Skip to content
BY 4.0 license Open Access Published by De Gruyter May 12, 2023

Zero affixes in derivational morphology: Introduction

  • Gianina Iordăchioaia EMAIL logo and Chiara Melloni

Abstract

In this paper we offer an overview of the linguistic phenomena that have traditionally been handled by means of zero affixes and of the theoretical debate around the advantages and disadvantages of employing such null morphemes in theoretical modeling. While the advantage of positing zero affixes is straightforward from an empirical perspective (see inflectional syncretism and affixless category change, among others), their theoretical legitimacy has been controversially debated for several decades. In this overview, we present the main problems that have been brought forward against zero affixation and some of the mechanisms that have been proposed as an alternative. Finally, we show how the different articles in our edited collection contribute to this debate by addressing the following three research questions: (1) How do current theories of derivational morphology deal with linguistic phenomena that seem to involve zero affixes? (2) How do zero derivational affixes compare with their “overt analogues” (i. e. overt derivational affixes)? (3) What insights can we obtain from language-specific properties, typological generalizations and/or larger data-oriented studies on the question whether zero affixation is necessary in derivational morphology?

1 State of the art in zero derivation

Since the early days of Sanskrit grammars, zero affixes, i. e. phonologically null morphology with syntactic and/or semantic content, have proved instrumental in describing language and have consistently been employed in modern structuralist linguistics and their later adherent theories (see Bergenholtz and Mugdan 2000; Bauer and Valera 2005; Dahl and Fábregas 2018 for overviews).

1.1 The empirical challenge

Natural language knowingly makes rich use of morphologically identical items for different functions. Inflectionally poor languages like English often show paradigmatic syncretisms such as in (1), where the present tense is overtly realized only in the third person singular form. But even when the language has a morpheme dedicated to a particular function such as the nominal plural in (2a), this may fail to be overtly realized in some items as in (2b):

(1)
a.

I walk1sg

b.

you walk2sg

c.

she walk-s 3sg

(2)
a.

dogdog-s pl

b.

sheepsheep pl

If the absence of a signifier challenges the traditional conception of the Saussurean sign, the postulation of zero morphemes for the corresponding functions, as illustrated in (1) and (2), facilitates a way to maintain the one-to-one mapping between form and meaning in puzzling cases in which a difference in meaning is not accompanied by a phonological form.

Besides inflectional paradigms, zero affixes have largely been postulated for derivational processes, which represent the focus of this collection. The standard phenomenon here is that of category change, illustrated in (3), but zero morphology has also been employed to account for labile verbs in argument structure alternations such as the causative-inchoative illustrated in (4). More complex constructions such as the Italian nominalized infinitive in (5) may also be argued to involve zero affixation at the interface between morphology and syntax.

(3)
a.

a table N > to tableV

b.

to walk V > a walkN

c.

clean A > to cleanV

(4)
a.

The boy broke the vase.

b.

The vase broke.

(5)

Il suo scrivere frasi lunghissime e prive di punteggiatura (mi irrita.)
the his write.inf very longish and devoid of punctuation (me bothers)
‘His writing very long sentences without punctuation (bothers me.)’

While in languages like English, the two forms related by a zero affix are perfectly identical as in (1)–(4), inflectionally richer languages like Italian or German may show small formal differences triggered by phonological changes such as ablaut and umlaut in (6) and by declension classes with gender marking as in (7).

(6)

brech-en ‘to break’ > ein BruchN ‘a break’
ein Kampf ‘a fight’ > kämpfV-en ‘to fight’
(German)

(7)

passaggi-are ‘to walk’ > un passaggiN- o masc ‘a walk’
cosegn-are ‘to deliver’ > una consegnN- a fem ‘a delivery’
(Italian)

Besides such formal differences, the productivity of morphological phenomena that involve zero affixes, in general, is higher in languages like English than in inflectionally richer languages. The reasons for this difference relate to the morphological type of the language, language-specific morphophonological constraints and the status of the base (root vs. stem vs. word) (Bauer and Valera 2005; Don 2005; Valera 2015).

