Skip to content
Licensed Unlicensed Requires Authentication Published by De Gruyter Mouton May 3, 2019

Expressing evolution in Pokémon names: Experimental explorations

  • Shigeto Kawahara EMAIL logo and Gakuji Kumagai

Abstract

There has been a growing interest in sound symbolic patterns in natural languages, in which some sounds are associated with particular meanings. Previous corpus-based research identified some specific sound symbolic relationships in Pokémon naming patterns in Japanese (Kawahara et al. 2018b). One of the main findings was that the names of Pokémon characters are more likely to contain voiced obstruents, and are longer in terms of mora count, when the Pokémon characters undergo evolution (e.g. nyoromonyorozo; poppopijotto). The current study reports three experiments that test whether (i) these patterns are productive in the minds of general Japanese speakers, and whether (ii) the same tendency holds with English speakers. The results show that the effect of phonological length was clearly observed both with Japanese and English speakers; the effects of voiced obstruents were observed clearly with Japanese speakers, but less clearly with English speakers. Along the way, we address other general issues related to sound symbolism: (iii) to what extent the sound symbolic effects identified in Kawahara et al. (2018b) rely on familiarity with Pokémon, and (iv) whether word-initial segments invoke stronger images than word-internal segments. In addition to its research value, we emphasize that this general project on Pokémon names can be useful for undergraduate phonetics education.

References

Alderete, John & Alexei Kochetov. 2017. Integrating sound symbolism with core grammar: The case of expressive palatalization. Language 93. 731–766.10.1353/lan.2017.0056Search in Google Scholar

Bauer, Hxarold R. 1987. Frequency code: Orofacial correlates of fundamental frequency. Phonetica 44. 173–191.10.1159/000261793Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Becker, Michael, Andrew Nevins & Jonathan Levine. 2012. Asymmetries in generalizing alternations to and from initial syllables. Language 88(2). 231–268.10.1353/lan.2012.0049Search in Google Scholar

Beckman, Jill. 1998. Positional faithfulness. Amherst, MA: University of Massachusetts dissertation.Search in Google Scholar

Berlin, Brent. 2006. The first congress of ethonozoological nomenclature. Journal of Royal Anthropological Institution 12. 23–44.10.1111/j.1467-9655.2006.00271.xSearch in Google Scholar

Blasi, Damián E., Søren Wichmann, Harald Hammarström, Peter F. Stadler & Morten H. Christianson. 2016. Sound-meaning association biases evidenced across thousands of languages. PNAS 113(39). 10818–10823.10.1073/pnas.1605782113Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Bolts, Marilyn G., Grace M. Mangigian & Molly B. Allen. 2016. Phonetic symbolism and memory for advertisement. Applied Cognitive Psychology 30. 1088–1092.10.1002/acp.3284Search in Google Scholar

Browman, Catherine. 1978. Tip of the tongue and slip of the ear: Implications for language processing. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics. 42. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/5sv6m8v8.Search in Google Scholar

Brown, Roger & David MacNeill. 1966. The ‘tip of the tongue’ phenomenon. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 5(4). 325–337.10.1016/S0022-5371(66)80040-3Search in Google Scholar

Chomsky, Noam. 1981. Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris.Search in Google Scholar

Chomsky, Noam. 1986. Language and problems of knowledge. Cambridge: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar

Chomsky, Noam. 1995. The minimalist program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar

Cole, Ronald. 1973. Listening for mispronunciations: A measure of what we hear during speech. Perception & Psychophysics 13. 153–156.10.3758/BF03207252Search in Google Scholar

Coulter, Keith & Robin A. Coulter. 2010. Small sounds, big deals: Phonetic symbolism effects in pricing. Journal of Consumer Research 37(2). 315–328.10.1086/651241Search in Google Scholar

Cutler, Anne, James McQueen & Ken Robinson. 1990. Elizabeth and John: Sound patterns of men’s and women’s names. Journal of Linguistics 26. 471–482.10.1017/S0022226700014754Search in Google Scholar

Cutler, Anne & Takashi Otake. 2002. Rhythmic categories in spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language 46(2). 296–322.10.1006/jmla.2001.2814Search in Google Scholar

D’Onofrio, Annette. 2014. Phonetic detail and dimensionality in sound-shape correspondences: Refining the bouba-kiki paradigm. Language and Speech 57(3). 367–393.10.1177/0023830913507694Search in Google Scholar

