Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Misophonia: Phenomenology, comorbidity and demographics in a large sample

  • Inge Jager ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft

    i.j.jager@amsterdamumc.nl

    Current address: Outpatient Clinic for Affective disorders, GGZ inGeest, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Amsterdam UMC (location AMC), Amsterdam, The Netherlands

  • Pelle de Koning,

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Amsterdam UMC (location AMC), Amsterdam, The Netherlands

  • Tim Bost,

    Roles Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Clinical and Experimental Audiology, Amsterdam Public Health, Amsterdam UMC (location AMC), University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

  • Damiaan Denys ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Damiaan Denys, Nienke Vulink

    Roles Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Amsterdam UMC (location AMC), Amsterdam, The Netherlands

  • Nienke Vulink

    Contributed equally to this work with: Damiaan Denys, Nienke Vulink

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Amsterdam UMC (location AMC), Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Abstract

Objective

Analyze a large sample with detailed clinical data of misophonia subjects in order to determine the psychiatric, somatic and psychological nature of the condition.

Methods

This observational study of 779 subjects with suspected misophonia was conducted from January 2013 to May 2017 at the outpatient-clinic of the Amsterdam University Medical Centers, location AMC, the Netherlands. We examined DSM-IV diagnoses, results of somatic examination (general screening and hearing tests), and 17 psychological questionnaires (e.g., SCL-90-R, WHOQoL).

Results

The diagnosis of misophonia was confirmed in 575 of 779 referred subjects (74%). In the sample of misophonia subjects (mean age, 34.17 [SD = 12.22] years; 399 women [69%]), 148 (26%) subjects had comorbid traits of obsessive-compulsive personality disorder, 58 (10%) mood disorders, 31 (5%) attention-deficit (hyperactivity) disorder, and 14 (3%) autism spectrum conditions. Two percent reported tinnitus and 1% hyperacusis. In a random subgroup of 109 subjects we performed audiometry, and found unilateral hearing loss in 3 of them (3%). Clinical neurological examination and additional blood test showed no abnormalities. Psychological tests revealed perfectionism (97% CPQ>25) and neuroticism (stanine 7 NEO-PI-R). Quality of life was heavily impaired and associated with misophonia severity (rs (184) = -.34 p = < .001, p = < .001).

Limitations

This was a single site study, leading to possible selection–and confirmation bias, since AMC-criteria were used.

Conclusions

This study with 575 subjects is the largest misophonia sample ever described. Based on these results we propose a set of revised criteria useful to diagnose misophonia as a psychiatric disorder.

Introduction

Misophonia is a recently recognized condition, characterized by an impulsive aversive physical reaction of irritation, anger, or disgust when confronted with specific, repetitive stimuli (for instance, eating sounds). The word was first used in audiology literature as a hatred of sounds[1]. In 2013 our research group at the Amsterdam University Medical Centers (Amsterdam UMC, location AMC) proposed the first diagnostic criteria for misophonia as a psychiatric disorder[2] (Table 1). Thereafter, research on misophonia has increased vastly. The Amsterdam viewpoint is misophonia is definitely a psychiatric disorder, though there’s no agreement among different research teams. For a recent descriptive overview, we refer to Taylor[3] or Brout et al.[4].

Currently, a total of 797 misophonia subjects has been described in 26 clinical research papers, including five sample studies [2,5,6,7,8]. Only subjects included in the AMC sample[2] and a sample study published last year (Erfanian, Kartsonaki & Keshavarz[9]) had a systematic medical and psychiatric examination. All other samples (of the papers included in our 2018 search) merely used questionnaires to diagnose misophonia.

Without a systematic clinical interview, which is missing in almost three quarters of all described subjects, misophonia symptoms could possibly be better explained by another disorder or results could be influenced by self-report biases (references in S1 Table and S1 Fig).

Therefore, we assessed a new sample of subjects with misophonia symptoms who were referred to the AMC by their general practitioner, which is both quantitatively and qualitatively superior to previous research. The first aim of this study was to determine whether referred subjects with misophonia-like symptoms actually suffered from misophonia using a psychiatric interview conducted by three experienced psychiatrists. The second aim was to determine phenomenology, comorbidity, and demographics of the misophonia sample to address three major issues: 1) whether misophonia should be approached from an audiological or psychiatric/psychological perspective; 2) whether specific psychological profiles, which have been associated with misophonia, such as disgust sensitivity[10], autism-like traits[11,12] and perfectionism[2] are still valid; and 3) whether misophonia is a distinct psychiatric disorder for which diagnostic criteria should be determined.