1.2 The theoretical debate

While the advantage of positing zero affixes is straightforward from an empirical perspective, their theoretical legitimacy has been controversially debated for several decades already (see Lieber 1981; 1992; 2004; Kiparsky 1982; Anderson 1992; Don 1993; 2005; Pesetsky 1995; Plag 1999; Mel’čuk 2002; Borer 2013 and the recent overview in Dahl and Fábregas 2018). In general, positing a contentful zero morpheme confronts us with the theoretical quandary of differentiating it from the lack of a morpheme altogether: how do we know where there is a zero and where there is nothing?

A reasonable condition for positing a zero affix is the existence of an overt affix with a similar function (i. e. its “overt analogue” in Sanders 1988). In inflectional paradigms one easily finds such evidence, as shown in (1), but for derivation it is not always straightforward. For conversion from verbs to nouns in English, for instance, we find zero and overtly marked pairs such as in (8), but it is debatable whether this zero affix has a coherent enough meaning to be retrieved from all such deverbal nouns: the noun climb-Ø denotes an event or a path, break-Ø may describe an event, a state or a result entity, and cook-Ø unambiguously denotes an agent, an interpretation otherwise contributed by the participant-denoting -er suffix. Therefore, Ø emerges here as a nominalizing suffix with a very polysemous meaning.

(8)
to climb > the climb-Ø the climb-ing
to break > the break-Ø the break-ing the break-age
to cook > the cook-Ø the cook-ing (cf. the cook-er )

Following a strong interpretation of the “overt analogue criterion” (Sanders 1988) for English denominal verbs as in (3a), it has been argued, for instance, that a zero suffix would be semantically too diverse to form a lexical entry by comparison with overt verbalizing suffixes such as -ify and -ize (Plag 1999; Lieber 2004). Another reservation about allowing zero affixes comes from the undesired effect of zero proliferation and the indeterminability of their ordering in the absence of sound criteria for their existence or interaction with each other (Anderson 1992; Bergenholtz and Mugdan 2000; Dahl and Fábregas 2018). However, in separationist theories such as Distributed Morphology (Halle and Marantz 1993; Marantz 2013; cf. Beard 1995), zero affixes represent a morphophonological reality, which is distinct from the morphosyntax and the interpretation of complex words. No lexical entries need to be posited for them, and their ordering comes from the underlying morphosyntax and semantics.

Among the various mechanisms that have been put forward as an alternative to zero affixation, we find relisting, underspecification and cumulative exponence. Relisting is a form of lexical coinage triggered by pragmatic needs and has been proposed by Lieber (1981; 1992; 2004) for conversion phenomena. In underspecification theories, the lexical category of words in conversion pairs gets specified only in a syntactic context (see Farrell 2001 and Borer 2013). In her syntax-based Exo-Skeletal Model, for instance, Borer (2013) takes both zero-derived verbs and nouns as in (3a, b) to be formed by means of a verbal/nominal external projection such as T(ense) and D(eterminer) attached to a category neutral root. Whether it is a verb or a noun that ensues depends strictly on the categorial nature of this syntactic projection.

Zero morphemes are also avoided in frameworks like A-Morphous Morphology (Anderson 1992) or Word-and-Paradigm models (Blevins 2016; Bonami and Strnadová 2019), which dispense with traditional morphemes altogether. In these theories, cumulative exponence is argued for cases where a word form expresses more than one morphosyntactic feature but not all features have a corresponding (sublexical) exponent. Zero exponence receives yet another explanation in frameworks such as Brody’s Mirror Theory (2000) or Starke’s Nanosyntax (2009), allowing a single morphophonological exponent to lexicalize multiple morphosyntactic features. Specifically, in Nanosyntax mechanisms like phrasal spellout (i. e. the insertion of vocabulary items into nonterminal nodes) and spanning (i. e. the lexicalization of a sequence of heads by a single vocabulary item) are deployed to explain the cumulative exponence of portmanteau morphemes, avoiding the postulation of zero morphemes (Caha 2018; Taraldsen 2018).

However, it has been argued that such mechanisms fail to capture relevant morphosyntactic and phonological constraints which would be explained under a zero affix approach (see Don 1993; 2005; Darby 2015). For instance, in English denominal zero-derived verbs as in (3a) are fully productive, while deverbal zero-derived nouns as in (3b) are more limited. Treating both by means of the same mechanism does not account for this difference, while two different zero affixes would account for the different productivity levels.

