Lang. J. 91, 788–799. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4781.2007.00669.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gebhard, M., Chen, I. A., and Britton, L. (2014). “Miss, nominalization is a nominalization:” English language learners' use of SFL metalanguage and their literacy practices. Linguistics and Education 26, 106–125. doi: 10.1016/j.linged.2014.01.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, J. K. (2019). The contributions of conversation analysis and interactional linguistics to a usage-based understanding of language: Expanding the transdisciplinary framework. Mod. Lang. J. 103, 80–94. doi: 10.1111/modl.12535

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, J. K. (2022). L2 classroom interaction and its links to L2 learners' developing L2 linguistic repertoires: a research agenda. Lang. Teach. 55, 100–115. doi: 10.1017/S0261444820000397

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halliday, M., and Burns, A. (2006). Applied linguistics: thematic pursuits or disciplinary moorings. J. Appl. Linguist. 3, 113–128. doi: 10.1558/japl.v3i1.113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halliday, M. A. K. (1974). A sociosemiotic perspective on language development. Bull. School Orient. Afric. Stud. 37, 98–118. doi: 10.1017/S0041977X0009488X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halliday, M. A. K. (1978). Language as Social Semiotic: The Social Interpretation of Language and Meaning. London: Edward Arnold.

Google Scholar

Halliday, M. A. K. (1991). “The notion of “context” in language education,” in The Language of Education–Collected Works of M. A. K. Halliday, Vol. 9, ed. J. J. Webster (London: Continuum), 269–290.

Google Scholar

Halliday, M. A. K. (1993). Towards a language-based theory of learning. Linguist. Educ. 5, 93–116. doi: 10.1016/0898-5898(93)90026-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halliday, M. A. K. (2014). An Introduction to Functional Grammar (Revised by C. M. I. M. Matthiessen). London and New York: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Hammond, J., and Gibbons, P. (2005). Putting scaffolding to work: the contribution of scaffolding in articulating ESL education. Prospect 20, 6–30.

Google Scholar

Hasan, R. (2005). “Semiotic mediation, language and society: three exotripic theories-Vygotsky, Halliday and Bernstein” in Language, Society and Consciousness, ed. J. J. Webster (Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia), 46–67.

Google Scholar

Hood, S. (2017). Live lectures: The significance of presence in building disciplinary knowledge. Onomázein (Special Issue) 179–208. doi: 10.7764/onomazein.sfl.07

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Humphrey, S., and Macnaught, L. (2015). Functional language instruction and the writing growth of English language learners in the middle years. TESOL Q. 50, 792–816. doi: 10.1002/tesq.247

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hymes, D. (1972). “On communicative competence,” in Sociolinguistics, eds J. B. Pride and J. Holmes (London: Penguin), 269–293.

Google Scholar

Kasper, G. (1997). “A” stands for acquisition: a response to Firth and Wagner. The Mod. Lang. J. 81, 307–312. doi: 10.2307/329304

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramsch, C., (ed.). (2002). Language Acquisition and Language Socialization. London and New York: Continuum.

Google Scholar

Kramsch, C., and Whiteside, A. (2007). Three fundamental concepts in second language acquisition and their relevance in multilingual contexts. Mod. Lang. J. 91, 907–922. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4781.2007.00677.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krashen, S. (1985). The Input Hypothesis: Issues and Implications. Harlow: Longman.

Google Scholar

Kress, G., and van Leeuwen, T. (2020). Reading Images: The Grammar of Visual Design (3rd edition). London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Kunitz, S., Markee, N., and Sert, O. (2021). Classroom-Based Conversation Analytic Research. Berlin: Springer International Publishing.

Google Scholar

Lantolf, J. (2011). “The sociocultural approach to second language acquisition: Sociocultural theory, second language acquisition, and artificial L2 development,” in Alternative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition, ed. D. Atkinson (New York: Routledge), 24–47.