Daland, Robert, Bruce Hayes, James White, Marc Garellek, Andrea Davis & Ingrid Norrmann. 2011. Explaining sonority projection effects. Phonology 28(2). 197–234.10.1017/S0952675711000145Search in Google Scholar

Diehl, Randy L. & Michelle Molis. 1995. Effects of fundamental frequency on medial [voice] judgments. Phonetica 52. 188–195.10.1159/000262170Search in Google Scholar

Diffloth, Gerard. 1994. i: Big, a: Small. In Leane Hinton, Johanna Nichols & John J. Ohala (eds.), Sound symbolism, 107–114. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511751806.008Search in Google Scholar

Dingemanse, Mark. 2012. Advances in the cross-linguistic study of ideophones. Language and Linguistic Compass 6. 654–672.10.1002/lnc3.361Search in Google Scholar

Dingemanse, Mark, Damián E. Blasi, Gary Lupyan, Morten H. Christianson & Padraic Monaghan. 2015. Arbitrariness, iconicity and systematicity in language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 19(10). 603–615.10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.013Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Dingemanse, Mark, Will Schuerman, Eva Reinisch, Sylvia Tufvesson & Holger Mitterer. 2016. What sound symbolism can and cannot do: Testing the iconicity of ideophones from five languages. Language 92(2). 117–133.10.1353/lan.2016.0034Search in Google Scholar

Dingemanse, Mark, Francisco Torreira & N. J. Enfield. 2013. Is “huh?” a universal word? conversational infrastructure and the convergent evolution of linguistic items. PloS One 8. 11. doi:https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094620.Search in Google Scholar

Elbert, Samuel H. & Mary Kawena Pukui. 1979. Hawaiian grammar. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.10.1515/9780824840792Search in Google Scholar

Ernestus, Mirjam & R. Harald Baayen. 2003. Predicting the unpredictable: Interpreting neutralized segments in Dutch. Language 79(1). 5–38.10.1353/lan.2003.0076Search in Google Scholar

Flemming, Edward. 1995. Auditory representations in phonology. Los Angeles, CA: University of California dissertation.Search in Google Scholar

Fort, Mathilde, Alexander Martin & Sharon Peperkamp. 2015. Consonants are more important than vowels in the bouba-kiki effect. Language and Speech 58. 247–266.10.1177/0023830914534951Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Garrigues, Stephen L. 1995. Mimetic parallels in Korean and Japanese. Studies in Language 19. 359–398.10.1075/sl.19.2.03garSearch in Google Scholar

Gelbart, Ben. 2005. Perception of foreignness. Amherst, MA: University of Massachusetts dissertation.Search in Google Scholar

Gussenhoven, Carlos. 2004. The phonology of tone and intonation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511616983Search in Google Scholar

Gussenhoven, Carlos. 2016. Foundations of intonational meaning: Anatomical and physiological factors. Topics in Cognitive Science 8. 425–434.10.1111/tops.12197Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Haiman, John. 1980. The iconicity of grammar: Isomorphism and motivation. Language 56(3). 515–540.10.2307/414448Search in Google Scholar

Haiman, John (ed.). 1985. Iconicity in syntax. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/tsl.6Search in Google Scholar

Haspelmath, Martin. 2006. Against markedness (and what to replace it with). Journal of Linguistics 42. 25–70.10.1017/S0022226705003683Search in Google Scholar

Hawkins, John A. & Anne Cutler. 1988. Psycholinguistic factors in morphological asymmetry. In John A. Hawkins (ed.), Explaining language universals, 280–317. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Search in Google Scholar

Hay, Jennifer, Janet Pierrehumbert & Mary Beckman. 2003. Speech perception, well-formedness, and the statistics of the lexicon. In John Local, Richard Ogden & Rosalind Temple (eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology VI: Phonetic interpretation, 58–74. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Hayes, Bruce & Zsuzsa. Londe. 2006. Stochastic phonological knowledge: The case of Hungarian vowel harmony. Phonology 23. 59–104.10.1017/S0952675706000765Search in Google Scholar

Hayes, Bruce & Donca Steriade. 2004. Introduction: The phonetic bases of phonological markedness. In Bruce Hayes, Robert Kirchner & Donca Steriade (eds.), Phonetically based phonology, 1–33. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511486401Search in Google Scholar