Directionality has often been invoked in support of a zero affix derivational approach for category change phenomena, and, depending on it, proposals have been made that some formations may require a zero affix analysis, while others should be accounted for by other means. Lieber (1981: ch. 3) first proposed to treat typical category change as a non-directional process and to employ zero affixes only exceptionally for morphologically systematic formations such as adjectival participles built from verbal participles. More recently, Darby (2015) and Darby and Lahiri (2016) have used psycholinguistic evidence to argue that in category change some noun-verb pairs show clear directional behavior, while others are underspecified. This suggests the need for two approaches: a derivational and an underspecified approach, respectively. Further support for the need of both approaches is also provided by the computational study in Kisselew et al. (2016). From their investigation of distributional semantic models, they conclude that zero-derived nouns in English lend themselves to a derivational analysis, while the behavior of zero-derived verbs conforms to an underspecified analysis.

Experimental support for derivational zero affixes in general has also been provided by neurolinguistic studies. Pliatsikas et al. (2014) reports the results of an fMRI experiment where they compared the processing of one-step (soaking < soak-V) and two-step (bridging < bridge-V < bridge-N) morphologically derived words. Interestingly, they found that, despite similar surface structure, two-step derivations trigger higher processing costs than one-step derivations, hence providing critical evidence for the morphological complexity of zero-derived forms as processed in the brain. These results are also confirmed by the psycholinguistic studies of Darby (2015) and Darby and Lahiri (2016), which employ lexical decision tasks with masked and delayed priming.

2 Contribution of the present volume

This collection aims to contribute to the debate on zero affixes in derivational morphology by addressing the following three research questions:

  1. How do theories of derivational morphology deal with linguistic phenomena that seem to require the use of zero affixes? How do they motivate the need for zeros and how do they implement them, or why do they argue against zeros and how do they technically do without them?

  2. How do zero derivational affixes compare with their “overt analogues” (i. e. overt derivational affixes)?

  3. What insights can we obtain from language-specific properties, typological generalizations and/or larger data-oriented studies on the question whether zero affixation is necessary in derivational morphology?

With respect to the first question, the volume collects insights from both lexicalist and syntactic orientations in theories of morphology, both of which may or may not employ zero affixes. Cetnarowska accounts for V-to-N conversion in Polish by positing several construction schemas in sign-based Construction Morphology (Booij 2010) to capture the variety of conversion nouns with respect to meaning, gender and inflectional class in an inflectionally rich language. Another lexicalist perspective is offered by Koutsoukos and Ralli, who employ a zero derivational suffix to account for the formal and semantic properties of a peculiar type of Greek adjectives with the alpha privative prefix ([a+noun]Ø). They argue that this type of formation can be likened to exocentric compounds in Modern Greek (Ralli 2013; 2022). Caha, De Clercq and Vanden Wyngaerd present an account of change of state verbs alternating between inchoative and causative forms in English, Dutch and Czech, by following Ramchand’s (2008) theory of event structure and the mechanism of phrasal spellout in Nanosyntax (Starke 2009). For instance, as a causative verb, narrow spells out a constituent that includes the adjectival state, the become inchoative event and the causative component at the same time, while theories that posit zero suffixes would take the latter two parts to be covertly realized. Drawing mainly on French data, Sleeman discusses affixless category change from a syntactic perspective. She investigates two problems that V-to-N conversions and A/N parallel suffixes raise for Borer’s (2013) Exo-Skeletal Model, which bans zero affixes for derivation, and proposes an extension of this model to include functional mixed categories and suffixal roots. Iordăchioaia and Melloni use corpus examples and manually annotated datasets of zero-derived nominals in English and Italian to highlight previously unaddressed similarities between zeros and their overt analogues (see our second research question). This evidence supports a Distributed Morphology analysis, in which zero is just a null spellout of an abstract nominalizing suffix, otherwise instantiated by overt suffixes (Alexiadou and Grimshaw 2008; Embick 2010). Zero and the other overt realizations may compete for the same roots and meanings but this competition is often resolved by different meaning specializations.