Methods

Subjects

In this sample study, we analyzed data collected from subjects who were referred with misophonia symptoms from 2013 through 2017 at the Department of Psychiatry at Amsterdam University Medical Center (Amsterdam UMC), the Netherlands. This study has been approved by the ethics committee of Amsterdam UMC and the need for informed consent was waived.

Of the 779 examined subjects, 575 subjects met criteria for misophonia. The 204 subjects excluded from this sample were: subjects with primary autism spectrum conditions (ASC), primary attention-deficit (hyperactivity) disorder (AD(H)D), a primary diagnosis on Axis II (varying from schizotypal personality disorder to obsessive compulsive personality disorder) and subjects without a DSM-IV diagnosis. Hearing impairments or audiologic disorders were no exclusion criterion.

Diagnostic procedures

Assessment of current Axis I and Axis II disorders based on the DSM-IV criteria[13] was determined with the MINI-International Neuropsychiatric Interview Plus[14] (MINI-plus) and sections of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders[15] (SCID II). Based on information obtained from clinical interview, questionnaires or psychiatric history specific sections of the SCID-II relevant to each subject were selected and conducted. DSM-5 was not in use for clinical purposes at our department until 2018. Three psychiatrists, specialized in anxiety disorders and obsessive-compulsive and related disorders, carried out the clinical (medical and psychiatric) interviews.

Somatic assessment consisted of a general physical and neurological examination and a general blood screening. Audiometry was performed with the Hughson-Westlake procedure[16] to obtain hearing thresholds in a random selection of participants (n = 109) in the first 300 subjects. Patients were randomly assigned to three psychiatrists. In a period of 20 months the assessment of one psychiatrist was extended with audiometry. Because the results were clear, we stopped performing audiometry in order not to unnecessarily burden subjects. Air conduction thresholds were measured at all octave frequencies from 0.25 to 8 kHz and bone conduction thresholds were measured at 0.25, 0.5, 1, and, 2 kHz, with adequate masking if necessary. The Pure Tone Average (PTA) was obtained by averaging air conduction thresholds 0.5, 1, 2, and, 4 kHz and hearing loss classification was defined according to WHO-classification[17].

Finally, a variety of self-report questionnaires examined the nature and severity of misophonia symptoms, quality of life, anxiety and depressive symptoms, and personality profile of the subjects. Given the naturalistic nature of the sample, the standard battery of questionnaires at our psychiatry outpatient clinic was used (seven questionnaires), with several additional questionnaires to understand the phenomenology of misophonia and the relation with possible correlated constructs. All questionnaires were administered during intake procedure. A random selection of subjects (n = 56) completed an additional personality questionnaire (see S2 Table). During 4 months all intakes (60 subjects in total) were approached for this additional personality questionnaire, which was completed by 56 subjects.

Questionnaires

The following questionnaires were administered: Misophonia Screening List (see S1 Appendix), Misophonia Sound List (MSL; see S2 Appendix), Amsterdam Misophonia Scale[2] (A-MISO-S), AMISOS Revised (AMISOS-R; see S3 Appendix), Hamilton Depression Rating Scale[18, 19] (HDRS), Hamilton Anxiety Scale[20,21] (HAS), Symptom Checklist 90 Revised [22, 23] (SCL-90-R), Manchester Short Assessment of Quality of life[24, 25] (MANSA), Sheehan Disability Scale[26] (SDS), WHO Quality of Life-BREF[27, 28] (WHOQoL-BREF), NEO-Personality Inventory-revised[29, 30] (NEO-PI-R), Autism Spectrum Quotient[31, 32] (AQ), Inventory of Interpersonal Situations[33] (IIS), Clinical Perfectionism Questionnaire[34] (CPQ), Frost Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale[35] (FMPS), Disgust Propensity and Sensitivity Scale Revised[36, 37] (DPSS-R), Disgust Scale Revised[38, 39] (DS-R). For more information, see S2 Table.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were conducted with SPSS statistical package version 24. We report the sample descriptively in terms of means and standard deviations or percentage of the sample, where appropriate. We used independent-samples t-tests to explore whether males and females differed in age of onset and symptom severity (i.e. A-MISO-S or AMISOS-R score). We used multiple linear regression to explore whether certain features were associated with symptom severity. AMISOS-R scores were included as independent variables, and CPQ, FMPS, AQ, DS-R and DPSS-R were included as dependent variables. We confirmed normality of residuals by checking the QQ plot of the model. We treated the full Likert scales as numerical, since assumptions of linear regression were met (residuals were normally distributed) and results are much easier to interpret. Finally, a non-parametric correlation (Spearman’s rho) was calculated to determine whether misophonia symptoms (A-MISO-S) correlated with quality of life (MANSA). No missing scores were imputed and no outliers were removed. We considered P < 0.05 to be statistically significant.