Hockett, Charles F. 1959. Animal “languages” and human language. Human Biology 31. 32–39.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Hollard, Morris K. & Michael Wertheimer. 1964. Some physiognomic aspects of naming, or maluma and takete revisited. Perceptual and Motor Skills 19. 111–117.10.2466/pms.1964.19.1.111Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Imai, Mutsumi, Sotaro Kita, Miho Nagumo & Hiroyuki Okada. 2008. Sound symbolism facilitates early verb learning. Cognition 109. 54–65.10.1016/j.cognition.2008.07.015Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Ito, Junko. 1989. A prosodic theory of epenthesis. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 7. 217–259.10.1007/BF00138077Search in Google Scholar

Ito, Junko. 1990. Prosodic minimality in Japanese. In Michael Ziolkowski, Manuela Noske & Karen Deaton (eds.), Proceedings of Chicago Linguistic Society: Parasession on the syllable in phonetics and phonology, 213–239. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Search in Google Scholar

Jakobson, Roman. 1978. Six lectures on sound and meaning. Cambridge: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar

Jespersen, Otto. 1922. Symbolic value of the vowel i. In Otto Jespersen (ed.), Linguistica: Selected papers in English, French and German, 283–303. Copenhagen: Levin and Munksgaard.Search in Google Scholar

Kawahara, Shigeto. 2006. A faithfulness ranking projected from a perceptibility scale: The case of [+voice] in Japanese. Language 82(3). 536–574.10.1353/lan.2006.0146Search in Google Scholar

Kawahara, Shigeto. 2008. Phonetic naturalness and unnaturalness in Japanese loanword phonology. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 17(4). 317–330.10.1007/s10831-008-9030-zSearch in Google Scholar

Kawahara, Sihgeto. 2016. Japanese has syllables: A reply to Labrune (2012). Phonology 33. 169–194.10.1017/S0952675716000063Search in Google Scholar

Kawahara, Shigeto, Miwa Isobe, Yukino Kobayashi, Tomoko Monou & Reiko Okabe. 2018a. Acquisition of sound symbolic values of vowels and voiced obstruents by Japanese children: Using a Pokémonastics paradigm. Journal of the Phonetic Society of Japan 22. 122–130.Search in Google Scholar

Kawahara, Shigeto & Tomoko Monou. 2018. Onshōchō-de gengogaku-o oshieru: Gutairei-no shōkai-wo tōshite [Teaching linguistics with sound symbolism: Some case studies]. Southern Review 32. 3–14.Search in Google Scholar

Kawahara, Shigeto & Jeff Moore. 2018. Exploring sound symbolic knowledge of English speakers using Pokémon character names. Ms Keio University and Sophia University.Search in Google Scholar

Kawahara, Shigeto, Kohei Nishimura & Hajime Ono. 2002. Unveiling the unmarkedness of Sino-Japanese. In William McClure (ed.), Japanese/Korean Linguistics 12, 140–151. Stanford: CSLI Publications.Search in Google Scholar

Kawahara, Shigeto, Atsushi Noto & Gakuji Kumagai. 2018b. Sound symbolic patterns in Pokémon names. Phonetica 75(3). 219–244.10.1159/000484938Search in Google Scholar

Kawahara, Shigeto & Kazuko Shinohara. 2011. Phonetic and psycholinguistic prominences in pun formation: Experimental evidence for positional faithfulness. In William McClure & Marcel Den Dikken (eds.), Japanese/Korean Linguistics 18, 177–188. Stanford: CSLI Publications.Search in Google Scholar

Kawahara, Shigeto, Kazuko Shinohara & Joseph Grady. 2015. Iconic inferences about personality: From sounds and shapes. In Masako K. Hiraga, William J. Herlofsky, Kazuko Shinohara & Kimi Akita (eds.), Iconicity: East meets west, 57–69. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/ill.14.03kawSearch in Google Scholar

Kawahara, Shigeto, Kazuko Shinohara & Yumi Uchimoto. 2008. A positional effect in sound symbolism: An experimental study. Proceedings of the Japan Cognitive Linguistics Association 8. 417–427. Tokyo: JCLA.Search in Google Scholar

Kawahara, Shigeto, Michinori Suzuki & Gakuji Kumagai. 2019. Sound symbolic patterns in Pokémon move names. Ms Keio University, International Christian University and Meikai University.Search in Google Scholar

Kingston, John & Randy L. Diehl. 1994. Phonetic knowledge. Language 70. 419–454.10.1353/lan.1994.0023Search in Google Scholar