Most contributions approach our second research question by comparing zero affixes among themselves, with their overt analogues or with inflectional affixes. Dal Maso and Piccinin investigate the online processing of zero-affixed verbal forms by native speakers of Italian during a task of visual word recognition. They compare the processing of zero affixation with that of overt morphological processes that are productive in Italian: suffixation and parasynthesis. Interestingly, they find that, notwithstanding the lack of a phonological form, the processing of zero-affixed verbs resembles that of suffixed verbs, while it differs from that of parasynthetic/prefixed verbs. Valera addresses the relationship between zero affixation and overt analogues in the verbal domain of Spanish. His corpus-based study compares the list of semantic categories attested in denominal verbs formed by zero-derivation and by counterpart overt affixes (-ar, -ecer, -(i)ficar, -izar, a-…-ar and en-…-ar). The results highlight commonalities and differences among these processes. Without committing to a particular framework, these findings shed light on the proximity between zero derivation and overt suffixation, in line with the psycholinguistic evidence reported by Dal Maso and Piccinin for Italian. Mititelu, Leseva and Stoyanova use the English V-N pairs in the Princeton WordNet to explore the characteristics of the zero derivational morpheme from two main perspectives: productivity and semantic load. They identify the semantic relations expressed by zero affixation in comparison with overt affixes involved in nominal and verbal derivations and discuss their similarities and differences with respect to productivity, semantic load and clusters of semantic primes.

From the perspective of our third research question, these contributions turn out to be complementary on multiple levels – theoretical, empirical, and methodological –, which only confirms once again the complexity of all these aspects in the study of zero derivation. First, from the theory-oriented studies we observe that there is no definite cut between lexicalist and syntactic theories in terms of use or dismissal of zero affixes. While Cetnarowska argues for the use of constructional schemas to capture the semantic and inflectional diversity of deverbal zero nouns in Polish, Ralli and Koutsoukos employ a zero suffix in a well-defined and restricted type of morphological formations in Greek, which does not raise the zero proliferation problem that Cetnarowska aims to avoid. In the Distributed Morphology approach to zero nouns proposed by Iordăchioaia and Melloni, zero proliferation is not as stringent a problem as in lexicalist frameworks, which rely on lexical entries for affixes. Namely, functional morphemes have no lexical entries – they are entirely abstract, and the late insertion mechanism makes their syntactic and semantic features independent of their post-syntactic (overt or covert) phonological realizations (Harley and Noyer 1999). The analysis of alternating causative verbs in Caha et al. employs no zero affixes but uses instead phrasal spellout and the superset principle to account for the multiple syntactic realizations of a single lexical entry. Sleeman’s study also sheds light on different phenomena of category flexibility which, in an intralinguistic perspective, allow us to appreciate alternative explanations that dispense with ad-hoc stipulations of zero affixes.

Second, the empirical challenge is also multifaceted: an account with zero affixes may be disadvantageous for zero formations that would require several homophonous affixes in a lexicalist framework (as discussed in Cetnarowska) but handy in the case of special constructions such as prefixed denominal adjectives in Greek, as in Ralli and Koutsoukos. The data-oriented (corpus-based and experimental) studies help to identify various systematic patterns in zero derivation which shed light on directionality, base selection and semantics, as well as on the relation between zeros and their overt counterparts (see Dal Maso and Piccinin’s, Valera’s, Mititelu et al.’s, and Iordăchioaia and Melloni’s works).

Third, from a methodological perspective, this collection presents a balanced body of work, where theory-oriented introspection- and elicitation-based studies (Cetnarowska, Ralli and Koutsoukos, Caha et al.) are complemented by corpus and experimental methodology (Valera, Dal Maso and Piccinin), as well as quantitative analyses of annotated datasets, drawing on computational and lexicographic resources (Valera, Mititelu et al., Iordăchioaia and Melloni).