Results

Demographics

Our sample was predominantly Caucasian, 69% were female, and 64% had a relationship. Over 85% were employed or studying and 5% were on sick-leave. Mean age at admission was 34.17 years (SD = 12.22) and mean age of onset was 13.17 years (SD = 7.37). Onset in females was not significantly earlier than in males (p = .076). Most subjects (93%) reported a gradual onset of the misophonia symptoms. Clinical interviews with older subjects indicated a chronic course and 33% reported a positive family history of misophonia.

Comorbidity

The comorbid DSM-IV disorders are shown in Table 2. 72% of subjects diagnosed with misophonia had no comorbid Axis I psychiatric disorder, 22% had one comorbid disorder, and 6% had two or more comorbid disorders. Common comorbid disorders were major depressive disorder (6.8%) and obsessive-compulsive disorder (2.8%). Another 5% of the sample had comorbid AD(H)D and 3% was diagnosed with a comorbid ASC.

thumbnail
Table 2. Percentage and absolute frequencies of current DSM-IV Axis I & Axis II disorders in misophonia subjects.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0231390.t002

The majority (59%) had no comorbidity on Axis II. Most prevalent were OCPD (2.4%) and borderline personality disorder (BPD, 1.7%). Obsessive-compulsive personality traits were found in 26%. Subjects exhibited especially high morality and clinical perfectionism.

Misophonia triggers

Almost all subjects reported to be triggered by eating sounds (96%) followed by nasal and breathing sounds (85%). Subjects were also regularly disturbed by sounds of repetitive tapping or mouth/throat sounds. All triggers are shown in Fig 1.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Triggers and provoked anger for misophonia subjects.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0231390.g001

Visual triggers were often reported, e.g., repetitive movements (68%). We found visual triggers were often directly associated with auditory triggers (e.g., grinding teeth) in clinical interviews. When visual triggers were reported, they occurred secondary to auditory triggers and had less impact than the auditory triggers. When visual and auditory stimuli occurred simultaneously (e.g., hearing and seeing someone chewing gum), subjects reported a more intense response. 59% were bothered by ambient sounds, particularly by sounds of neighbors.

Nature of the response

Subjects reported extreme irritation, anger, and disgust as primary emotional responses (see Table 3). Aggressive outbursts were seldom reported at psychiatric interviews; verbal aggression was common, but physical aggression was rare. Frequency was not assessed with a questionnaire.

thumbnail
Table 3. Emotional response and hyper focus to sounds in misophonia subjects.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0231390.t003

None of the subjects reported anxiety as a primary response, neither at psychiatric examination nor with questionnaires. Only five subjects (1%) reported secondary anxiety, following anger or disgust. The majority of the sample reported anticipatory anxiety, which was mild and related to thinking of future misophonic situations. In clinical interviews, all subjects reported confrontations with triggers as stressful events. Subjects worried about misophonic triggers and their capacity to cope. A perceived loss of control was seen in 81% of the subjects and 90% reported severe to extreme powerlessness. In clinical interviews, most subjects expressed shame or guilt. Anticipatory anxiety and preoccupation with misophonic triggers appeared simultaneously. Two different scales showed 86 to 91% experienced serious preoccupation.

Most used coping was turning on music (99%) and walking away (99%). Making noise or making noise in the same rhythm, e.g., chewing or typing, was also used frequently, 86% and 77%, respectively. Finally, 86% described using earplugs; the majority of these subjects used them in the last week (73%).

In general, subjects reported to spend a lot of time actively avoiding triggers: 24% 0 to 1 hour each day, 24% 1 to 3 hours, 32% 3 to 8 hours, and 9% avoided over 8 hours each day.