Kingston, John & Randy L. Diehl. 1995. Intermediate properties in the perception of distinctive feature values. In Bruce Connell & Amalia Arvaniti (eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology IV: Phonology and phonetic evidence, 7–27. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511554315.002Search in Google Scholar

Klink, Richard R. 2000. Creating brand names with meaning: The use of sound symbolism. Marketing Letters 11. 5–20.10.1023/A:1008184423824Search in Google Scholar

Kochetov, Alexei & John Alderete. 2011. Scales and patterns of expressive palatalization: Experimental evidence from Japanese. Canadian Journal of Linguistics 56(3). 345–376.10.1017/S0008413100002048Search in Google Scholar

Köhler, Woldgang. 1947. Gestalt psychology: An Introduction to new concepts in modern psychology. New York: Liveright.Search in Google Scholar

Koppensteiner, Markus, Pie Stephan & Paul Michael Jäschke. 2016. Shaking takete and flowing maluma. Non-sense words are associated with motion patterns. PloS One. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0150610.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Kubozono, Haruo. 1999. Mora and syllable. In Natsuko Tsujimura (ed.), The handbook of Japanese linguistics, 31–61. Oxford: Blackwell.10.1002/9781405166225.ch2Search in Google Scholar

Kubozono, Haruo. 2002. Temporal neutralization in Japanese. In Carlos Gussenhoven & Natasha Warner (eds.), Laboratory Phonology 7, 171–202. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.10.1515/9783110197105.1.171Search in Google Scholar

Kubozono, Haruo. 2004. Weight neutralization in Japanese. Journal of Japanese Linguistics 20(1). 51–70.10.1515/jjl-2004-0106Search in Google Scholar

Kumagai, Gakuji & Shigeto Kawahara. 2017. How abstract is sound symbolism? Labiality in Japanese diaper names [in Japanese]. Proceedings of the 31st meeting of the Phonetic Society of Japan. 49–54.Search in Google Scholar

Kumagai, Gakuji & Shigeto Kawahara. 2019. Pokémon-no naduke-ni okeru boin-to yuuseisogaion-no kouka. Gengo Kenkyu [Journal of the Linguistic Society of Japan] 155.Search in Google Scholar

Lindauer, Martin S. 1990. The meanings of the physiognomic stimuli taketa and maluma. Bulletin of Psychonomic Society 28(1). 47–50.10.3758/BF03337645Search in Google Scholar

Lisker, Leigh. 1978. On buzzing the English /b/. Haskins Laboratories Status Report on Speech Research, SR-55/56: 251–259.Search in Google Scholar

Lisker, Leigh. 1986. “Voicing” in English: A catalog of acoustic features signaling /b/ versus /p/in trochees. Language and Speech 29. 3–11.10.1177/002383098602900102Search in Google Scholar

Lockwood, Gwilym & Mark Dingemanse. 2015. Iconicity in the lab: A review of behavioral, developmental, and neuroimaging research into sound-symbolism. Frontiers in Psychology. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01246.Search in Google Scholar

Lyman, Benjamin Smith. 1894. Change from surd to sonant in Japanese compounds. Philadelphia: Oriental Studies of the Oriental Club of Philadelphia. 1–17.Search in Google Scholar

Marslen-Wilson, William D. 1975. Sentence perception as an interactive parallel process. Science 189. 226–228.10.1126/science.189.4198.226Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Mattys, Sven & Arthur G. Samuel. 2000. Implications of stress-pattern differences in spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language 42. 571–596.10.1006/jmla.1999.2696Search in Google Scholar

Maurer, Daphne, Thanujeni Pathman & Catherine J. Mondloch. 2006. The shape of boubas: Sound-shape correspondences in toddlers and adults. Developmental Science 9. 316–322.10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00495.xSearch in Google Scholar PubMed

McCarthy, John J. 1993. A case of surface constraint violation. Canadian Journal of Linguistics 38. 169–195.10.1017/S0008413100014730Search in Google Scholar

McCarthy, John J. & Alan Prince. 1994. The emergence of the unmarked: Optimality in prosodic morphology. In Mercè Gonzàlez (ed.), Proceedings of the North East Linguistic Society 24, 333–379. Amherst, MA: GLSA Publications.Search in Google Scholar

Mutsukawa, Masahiko. 2016. On Japanese unisex names: Names and their environment. Proceedings of the 25th International Congress of Onomastic Sciences. 186–199.Search in Google Scholar

