Skip to content
BY 4.0 license Open Access Published by De Gruyter Open Access August 15, 2022

Response inhibition in the Negative Compatibility Effect in the absence of inhibitory stimulus features

  • Thomas Schmidt EMAIL logo , Sven Panis , Maximilian P. Wolkersdorfer and Dirk Vorberg
From the journal Open Psychology

Abstract

The Negative Compatibility Effect (NCE) is a reversal in priming effects that can occur when a masked arrow prime is followed by an arrow target at a long stimulus-onset asynchrony (SOA). To test the explanation that the NCE is actually a positive priming effect elicited by mask features associated with the prime-opposed response, we devise masks that always point in the same direction as the prime, eliminating all antiprime features. We find large positive priming effects for arrow primes without masks and for arrow masks without primes. When a neutral mask is introduced, priming effects turn negative at long SOAs. In the critical case where the mask is an arrow in the same direction as the prime, the prime does not add to the positive priming effect from the mask shape, but instead strongly diminishes it and induces response errors even though all stimuli point in the same direction. No such feature-free inhibition is seen when arrows are replaced by color stimuli. We conclude that even though response activation by stimulus features plays a role in the NCE, there is a strong inhibitory component (though perhaps not in all feature domains) that is not based on visual features.

References

Atas, A., & Cleeremans, A. (2015). The temporal dynamic of automatic inhibition of irrelevant actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 41, 289-305. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a003865410.1037/a0038654Search in Google Scholar

Bakeman, R., & McArthur, D. (1996). Picturing repeated measures: Comments on Loftus, Morrison, and others. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28 (4), 584-589. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/1082-989X.2.4.35710.3758/BF03200546Search in Google Scholar

Baker, D. H., Vilidaite, G., Lygo, F. A., Smith, A. K., Flack, T. R., Gouws, A. D., & Andrews, T. J. (2021). Power contours: Optimising sample size and precision in experimental psychology and human neuroscience. Psychological Methods, 26, 295-314. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/met000033710.1037/met0000337Search in Google Scholar

Biafora, M., & Schmidt, T. (2020). Induced dissociations: Opposite time-courses of priming and masking induced by custom-made maskcontrast functions. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82, 1333-1354. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-019-01822-4Search in Google Scholar

Brenner, E., & Smeets, J. B. J. (2004). Colour vision can contribute to fast corrections of arm movements. Experimental Brain Research, 158, 302-307. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00644.2003Search in Google Scholar

Brysbaert, M., & Stevens, M. (2018). Power analysis and effect size in mixed effects models: A tutorial. Journal of Cognition, 1, Art. 9. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00644.2003Search in Google Scholar

Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (1998). Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1737-1747. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.24.6.1737Search in Google Scholar

Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (2003). Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming. Biological Psychology, 64, 7-26. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0301-0511(03)00100-5Search in Google Scholar

Eisenhart, C. (1962). Realistic evaluation of the precision and accuracy of instrument calibration systems. In H. H. Ku (Ed.), Precision Measurement and Calibration (1969, pp. 21-48). Washington, D.C.: National Bureau of Standards.Search in Google Scholar

Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143-149.10.3758/BF03203267Search in Google Scholar

Jaśkowski, P. (2007). The effect of nonmasking distractors on the priming of motor responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 456-468. DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.2.456Search in Google Scholar

Jaśkowski, P. (2008). The negative compatibility effect with nonmasking flankers: A case for mask-triggered inhibition hypothesis. Consciousness & Cognition, 17, 765-777. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2007.12.002Search in Google Scholar

Jaśkowski, P. (2009). Negative compatibility effect: the object-updating hypothesis revisited. Experimental Brain Research, 193, 157-160. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1700-6Search in Google Scholar

Jaśkowski, P., Białuńska, A., Tomanek, M., & Verleger, R. (2008). Mask- and distractor-triggered inhibitory processes in the priming of motor responses: An EEG study. Psychophysiology, 45, 70-85. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00595.xSearch in Google Scholar

Jaśkowski, P., & Przekoracka-Krawczyk, A. (2005). On the role of mask structure in subliminal priming. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 65, 409-417.Search in Google Scholar

Klotz, W., & Neumann, O. (1999). Motor activation without conscious discrimination in metacontrast masking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(4), 976–992. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.25.4.976Search in Google Scholar

Klotz, W., & Wolff, P. (1995). The effect of a masked stimulus on the response to the masking stimulus. Psychological Research, 58(2), 92–101. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00571098Search in Google Scholar

Leuthold, H., & Kopp, B. (1998). Mechanisms of priming by masked stimuli: Inferences from event-related brain potentials. Psychological Science, 9, 263-269. https://doi.org/10.1111%2F1467-9280.0005310.1111/1467-9280.00053Search in Google Scholar

Lingnau, A., & Vorberg, D. (2005). The time course of response inhibition in masked priming. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 545-557. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193330Search in Google Scholar

Lleras, A., & Enns, J. T. (2004). Negative compatibility or object updating? A cautionary tale of mask-dependent priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 475-493. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0096-3445.133.4.47510.1037/0096-3445.133.4.475Search in Google Scholar

Ocampo, B., & Finkbeiner, M. (2013). The negative compatibility effect with relevant masks: A case for automatic motor inhibition. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 822. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00822Search in Google Scholar