General somatic

76% of the sample reported no diagnosis on Axis III, 20% reported one diagnosis, and 4% had multiple diagnoses. Most common diagnoses were: migraine, irritable bowel syndrome, asthma, and back pain. During physical examination, a primary neurological disorder was never determined, but a mild somatic comorbid disorder was regularly found (e.g., hypertension). Approximately 1% of blood tests results were abnormal (e.g., decreased Hb levels, thyroid abnormalities or increased liver functions).

Audiology

Of the total sample, four subjects (0.7%) were previously diagnosed with hyperacusis only, ten subjects (1.7%) with tinnitus only, and one patient with both. Twenty subjects reported hearing loss or other hearing problems.

The subgroup performing an audiogram consisted of 109 subjects (69% female) with a mean age of 36.70 years (SD = 12.08). 106 subjects had bilateral normal hearing (PTA < = 25 dB HL). The remaining three subjects had a unilateral hearing loss: one slight conductive hearing loss (25–40 dB HL), one moderate conductive hearing loss (40–60 dB HL), and one profound sensorineural hearing loss (80+ dB HL). This implies all participants had at least one ear with normal hearing.

Severity

Subjects had moderate to moderate-severe symptoms according to the A-MISO-S and AMISOS-R. No significant sex differences were found (respectively p = .44 and p = .29). Subjects had mild symptoms on the HAS and HDRS and a high score on the SCL-90-R. Quality of life varied from a low satisfaction on the MANSA, to some impairment in day-to-day life on the SDS, particularly with family relations[40], to a slightly lower perceived quality of life on the WHOQoL-BREF[41]. Subjects all described to have made adjustments to their day-to-day life, such as avoiding public transport. Many subjects lost work or relationships because of misophonia. Rarely, subjects were desperate for help or expressed suicidal ideations in the clinical interviews.

Personality profile

Subjects scored average on the AQ and ISS. Subjects scored low on the DS-R and on the DPSS-R[42]. There was a significant positive correlation between the DPSS-R and DS-R (r = .545, n = 442, p < .000). Further, subjects scored high on the CPQ (healthy controls in an unpublished AMC study have M = 23.82). A total of 97% had a score over 25, indicating clinical perfectionism. Subjects scored slightly higher on the FMPS (healthy controls in an unpublished AMC study have M = 92.70). A total of 66% had a score over 22 on the scale ‘Concern over mistakes’, indicating clinical perfectionism (cut off suggested by Egan & Hine[43]). There was no significant correlation between the CPQ and FMPS (r = .036, p = .571).

The NEO-PI-R sub-sample showed no sex differences and subjects scored above average (stanine 7) only on Neuroticism, with the facet Angry hostility (stanine 7). All characteristics are shown in Table 4.

Using a standard multiple regression, perfectionism (CPQ: p = .487; FMPS: p = .651), autism traits (AQ: p = .270), and disgust sensitivity (DS-R: p = .628; DPSS-R: p = .961) showed no significant relation to the severity of the misophonia symptoms, measured by the AMISOS-R.

Finally, non-parametric correlation (Spearman’s rho) was used to determine whether misophonia symptoms (A-MISO-S) correlated with a decreased quality of life (MANSA). The more severe the misophonia symptoms, the lower the satisfaction with quality of life, rs (184) = -.34 p = < .001.

Discussion

This is the largest qualitative and quantitative description of a sample of misophonia subjects so far (N = 575). Our study demonstrates that clinical examination from a medical-psychiatric perspective is invaluable for diagnosing misophonia, as one out of four referred subjects does not suffer from misophonia. Risk of misdiagnosis is high, because misophonia-like symptoms could be explained by comorbid conditions such as OCPD traits, mood disorders, AD(H)D, and ASC. From a somatic perspective, our misophonia subjects do not have specific somatic comorbid disorders. Furthermore, they have normal hearing, which is in contrast to hearing in tinnitus[44]. Prevalence of hearing loss found in our population is even less than expected based on the prevalence of disabling hearing loss in normal population for the Netherlands (PTA > 40 dB HL in better ear around 5% for adult population[45]). From a psychological perspective, misophonia can be seen as an independent construct. No association was found between misophonia symptoms and ASC, disgust sensitivity, or clinical perfectionism. Clinical perfectionism, however, was seen in 66 to 97% of the subjects. Severity of misophonia symptoms is negatively correlated with quality of life. Family relations especially suffer, but the influence on working life remains limited with only 5% on sick leave.