The conclusion we can draw from these contributions together is that positing derivational zero affixes is both theory- and language-dependent. Theories present different grounding principles and strategies that can successfully accommodate zero affixes or do without them, while the empirical domain shows that different derivational patterns within and across languages may require the use of zero affixes or make it unnecessary. A specific result on the nature of derivational zeros comes out of the two studies on Italian and Spanish by Dal Maso and Piccinin and by Valera, respectively, whose experimental and corpus-based analyses suggest that category-changing zero resembles suffixes more than prefixes. This lends support to the other approaches which handle this type of formations as involving a zero suffix.

The greatest appeal of our collection lies in its bringing together various theoretical approaches relying on construction-based, lexicalist and syntax-oriented frameworks, and complementing such theory-guided insights with corpus-informed and experimental methodology. We believe that the complementarity between the different contributions from different view angles can foster a productive exchange between theory and methodology in linguistics with application to zero derivation/conversion and beyond. To the best of our knowledge, it also represents the first collection on zero affixation since Bauer and Valera’s (2005) edited volume Approaches to conversion/zero-derivation. While the contributions in this volume are mainly theoretical, our collection has the advantage of offering an update that reflects the current methodological diversity in linguistic research with application to zero affixation.

Acknowledgment

We gratefully acknowledge the editorial board of Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft and our handling editor Natascha Pomino, the contributors to this special issue, and all the participants in the SLE 2020 Workshop “Derivational zero affixes”, which inspired the present editorial project.

Iordăchioaia’s contribution and her collaboration with Melloni were funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) – project number 404208593. The final stage of Iordăchioaia’s editorial work was supported by the Postdoc Network Brandenburg (PNB) via a PNB Individual Grant for Postdoctoral Researchers.

References

Alexiadou, Artemis & Jane Grimshaw. 2008. Verbs, nouns and affixation. In Florian Schäfer (ed.), Working papers of the SFB 732: Incremental specification in context, 1–16. Stuttgart: University of Stuttgart.Search in Google Scholar

Anderson, Stephen R. 1992. A-morphous morphology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511586262Search in Google Scholar

Bauer, Laurie & Salvador Valera. 2005. Conversion or zero-derivation: An introduction. In Laurie Bauer & Salvador Valera (eds.), Approaches to conversion/zero-derivation, 7–17. Münster: Waxmann.Search in Google Scholar

Beard, Robert. 1995. Lexeme-morpheme base morphology. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.Search in Google Scholar

Bergenholtz, Henning & Joachim Mugdan. 2000. Nullelemente in der Morphologie. In Geert Booij, Christian Lehmann & Joachim Mugdan (eds.), Morphology. An international handbook of inflection and word-formation, vol. 1, 435–450. Berlin & New York: De Gruyter.10.1515/9783110111286.1.6.435Search in Google Scholar

Blevins, James P. 2016. Word and paradigm morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199593545.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Booij, Geert. 2010. Construction Morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1111/j.1749-818X.2010.00213.xSearch in Google Scholar

Bonami, Olivier & Jana Strnadová. 2019. Paradigm structure and predictability in derivational morphology. Morphology 29(2). 167–197.10.1007/s11525-018-9322-6Search in Google Scholar

Borer, Hagit. 2013. Structuring sense: Volume III: Taking form. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199263936.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Brody, Michael. 2000. Mirror theory: Syntactic representation in perfect syntax. Linguistic Inquiry 31(1). 29–56.10.1162/002438900554280Search in Google Scholar

Caha, Pavel. 2018. Notes on insertion in Distributed Morphology and Nanosyntax. In Lena Baunaz, Liliane Haegeman, Karen De Clercq & Eric Lander (eds.), Exploring Nanosyntax, 57–87. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/oso/9780190876746.003.0002Search in Google Scholar

Dahl, Eystein & Antonio Fábregas. 2018. Zero morphemes (Oxford research encyclopedia of linguistics). Oxford University Press. 10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.013.592.Search in Google Scholar

Darby, Jeannique. 2015. The processing of conversion in English: Morphological complexity and underspecification. PhD dissertation, University of Oxford.Search in Google Scholar

Darby, Jeannique & Aditi Lahiri. 2016. Covert morphological structure and the processing of zero-derived words. The Mental Lexicon 11(2). 186–215.10.1075/ml.11.2.02darSearch in Google Scholar