Newman, Stanley S. 1933. Further experiments on phonetic symbolism. American Journal of Psychology 45. 53–75.10.2307/1414186Search in Google Scholar

Nielsen, Alan K. S. & Drew Rendall. 2013. Parsing the role of consonants versus vowels in the classic takete-maluma phenomenon. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 67(2). 153–163.10.1037/a0030553Search in Google Scholar

Nooteboom, Sieb G. 1981. Lexical retrieval from fragments of spoken words: Beginnings vs. endings. Journal of Phonetics 9. 407–424.10.1016/S0095-4470(19)31017-4Search in Google Scholar

Nuckolls, Janis B. 1999. The case for sound symbolism. Annual Review of Anthropology 28. 225–252.10.1146/annurev.anthro.28.1.225Search in Google Scholar

Nygaard, Lynee C., Allison E. Cook & Laura L. Namy. 2009. Sound to meaning correspondences facilitate word learning. Cognition 112. 181–186.10.1016/j.cognition.2009.04.001Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Ohala, John J. 1984. An ethological perspective on common cross-language utilization of f0 of voice. Phonetica 41. 1–16.10.1159/000261706Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Ohala, John J. 1994. The frequency code underlies the sound symbolic use of voice pitch. In Leane Hinton, Johanna Nichols & John J. Ohala (eds.), Sound symbolism, 325–347. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511751806.022Search in Google Scholar

Ohyama, Yuka. 2016. What can Pokémon names tell us about Japanese and English morphology and phonology. London: University of College London Master’s thesis.Search in Google Scholar

Otake, Takashi, Giyoo Hatano, Anne Cutler & Jacques Mehler. 1993. Mora or syllable? Speech segmentation in Japanese. Journal of Memory and Language 32. 258–278.10.1006/jmla.1993.1014Search in Google Scholar

Peterson, Robert A. & Ivan Ross. 1972. How to name new brand names. Journal of Advertising Research 12(6). 29–34.Search in Google Scholar

Poser, William. 1990. Evidence for foot structure in Japanese. Language 66. 78–105.10.1353/lan.1990.0031Search in Google Scholar

Prince, Alan & Paul Smolensky. 2004. Optimality theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. Malden and Oxford: Blackwell.10.1002/9780470759400Search in Google Scholar

Ramachandran, Vilayanur S. & Edward M. Hubbard. 2001. Synesthesia–A window into perception, thought, and language. Journal of Consciousness Studies 8(12). 3–34.Search in Google Scholar

Raphael, Lawrence. 1981. Duration and contexts as cues to word-final cognate opposition in English. Phonetica 38. 126–147.10.1159/000260019Search in Google Scholar

Saji, Nobuko, Kimi Akita, Mutsumi Imai, Katerina Kantartzis & Sotaro Kita. 2013. Cross-linguistically shared and language-specific sound symbolism for motion: An exploratory data mining approach. Proceedings of CogSci 2013. 1253–1258.Search in Google Scholar

Sapir, Edward. 1929. A study in phonetic symbolism. Journal of Experimental Psychology 12. 225–239.10.1037/h0070931Search in Google Scholar

Saussure, Ferdinand. 1916. Cours de linguistique générale. Paris: Payot.Search in Google Scholar

Selkirk, Elizabeth. 1995. Sentence prosody: Intonation, stress, and phrasing. In John A. Goldsmith (ed.), The handbook of phonological theory, 550–569. Cambridge, MA and Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Search in Google Scholar

Shih, Stephanie S., Jordan Ackerman, Noah Hermalin, Sharon Inkelas & Darya Kavitskaya. 2018. Pokémonikers: A study of sound symbolism and Pokémon names. Proceedings of LSA 2018.10.3765/plsa.v3i1.4335Search in Google Scholar

Shinohara, Kazuko & Shigeto Kawahara. 2016. A cross-linguistic study of sound symbolism: The images of size. Proceedings of the 36th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society. 396–410. Berkeley: Berkeley Linguistics Society.10.3765/bls.v36i1.3926Search in Google Scholar

Shinohara, Kazuko, Naoto Yamauchi, Shigeto Kawahara & Hideyuki Tanaka. 2016. Takete and maluma in action: A cross-modal relationship between gestures and sounds. PloS One. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0163525.Search in Google Scholar

Sidhu, David D. & Penny M. Pexman. 2015. What’s in a name? sound symbolism and gender in first names. PloS One. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0126809.Search in Google Scholar