Overall prevalence rate of comorbid DSM-IV Axis I disorders is similar to general population in the Netherlands, except for mood disorders (twice as prevalent[46]), AD(H)D (two-and-a-half times more prevalent[46]) and the ASC (threefold the prevalence[47]). Some studies[6, 48] suggest an association with affective disorders, particularly post-traumatic stress disorder. The preliminary results of a new sample study using psychiatric evaluation[9] even showed a prevalence of 15%. However, prevalence of PTSD in our sample is not higher. The prevalence rate of comorbid DSM-IV Axis II disorders is mildly higher and corresponds with findings of Rouw & Erfanian[6]. We found OCPD traits in one out of four subjects. Regardless, the exact prevalence of personality traits in community samples is unknown, we consider a 26% prevalence of OCPD traits high. 52% of the 2013 AMC sample[2] had a comorbid OCPD. This difference can be explained by a smaller sample size and selection bias in this previous sample.

Development and severity of misophonia symptoms in this large sample are consistent with findings in the 2013 AMC sample[2]. In our sample, however, a larger percentage is female. Specifically, age of onset, course, severity of symptoms, and a positive family history[49, 6, 7] supports misophonia as a distinct disorder[50]. Findings from psychiatric, medical, and psychological assessments substantiate this conclusion. Our findings result in a revision of the 2013 criteria, which are illustrated in the next paragraph and marked in Table 5. We emphasize that to be diagnosed with misophonia, all criteria should be met. As in all psychiatric disorders a subclinical group probably exists, in most cases lacking criterion E-R.

thumbnail
Table 5. Amsterdam UMC 2020 revised diagnostic criteria for misophonia.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0231390.t005

Misophonia triggers

Our detailed investigation of triggers leads to new conclusions. Approximately all subjects in our sample report eating sounds as a trigger (96%) and the majority reports nasal or breathing sounds as a trigger (85%). Combined, all subjects report either oral or nasal sounds as a trigger. Therefore, we propose other triggers can be a part of the condition, but an emotional reaction to oral or nasal sounds is requiredᵇ. Visual triggers, like scratching, and non-human triggers, like animal sounds or air-conditioning sounds, were occasionally described[51, 52]. We indeed found evidence for non-auditory triggers in 78% of our sample, but auditory triggers are primary triggersc. If combined, these triggers cause a more intense emotional reaction. In a mass experiment which was performed among the general population, adding a corresponding image to a disgusting sound had no effect[53]. This interaction effect is probably typical for misophonia subjects.

Ambient sounds are most often reported amongst other misophonia triggersᵈ (see S2 Table). If subjects are bothered only by ambient sounds (e.g., sounds of neighbors), misophonia should not be diagnosed, even though subjects describe a similar response. Over 8% of the Dutch population reported serious nuisance by sounds of neighbors in the last year and 29% reported mild or moderate nuisance. A much higher percentage is bothered by various traffic sounds[54]. In these cases, symptoms can be seen as a more general disturbance of sounds, such as noise sensitivity or sensory over-responsivity, which also occurs in a normal population[55]. We used typical examples of avoidance from our psychiatric assessmentsᵉ.

Nature of the response

As in other samples and case studies, we show misophonia is associated with considerable non-expressed aggression, but physical aggressive outbursts are rare[3]. Subjects with a comorbid affective instability, due to for instance comorbid BPD, sometimes reported aggressive outbursts in our clinical interviews. These outbursts were mild in comparison to the internal aggressive thoughts all misophonia subjects described. Anxiety is also frequently described as a response to misophonia triggers[56, 57, 5, 8]. In our sample, subjects do not report anxiety as a prompt reaction to a trigger, but experience anticipatory anxiety and physical stress. Possibly this anticipatory anxiety is elsewhere mistaken for anxiety as a primary response. This emphasizes the value of a thorough psychiatric evaluation once more. We found all subjects consider their reaction to be out of proportion. Interestingly, only one case study describes a patient who perceived her reaction to sounds as inappropriate[58]. Subjects with a primary OCPD or ASC often do not consider their reaction to be out of proportion, and OCPD or ASC are possible differential diagnosesᶠ.

Subjects often stated the emotional response to be far more intense towards loved ones inducing misophonia triggers. Context also influenced the emotional response; when misophonia trigger sounds were made by toddlers, mentally disabled adults, or elderly with dementia, an emotional response seldom occurred.