Google Scholar

Lantolf, J. P., and Thorne, J. L. S. (2006). Sociocultural Theory and the Genesis of Second Language Development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Larsen-Freeman, D. (2007). Reflecting on the cognitive–social debate in second language acquisition. Mod. Lang. J. 91, 773–787. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4781.2007.00668.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larsen-Freeman, D. (2018). Looking ahead: future directions in, and future research into, second language acquisition. Foreign Lang. Ann. 51, 55–72. doi: 10.1111/flan.12314

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, Y. A. (2006). Respecifying display questions: Interactional resources for language teaching. TESOL Q. 40, 691–713. doi: 10.2307/40264304

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, Y. A. (2013). Descriptions of second language interaction: toward descriptive adequacy. Mod. Lang. J. 97, 853–868. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4781.2013.12041.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, S. (2010). The effectiveness of corrective feedback in SLA: a meta analysis. Lang. Learn. 60, 309–365. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2010.00561.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liardet, C. (2013). An exploration of Chinese EFL learner's deployment of grammatical metaphor: learning to make academically valued meanings. J. Second Lang. Writ. 22, 161–178. doi: 10.1016/j.jslw.2013.03.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liddicoat, A. (1997). Interaction, social structure, and second language use: a response to Firth and Wagner. Mod. Lang. J. 81, 313–317. doi: 10.2307/329305

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loewen, S., and Sato, M. (2018). Interaction and instructed second language acquisition. Lang. Teach. 51, 285–329. doi: 10.1017/S0261444818000125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Long, M. H. (1983). Native speaker/non-native speaker conversation and the negotiation of comprehensible input. Appl. Linguist. 4, 126–141. doi: 10.1093/applin/4.2.126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Long, M. H. (1996). “The role of the linguistic environment in second language acquisition,” in Handbook of Second Language Acquisition, eds. W. Ritchie and T. Bhatia (San Diego: Academic Press), 413–468.

Google Scholar

Long, M. H. (1997). Construct validity in SLA research: a response to Firth and Wagner. Mod. Lang. J. 81, 318–323. doi: 10.2307/329306

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Long, M. H. (2006). Problems in SLA. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Long, M. H., and Sato, C. J. (1983). “Classroom foreigner talk discourse: forms and functions of teachers' questions,” in Classroom Oriented Research in Second Language Acquisition, eds. H. W. Seliger and M. H. Long (Rowley, MA.: Newbury House), 268–285.

Google Scholar

Lyster, R., and Saito, K. (2010). Oral feedback in classroom SLA: a meta-analysis. Stud. Second Lang. Acquisit. 32, 265–302. doi: 10.1017/S0272263109990520

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mackey, A., and Goo, J. (2007). “Interaction research in SLA: a meta-analysis and research synthesis,” in Conversational Interaction in SLA: A Collection of Empirical Studies, ed. A. Mackey (New York: Oxford University Press), 408–452.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Macnaught, L. (2015). Classroom Talk and the Negotiation of Academic English: A Linguistic Analysis of Collaborative Text Creation. Unpublished PhD thesis. University of Technology, Sydney.

Google Scholar

Markee, N. (2008). Toward a learning behavior tracking methodology for CA-for-SLA. Appl. Linguist. 29, 404–427. doi: 10.1093/applin/amm052

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markee, N., and Kasper, G. (2004). Classroom talks: an introduction. Mod. Lang. J. 88, 491–500.

Google Scholar

Matthiessen, C. M. (2009). Meaning in the making: Meaning potential emerging from acts of meaning. Lang. Learn. 59, 206–229. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2009.00541.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormick, D. E., and Donato, R. (2000). “Teacher questions as scaffolded assistance,” in Second and Foreign Language Learning Through Classroom Interaction, eds. K. Hall and L. S. Verplaetse (Mahwah, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum), 183–201.

Google Scholar

McNeil, L. (2012). Using talk to scaffold referential questions for English language learners. Teach. Teach. Educ. 28, 396–404. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2011.11.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohan, B., and Beckett, G. H. (2001). A functional approach to research on content-based language learning: recasts in causal explanations. Can. Mod. Lang. Rev. 58, 133–155. doi: 10.3138/cmlr.58.1.133

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nassaji, H. (2020). Assessing the effectiveness of interactional feedback for L2 acquisition: issues and challenges. Lang. Teach. 53, 3–28. doi: 10.1017/S0261444819000375

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicholas, H., Lightbown, P. M., and Spada, N. (2001). Recasts as feedback to language learners. Lang. Learn. 51, 719–758. doi: 10.1111/0023-8333.00172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norris, J., and Ortega, L. (2003). “Defining and measuring SLA,” in The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition, eds. C. J. Doughty and M. H. Long (Malden, MA: Blackwell), 717–761.