Panis, S., & Hermens, F. (2014). Time course of spatial contextual interference: Event history analyses of simultaneous masking by nonoverlapping patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 40 (1), 129-144. Doi:10.1037/a0032949Search in Google Scholar

Panis, S., Moran, R., Wolkersdorfer, M., & Schmidt, T. (2020). Studying the dynamics of visual search behavior using RT hazard and microlevel speed-accuracy tradeoff functions: A role for recurrent object recognition and cognitive control processes. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 82, 689-714. doi.org/10.3758/s13414-019-01897-z10.3758/s13414-019-01897-zSearch in Google Scholar

Panis, S., & Schmidt, T. (2016). What is shaping RT and accuracy distributions? Active and selective response inhibition causes the negative compatibility effect. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 28(11), 1651-1671. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00998.)Search in Google Scholar

Panis, S., & Schmidt, T. (2020, May 25). What is causing “inhibition of return” in spatial cueing tasks? Temporally disentangling multiple cue-triggered effects on multiple time scales using response history and conditional accuracy analyses. Preprint. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/udpvsSearch in Google Scholar

Panis, S., & Schmidt, T. (2022). When does “inhibition of return” occur in spatial cueing tasks? Temporally disentangling multiple cuetriggered effects using response history and conditional accuracy analyses. Open Psychology, 4, 84-114.10.1515/psych-2022-0005Search in Google Scholar

Schmidt, T. (2000). Visual perception without awareness: Priming responses by color. In T. Metzinger (Ed.), Neural correlates of consciousness (pp. 157-179). Cambridge: MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/4928.003.0014Search in Google Scholar

Schmidt, T. (2002). The finger in flight: Real-time motor control by visually masked color stimuli. Psychological Science, 13, 112-118. https://doi.org/10.1111%2F1467-9280.0042110.1111/1467-9280.00421Search in Google Scholar

Schmidt, F., Haberkamp, A., & Schmidt, T. (2011). Dos and don’ts in response priming research. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 7, 120–131. https://doi.org/10.2478/v10053-008-0092-2Search in Google Scholar

Schmidt, T., Hauch, V., & Schmidt, F. (2015). Mask-triggered thrust reversal in the Negative Compatibility Effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77, 2377-2398. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-015-0923-4Search in Google Scholar

Schmidt, T., Niehaus, S., & Nagel, A. (2006). Primes and targets in rapid chases: Tracing sequential waves of motor activation. Behavioral Neuroscience, 120(5), 1005–1016. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.120.5.1005Search in Google Scholar

Schmidt, F., & Schmidt, T. (2014). Rapid processing of closure and viewpoint-invariant symmetry: Behavioral criteria for feedforward processing. Psychological Research, 78, 37-54. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0478-8Search in Google Scholar

Schmidt, T., & Schmidt, F. (2009). Processing of natural images is feedforward: A simple behavioral test. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(3), 594-606. doi:10.3758/APP.71.3.594Search in Google Scholar

Schmidt, T., & Vorberg, D. (2006). Criteria for unconscious cognition: Three types of dissociation. Perception & Psychophysics, 68(3), 489–504. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193692Search in Google Scholar

Schmidt, F., Weber, A., & Schmidt, T. (2014). Activation of response force by self-splitting objects: Where are the limits of feedforward Gestalt processing? Journal of Vision, 14, 1-16. doi: 10.1167/14.9.20Search in Google Scholar

Schwarz, W., & Mecklinger, A. (1995). Relationship between flanker identifiability and compatibility effect. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 1045-1052. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03205463Search in Google Scholar

Smith, P. L., & Little, D. R. (2018). Small is beautiful: In defense of the small-N design. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(6), 2083–2101. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1451-8Search in Google Scholar

Sumner, P. (2008). Mask-induced priming and the negative compatibility effect. Experimental Psychology, 55, 133-141. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.55.2.133Search in Google Scholar

Vath, N., & Schmidt, T. (2007). Tracing sequential waves of rapid visuomotor activation in lateralized readiness potentials. Neuroscience, 145, 197-208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.11.044Search in Google Scholar

Verleger, R., Jaśkowski, P., Aydemir, A., van der Lubbe, R., & Groen, M. (2004). Qualitative differences between conscious and nonconscious processing? On inverse priming induced by masked arrows. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 494-515. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0096-3445.133.4.49410.1037/0096-3445.133.4.494Search in Google Scholar

Vorberg, D. (2005, April 4-6). Ist Hemmung oder Bahnung die Grundlage des umgekehrten Priming-Effekts? [Conference presentation]. 47. Tagung experimentell arbeitender Psychologen, Regensburg, Germany.Search in Google Scholar

Vorberg, D., Mattler, U., Heinecke, A., Schmidt, T., & Schwarzbach, J. (2003). Different time courses for visual perception and action priming. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 100(10), 6275–6280. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0931489100Search in Google Scholar

Wolkersdorfer, M., Panis, S., & Schmidt, T. (2020). Temporal dynamics of sequential motor activation in a dual-prime paradigm: Insights from conditional accuracy and hazard functions. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 82, 2581-2602. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-020-02010-5Search in Google Scholar

Received: 2022-02-09
Accepted: 2022-05-18
Published Online: 2022-08-15

© 2022 Thomas Schmidt et al., published by De Gruyter

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Downloaded on 30.4.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/psych-2022-0012/html
Scroll to top button