We consider preoccupation an additional core symptom of misophonia, since approximately all subjects in our sample report hyper focusᵃ. Hyper focus was also reported in 82% of the sample of Edelstein et al[49] and an intervention targeting hyper focus has a clear effect on misophonia symptoms[59].

Limitations

This is the first large sample study of misophonia subjects in which we not only explored the dynamics of misophonia symptoms, but also examined the impact of symptoms on quality of life. However, this research also has its limitations. First, the Amsterdam UMC is the only center in the Netherlands with a specific treatment for misophonia, which could lead to a selection bias. Furthermore, since no other criteria were available, AMC 2013 diagnostic criteria were used for selection of subjects, possibly leading to confirmation bias. However, we believe we limited confirmation bias, because we examined all patients who were referred with misophonia-like symptoms; using questionnaires with a broad scope, and we investigated alternative symptoms, e.g., anxiety, in our psychiatric evaluations. Finally, types or versions of questionnaires administered changed over time in this sample. Some of the questionnaires administered are not yet validated in Dutch translation or lack a norm group, but were the best available. Also, the A-MISO-S is not psychometrically validated and the AMISOS-R is in the process of validation.

Conclusions

In conclusion, this analysis of a large sample confirms that misophonia is a distinct psychiatric disorder characterized by an intense emotional reaction of irritation, anger, and often disgust elicited by specific auditory, visual or sensory triggers predominantly induced by another person, resulting in preoccupation and avoidance. We suggest future studies to use the revised Amsterdam UMC proposed criteria and to conduct international multi-center studies. A multi-disciplinary approach, especially including psychiatry, audiology, and psychology, would be preferable. International confirmation of the Amsterdam UMC revised criteria is needed before next steps in research can be taken. Future research should also include more treatment studies (e.g., a RCT of CBT or a new intervention) and validation of misophonia questionnaires.

Acknowledgments

We thank Arjan Schröder and Arnoud van Loon for their contribution to misophonia research in general at Amsterdam UMC and Arjan for the development of misophonia questionnaires, in particular. We also thank UnYoung Chavez Baldini for textual comments on the manuscript. We thank Martijn Figee for data collection and Isidoor Bergfeld for his advice on the statistics. Finally, we thank all misophonia patients for their contribution.