Don, Jan. 1993. Morphological conversion. PhD dissertation, Utrecht University.Search in Google Scholar

Don, Jan. 2005. On conversion, relisting and zero-derivation. SKASE Journal of Theoretical Linguistics 2(2). 2–16.Search in Google Scholar

Embick, David. 2010. Localism versus globalism in morphology and phonology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/9780262014229.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Farrell, Patrick. 2001. Functional shift as category underspecification. English Language and Linguistics 5(1). 109–130.10.1017/S1360674301000156Search in Google Scholar

Halle, Morris & Alec Marantz. 1993. Distributed Morphology and the pieces of inflection. In Ken Hale & Samuel J. Keyser (eds.), The view from Building 20, 111–176. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar

Harley, Heidi & Rolf Noyer. 1999. Distributed Morphology. Glot International 4(4). 3–9.Search in Google Scholar

Kiparsky, Paul. 1982. From cyclic phonology to lexical phonology. In Harry van der Hulst & Norval Smith (eds.), The structure of phonological representations, 131–175. Dordrecht: Foris.Search in Google Scholar

Kisselew, Max, Laura Rimell, Alexis Palmer & Sebastian Padó. 2016. Predicting the direction of derivation in English conversion. In Proceedings of the ACL SIGMORPHON Workshop on Computational Research in Phonetics, Phonology, and Morphology, 93–98. Berlin: Association for Computational Linguistics.10.18653/v1/W16-2015Search in Google Scholar

Lieber, Rochelle. 1981. On the organization of the lexicon. PhD dissertation, University of New Hampshire.Search in Google Scholar

Lieber, Rochelle. 1992. Deconstructing morphology. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Search in Google Scholar

Lieber, Rochelle. 2004. Morphology and lexical semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Marantz, Alec. 2013. No escape from morphemes in morphological processing. Language and Cognitive Processes 28. 905–916.10.1080/01690965.2013.779385Search in Google Scholar

Mel’čuk, Igor. 2002. Towards a formal concept ‘zero linguistic sign’. In Sabrina Bendjaballah, Wolfgang U. Dressler, Oskar E. Pfeiffer & Maria D. Voeikova (eds.), Morphology 2000, 241–258. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Search in Google Scholar

Pesetsky, David. 1995. Zero syntax: Experiencers and cascades. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar

Plag, Ingo. 1999. Morphological productivity. Structural constraints in English derivation. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar

Pliatsikas, Christos, Linda Wheeldon, Aditi Lahiri & Peter C. Hansen. 2014. Processing of zero-derived words in English: An fMRI investigation. Neuropsychologia 53. 47–53.10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.11.003Search in Google Scholar

Ralli, Angela. 2013. Compounding in Modern Greek. Dordrecht: Springer.10.1007/978-94-007-4960-3Search in Google Scholar

Ralli, Angela. 2022. Μoρφoλoγία [Morphology], 2nd revised and augmented edn. Athens: Patakis.Search in Google Scholar

Ramchand, Gillian. 2008. Verb meaning and the lexicon. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511486319Search in Google Scholar

Sanders, Gerald. 1988. Zero derivation and the overt analogue criterion. In Michael Hammond & Michael Noonan (eds.), Theoretical morphology, 155–175. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.10.1163/9789004454101_012Search in Google Scholar

Starke, Michal. 2009. Nanosyntax. Nordlyd 36(1). 1–6.10.7557/12.213Search in Google Scholar

Taraldsen, Knut Tarald. 2018. Spanning versus constituent lexicalization. In Lena Baunaz, Liliane Haegeman, Karen De Clercq & Eric Lander (eds.), Exploring Nanosyntax, 88–110. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/oso/9780190876746.003.0003Search in Google Scholar

Valera, Salvador. 2015. Conversion. In Peter O. Müller, Ingeborg Ohnheiser, Susan Olsen & Franz Rainer (eds.), Word-formation. An International handbook of the languages of Europe, vol. 2, 322–329. Berlin & New York: De Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar

Published Online: 2023-05-12
Published in Print: 2023-06-27

© 2022 the author(s), published by De Gruyter

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Downloaded on 28.4.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/zfs-2022-2009/html
Scroll to top button