Sidhu, David D. & Penny M. Pexman. 2017. Five mechanisms of sound symbolic association. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 25. 1619–1643.10.3758/s13423-017-1361-1Search in Google Scholar

Smith, Jennifer. 2002. Phonological augmentation in prominent positions. Amherst, MA: University of Massachusetts dissertation.Search in Google Scholar

Stevens, Kenneth N. & Shelia E. Blumstein. 1981. The search for invariant acoustic correlates of phonetic features. In Peter Eimas & Joanne L. Miller (eds.), Perspectives on the study of speech, 1–38. New Jersey: Earlbaum.Search in Google Scholar

Tzeng, Christina, Lynne C. Nygaard & Laura L. Namy. 2016. The specificity of sound symbolic correspondences in spoken language. Cognitive Science. doi:10.1111/cogs.12474.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Ultan, Russell. 1978. Size-sound symbolism. In Joseph Greenberg (ed.), Universals of human language II: Phonology, 525–568. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Vance, Timothy. 2007. Have we learned anything about rendaku that Lyman didn’t already know? In Bjarke Frellesvig, Masayoshi Shibatani & John Charles Smith (eds.), Current issues in the history and structure of Japanese, 153–170. Tokyo: Kurosio Publishers.Search in Google Scholar

Westbury, Chris. 2005. Implicit sound symbolism in lexical access: Evidence from an interference task. Brain and Language 93. 10–19.10.1016/j.bandl.2004.07.006Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Wichmann, Søren, Eric W. Holman & Cecil H. Brown. 2010. Sound symbolism in basic vocabulary. Entropy 12(4). 844–858.10.3390/e12040844Search in Google Scholar

Wright, Saundra, Jennifer Hay & Bent Tessa. 2005. Ladies first? phonology, frequency, and the naming conspiracy. Linguistics 43(3). 531–561.10.1515/ling.2005.43.3.531Search in Google Scholar

Yorkston, Eric & Geeta Menon. 2004. A sound idea: Phonetic effects of brand names on consumer judgments. Journal of Consumer Research 31. 43–51.10.1086/383422Search in Google Scholar

  Appendix

Table 3:

The stimuli for Experiments 2 and 3.

condition 1No voiced obstruents1 voiced obstruent
hifurodomana
mureyazuhemi
ritohazetemu
matohanegemu
mishimimabiho
riyaretazuri
nehomafuriba
tsureratohoze
karunohafubi
satoraruyoga
condition 21 voiced obstruent2 voiced obstruents
bamachibedeme
gasoyuzazohi
bonechigudeyo
genefudarobe
goyamugoruzu
dosoradokuba
zeyuriberada
sozafuyabude
najiyokuguji
hodamoneguzu
condition 3All light syllablesFinal long vowel
sakirohokinee
sukihimuhuraa
saheshikishimaa
tsumohikutonaa
wasehemomuruu
samimutsunokee
wakeyakorunii
rihepimekiree
soromosemafuu
ranehomyusaroo
condition 44 light syllables5 light syllables
hukoyotanorutehume
tokusuhimonohehita
henarohonoshiyohoya
manoyakimiyarifuchi
mumotokeyaserenama
nushikoyaharetamonu
harochifuhomiherori
sunemarotaharohore
fuchikehohisahemetsu
ko(s)emunatakimekama

Note

The materials presented in this paper have been used in introductory phonetics/linguistics class at International Christian University, Keio University, Kwansei Gakuin University, Otsuma Women’s University and Tokyo Metropolitan University; this paper was also presented as invited lectures at Keio University, Meio University, Seoul International Phonology Conference, and Tokyo Institute for Advanced Studies of Language (TEC). Students and audience at these occasions provided extremely helpful feedback for this general project on sound symbolic effects in Pokémon names; they also helped us confirm that this is great teaching material to use to introduce phonetic concepts and statistical techniques. We thank two anonymous reviewers, as well as Lisa Davidson and Yasuyo Minagawa, who provided constructive written comments on previous versions of this paper. We are grateful to Donna Erickson for proofreading the manuscript. Last but not least, we would like to thank Ms toto-mame for letting us use her pictures for the current experiments, and for letting us reproduce some of them in this paper. The remaining errors are ours.


Published Online: 2019-05-03
Published in Print: 2019-05-27

© 2019 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 28.4.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/jjl-2019-2002/html
Scroll to top button