References

  1. 1. Jastreboff MM, Jastreboff PJ. Components of decreased sound tolerance: hyperacusis, misophonia and phonophobia. ITHS Newsletter. 2001;2:5–7.
  2. 2. Schroder A, Vulink N, Denys D. Misophonia: Diagnostic Criteria for a New Psychiatric Disorder. PLOSONE. 2013;8 (1):e54706.
  3. 3. Taylor S. Misophonia: A new mental disorder? Med Hypotheses. 2017;103:109–117. pmid:28571795
  4. 4. Brout JJ, Edelstein M, Erfanian M, et al. Investigating Misophonia: A Review of the Empirical Literature, Clinical Implications, and a Research Agenda. Front Neurosci. 2018;12:36.
  5. 5. Wu MS, Lewin AB, Murphy TK, Storch EA. Misophonia: Incidence, phenomenology, and clinical correlates in an undergraduate student sample. J Clin Psychol. 2014;70(10):994–1007. pmid:24752915
  6. 6. Rouw R, Erfanian M. A large-scale study of misophonia. J Clin Psychol. 2017;74(3):453–479. pmid:28561277
  7. 7. Sanchez TG, Silva FED. Familial misophonia or Selective Sound Sensitivity Syndrome: Evidence for autosomal dominant inheritance? Braz J Otorhinolaryngol. 2017;84(5):553–559. pmid:28823694
  8. 8. Zhou X, Wu MS, Storch EA. Misophonia symptoms among Chinese university students: Incidence, associated impairment, and clinical correlates. J Obsessive Compuls Relat Disord. 2017;14:7–12.
  9. 9. Erfanian M, Kartsonaki C, Keshavarz A. Misophonia and comorbid psychiatric symptoms: a preliminary study of clinical findings. Nord J Psychiatry, 2019;73(4–5):219–228. pmid:31066600
  10. 10. Taylor S, Conelea CA, McKay D, Crowe KB, Abramowitz JS. Sensory intolerance: latent structure and psychopathologic correlates. Compr Psychiatry. 2014;55(5):1279–1284. pmid:24703593
  11. 11. Danesh AA, Lang D, Kaf W, Andreassen WD, Scott J, Eshraghi AA. Tinnitus and hyperacusis in autism spectrum disorders with emphasis on high functioning individuals diagnosed with Asperger's Syndrome. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2015;79(10):1683–1688. pmid:26243502
  12. 12. Tavassoli T, Miller LJ, Schoen SA, Jo Brout J, Sullivan J, Baron-Cohen S. Sensory reactivity, empathizing and systemizing in autism spectrum conditions and sensory processing disorder. Dev Cog Neurosci. 2017;29:72–77.
  13. 13. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed, text revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000.
  14. 14. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59 Suppl 20:22–33;quiz 34–57.
  15. 15. First MB, Gibbon M, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Benjamin LS. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II) Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1997.
  16. 16. Carhart R, Jerger JF. Preferred method for clinical determination of pure-tone thresholds. J Speech Hear Dis. 1959;24:330–345.
  17. 17. WHO. Report of the Informal Working Group On Prevention Of Deafness And Hearing Impairment Programme Planning. Geneva; 1991.
  18. 18. de Jonghe F. Leidraad voor het scoren van de Hamilton Depression Rating Scale. Benecke: Amsterdam; 1994.
  19. 19. Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1960;23:56–62. pmid:14399272
  20. 20. Maier W, Buller R, Philipp M, Heuser I. The Hamilton Anxiety Scale: reliability, validity and sensitivity to change in anxiety and depressive disorders. J Affect Disord. 1988;14(1):61–68. pmid:2963053
  21. 21. Hamilton M. The assessment of anxiety states by rating. Br J Med Psychol. 1959;32(1):50–55. pmid:13638508
  22. 22. Arrindell WA, Ettema JHM. SCL-90: Handleiding bij een multidimensionele psychopathologie-indicator. Swets test publishers; 1986.
  23. 23. Derogatis LR, Lipman RS, Covi L. SCL-90: an outpatient psychiatric rating scale—preliminary report. Psychopharmacol Bull. 1973;9(1):13–28. pmid:4682398
  24. 24. van Nieuwenhuizen C, Janssen-de Ruijter EAW, Nugter M. Handleiding Manchester Short Assessment of Quality of Life (MANSA); 2017.
  25. 25. Priebe S, Huxley P, Knight S, Evans S. Application and results of the Manchester Short Assessment of Quality of Life (MANSA). Int J Soc Psychiatry. 1999;45(1):7–12. pmid:10443245
  26. 26. Sheehan DV. Sheehan disability scale. Handbook of psychiatric measures 2; 1983: 100–2.
  27. 27. Trompenaars FJ, Masthoff ED, Van Heck GL, Hodiamont PP, De Vries J. Content validity, construct validity, and reliability of the WHOQOL-Bref in a population of Dutch adult psychiatric outpatients. Quality of Life Research. 2005;14(1):151–160. pmid:15789949
  28. 28. Development of the World Health Organization WHOQOL-BREF quality of life assessment. The WHOQOL Group. Psychol Med. 1998;28(3):551–558. pmid:9626712
  29. 29. Miller JD, Lynam DR, Rolland JP, et al. Scoring the DSM-IV personality disorders using the Five-Factor Model: development and validation of normative scores for North American, French, and Dutch-Flemish samples. J Pers Disord. 2008;22(5):433–450. pmid:18834293
  30. 30. Costa PT, McCrae RR. Neo Pi-R. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1992.
  31. 31. Hoekstra RA, Bartels M, Cath DC, Boomsma DI. Factor structure, reliability and criterion validity of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): a study in Dutch population and patient groups. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38(8):1555–1566. pmid:18302013