Google Scholar

Ortega, L. (2012). Epistemological diversity and moral ends of research in instructed SLA. Lang. Teach. Res. 16, 206–226. doi: 10.1177/0267658311431373

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Painter, C. (1999). Learning Through Language in Early Childhood. London: Cassell.

Google Scholar

Panova, I., and Lyster, R. (2002). Patterns of corrective feedback and uptake in an adult ESL classroom. TESOL Q. 36, 573–595. doi: 10.2307/3588241

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rose, D. (2014). Analyzing pedagogic discourse: an approach from genre and register. Funct. Linguist. 1, 1–32. doi: 10.1186/s40554-014-0011-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rose, D., and Martin, J. R. (2012). Learning to Write, Reading to Learn: Genre, Knowledge and Pedagogy in the Sydney School. London: Equinox.

Google Scholar

Schleppegrell, M. (2004). The Language of Schooling: A Functional Linguistics Perspective. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Schleppegrell, M. (2013). The role of meta-language in supporting academic language development. Lang. Learn. 63, 153–170. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2012.00742.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seedhouse, P. (2005). Conversation analysis and language learning. Lang. Teach. 38, 165–187. doi: 10.1017/S0261444805003010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheen, Y. (2006). Exploring the relationship between characteristics of recasts and learner uptake. Lang. Teach. Res. 10, 361–392. doi: 10.1191/1362168806lr203o

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swain, M. (1985). “Communicative competence: some roles of comprehensible input and comprehensible output in its development,” in Input in Second Language Acquisition, eds. S. Gass and C. Madden (Rowley, Mass.: Newbury House), 235–253.

Google Scholar

Swain, M. (2005). “The output hypothesis: theory and research,” in Handbook of Research in Second Language Teaching and Learning, ed. E. Hinkel (Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 471–483.

Google Scholar

van Lier, L. (1996). Interaction in the Language Curriculum: Awareness, Autonomy and Authenticity. London: Longman.

Google Scholar

van Lier, L. (2004). The Ecology and Semiotics of Language Learning: A Sociocultural Perspective. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Kluwer.

Google Scholar

van Lier, L. (2007). Action-based teaching, autonomy and Identity. Innov. Lang. Learn. Teach. 1, 46–65. doi: 10.2167/illt42.0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Mental Process. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Walsh, S. (2011). Exploring Classroom Discourse: Language in Action. London and New York: Taylor and Francis.

Google Scholar

Waring, H. Z. (2015). Theorizing Pedagogical Interaction: Insights From Conversation Analysis. New York: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Watson-Gegeo, K., and Nielsen, S. (2003). “Language socialization in SLA,” in The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition, eds. C. Doughty and M. Long (Malden, MA: Blackwell), 155–177.

Google Scholar

Wertsch, J. V. (1985). Vygotsky and the Social Formation of Mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Wood, D., Bruner, J. S., and Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 25, 45–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1976.tb00381.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xie, M. (2021). Enabling students' knowledge building in English classrooms in China: the role of teacher monologue. Text Talk 41, 261–285. doi: 10.1515/text-2019-0126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuengler, J., and Miller, E. R. (2006). Cognitive and sociocultural perspectives: two parallel SLA worlds? TESOL Q. 40, 35–58. doi: 10.2307/40264510

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: teacher talk, cognitive approach, social approach, systemic functional linguistics (SFL), second language classrooms

Citation: Xie M and Tu B (2023) Reconciling the cognitive and social approaches to describing teacher talk in second language classrooms: The contribution of systemic functional linguistics. Front. Commun. 8:1072880. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2023.1072880

Received: 18 October 2022; Accepted: 09 January 2023;
Published: 02 February 2023.

Edited by:

John W. Schwieter, Wilfrid Laurier University, Canada

Reviewed by:

Mary Macken-Horarik, Australian Catholic University, Australia
Inmaculada Fortanet-Gomez, University of Jaume I, Spain

Copyright © 2023 Xie and Tu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Binglan Tu, yes 22-125@gduf.edu.cn

Download