  32. 32. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E. The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001;31(1):5–17. pmid:11439754
  33. 33. van Dam-Baggen R, Kraaimaat FW. Handleiding Inventarisatielijst Omgaan met Anderen, rev. ed. (Revised Manual of the Inventory of Social Situations). Amsterdam/Lisse: Swets en Zeitlinger; 2000.
  34. 34. Shafran R, Cooper Z, Fairburn CG. Clinical perfectionism: a cognitive-behavioural analysis. Behav Res Ther. 2002;40(7):773–791. pmid:12074372
  35. 35. Frost RO, Marten P, Lahart C, Rosenblate R. The dimensions of perfectionism. Cognit Ther Res. 1990;14(5):449–468.
  36. 36. van Overveld WJM, de Jong PD, Peters ML, Cavanagh K, Davey GCL. Disgust propensity and disgust sensitivity: Separate constructs that are differentially related to specific fears. Pers Individ Dif. 2006;41(7):1241–1252.
  37. 37. Cavanagh K, Davey GCL. The development of a measure of individual differences in disgust. Winchester: British Psychology Society; 2000.
  38. 38. van Overveld M, de Jong PJ, Peters ML & Schouten E. The Disgust Scale-R: A valid and reliable index to investigate separate disgust domains? Pers Individ Dif. 2011;51:325–330.
  39. 39. Haidt J, McCauley C, Rozin P. Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Pers Individ Dif. 1994;16(5):701–713.
  40. 40. Olfson M, Broadhead WE, Weissman MM, et al. Subthreshold psychiatric symptoms in a primary care group practice. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;53(10):880–886. pmid:8857864
  41. 41. Masthoff ED, Trompenaars FJ, van Heck GL, Hodiamont PP, de Vries J. Quality of life and psychopathology: investigations into their relationship. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2006;40(4):333–340. pmid:16620315
  42. 42. Olatunji BO, Cisler JM, Deacon BJ, Connolly K, Lohr JM. The Disgust Propensity and Sensitivity Scale-Revised: psychometric properties and specificity in relation to anxiety disorder symptoms. J Anxiety Disord. 2007;21(7):918–930. pmid:17236747
  43. 43. Egan SJ, Hine P. Cognitive behavioural treatment of perfectionism: A single case experimental design series. Behaviour Change. 2008;25(4):245–258.
  44. 44. Jastreboff PJ. Phantom auditory perception (tinnitus): mechanisms of generation and perception. Neurosci Res. 1990;8(4):221–254. pmid:2175858
  45. 45. World Health Organisation. Global estimates on hearing loss. PowerPoint, numbers of 2018 https://www.who.int/deafness/estimates/en/. Accessed February 27, 2019.
  46. 46. de Graaf R, ten Have M, van Gool C, van Dorsselaer S. Prevalence of mental disorders and trends from 1996 to 2009. Results from the Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study-2. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2012;47(2):203–213. pmid:21197531
  47. 47. Roelfsema MT, Hoekstra RA, Allison C, et al. Are autism spectrum conditions more prevalent in an information-technology region? A school-based study of three regions in the Netherlands. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42(5):734–739. pmid:21681590
  48. 48. Kumar S, Tansley-Hancock O, Sedley W, et al. The Brain Basis for Misophonia. Curr biol. 2017;27(4):527–533. pmid:28162895
  49. 49. Edelstein M, Brang D, Rouw R, Ramachandran VS. Misophonia: physiological investigations and case descriptions. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:296. pmid:23805089
  50. 50. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.
  51. 51. Dozier TH. Counterconditioning treatment for misophonia. Clin Case Stud. 2015;14(5):374–387.
  52. 52. Ferreira GM, Harrison BJ, Fontenelle LF. Hatred of sounds: Misophonic disorder or just an underreported psychiatric symptom? Ann Clin Psychiatry. 2013;25(4):271–274. pmid:24199217
  53. 53. Cox TJ. The effect of visual stimuli on the horribleness of awful sounds. Applied Acoustics. 2008;691–703.
  54. 54. van Poll R, Breugelmans O, Houthuijs D, van Kamp I. Perceiving the physical living environment in the Netherlands: Annoyance inventory 2016. National Institute for Public Health and the Environment (RIVM);2018–0084 https://www.rivm.nl/bibliotheek/rapporten/2018-0084.pdf
  55. 55. Robertson AE, Simmons DR. The relationship between sensory sensitivity and autistic traits in the general population. J Autism Dev disord. 2013;43(4):775–784. pmid:22832890
  56. 56. Bruxner G. 'Mastication rage': a review of misophonia—an under-recognized symptom of psychiatric relevance? Australas Psychiatry. 2016;24(2):195–197. pmid:26508801
  57. 57. Dozier TH, Morrisson KL. Phenomenology of Misophonia: Initial Physical and Emotional Responses. Am J Psychol. 2017;130(4).
  58. 58. Bernstein RE, Angell KL, Dehle CM. A brief course of cognitive behavioural therapy for the treatment of misophonia: A case example. Cognitive Behaviour Therapist. 2013;6.
  59. 59. Schroder AE, Vulink NC, van Loon AJ, Denys DA. Cognitive behavioral therapy is effective in misophonia: An open trial. J Affect Disord. 2017;217:289–294. pmid:28441620