Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Intermanual transfer of visuomotor adaptation is related to awareness

  • Susen Werner ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    werner@dshs-koeln.de

    Affiliation Institute of Movement and Neurosciences, German Sport University, Cologne, Germany

  • Heiko K. Strüder,

    Roles Resources

    Affiliation Institute of Movement and Neurosciences, German Sport University, Cologne, Germany

  • Opher Donchin

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel

Abstract

Previous studies compared the effects of gradual and sudden adaptation on intermanual transfer to find out whether transfer depends on awareness of the perturbation. Results from different groups were contradictory. Since results of our own study suggest that awareness depends on perturbation size, we hypothesize that awareness-related intermanual transfer will only appear after adaptation to a large, sudden perturbation but not after adaptation to a small sudden perturbation or a gradual perturbation, large or small. To confirm this, four groups (S30, G30, S75, G75) of subjects performed out-and-back reaching movements with their right arm. In a baseline block, they received veridical visual feedback of hand position. In the subsequent adaptation block, feedback was rotated by 30 deg (S30, G30) or 75 deg (S75, G75). This rotation was either introduced suddenly (S30, S75) or gradually in steps of 3 deg (G30, G75). After the adaptation block, subjects did an awareness test comprising exclusion and inclusion conditions. The experiment concluded with an intermanual transfer block, in which movements were performed with the left arm under rotated feedback, and a washout block again under veridical feedback. We used a hierarchical Bayesian model to estimate individual movement directions and group averages. The movement directions in different conditions were then used to calculate group and individual indexes of adaptation, awareness, unawareness, transfer and washout. Both awareness and transfer were larger in S75 than in other groups, while unawareness and washout were smaller in S75 than in other groups. Furthermore, the size of awareness indices correlated to intermanual transfer across subjects, even when transfer was normalized to final adaptation level. Thus, we show for the first time that the amount of intermanual transfer directly relates to the extent of awareness of the learned perturbation.

Introduction

There has been an enduring public interest in the topic of two-footedness in world-class soccer players. Regardless of the actual prevalence of true two-footedness in elite players there is widespread agreement that players who can skillfully use both legs are more versatile and can choose between more options for action than players with strong foot dominance. Against this background, transferability of motor learning to different effectors is of strong interest for the field of athletic training. This holds for elite athletes, but it is also true for recreational sports and also has implications for rehabilitation. In rehabilitation, the transfer of motor skills from the trained to the untrained arm can be useful in the therapy of brain-lesioned patients [1] or in upper-limb prosthesis training [24].

Intermanual transfer after sensorimotor adaptation has been extensively studied. Sensorimotor adaptation is a specific type of motor learning where an existing motor behavior is altered in response to environmental changes [57]. One common paradigm has participants perform well known movements like reaching movements to computer generated targets. After initial training the environment is perturbed by means of a force field that pushes on the moving arm (dynamic perturbation) or by changes of visual feedback (visuomotor perturbation). These perturbations drive adaptation so that reaching behavior returns to baseline despite the perturbation. Intermanual transfer is measured by testing performance with the untrained limb after adaptation in the trained limb is complete.

Awareness is thought to be among the factors influencing intermanual transfer. Malfait and Ostry [8] compared dynamic adaptation under two conditions to directly assess the influence of awareness on transfer: force fields were introduced either suddenly or gradually. Sudden introduction caused large initial movement errors and, presumably, awareness of the perturbation. Gradual introduction did not cause large errors and seemed to be learned without awareness. After adaptation, the untrained left arm showed a benefit only after sudden introduction of the force field. However, in visuomotor adaptation, two groups (using small magnitudes of perturbation) did not see a difference [9,10]. Indeed, even explaining the perturbation to participants (presumably leading to full awareness) did not lead to increased intermanual transfer compared to adaptation without explanation [9]. Nor did providing an explicit strategy lead to an effect [10]. Accordingly, both groups concluded that awareness has a negligible effect on intermanual transfer of visuomotor adaptation.

These contradictory results could be attributed to a different pattern of intermanual transfer following adaptation to a force field or a visuomotor perturbation. Yet, it is also possible that the previous studies on visuomotor adaptation failed to demonstrate the effect of awareness on transfer because the chosen perturbation sizes (32 deg in [9] and 22.5 deg in [10]) did not cause awareness. Perturbations of 20 deg have been shown to induce almost no awareness, even when explained to subjects in advance [11,12]. In at least one study, even perturbations as large as 40 deg engaged very little awareness [11]. Hence, the different manipulations of adaptation condition may have not resulted in the desired effect on awareness of the perturbation. This is an issue because studies of intermanual transfer, generally, did not test awareness directly. Rather, they either assumed that sudden and gradual introduction of the perturbation would cause differential levels of awareness [8], or else they tested for a difference using a post-session questionnaire [9,10]. It has been argued that verbal responses may not reveal all a subjects’ knowledge because the knowledge is held with low confidence or because of the difference of retrieval contexts (e.g. [1315]).

A growing interest in explicit and implicit processes of sensorimotor adaptation has seen the development of methods for assessment of cognitive aspects such as awareness of adaptation. Some methods rely on subjects reporting their intended direction of movement (termed prediction methods in cognitive science). This can be done in various ways such as indicating when a rotating line matches their aiming direction [16,17] or reporting the direction explicitly with the help of a circular display of landmarks [18]. Using such methods, Poh et al. [19] found that visuomotor rotation to 45 deg had both explicit and implicit components. However, since reporting continued during transfer, the paradigm may have encouraged an explicit transfer (as subjects made an effort to maintain consistency with the previously reported strategy). In addition, prediction methods can be based on feelings of familiarity [20] and lead to an overestimation of awareness [21].

Jacoby [20] introduced the process dissociation procedure (PDP) as a method for determining awareness and it is now in wide use by cognitive scientists (e.g. [22,23]). The PDP is based on defining conscious knowledge as controllable knowledge. Thus, aware and unaware learning can be estimated by comparing performance when participants attempt to either express or repress a learned behavior. We recently implemented this method in the field of sensorimotor adaptation [11]. We were able to directly measure awareness and unawareness indices after adaptation to different rotation angles, and to show that awareness depends on perturbation size.

Our current study applies the PDP to measure the involvement of awareness in transfer of visuomotor adaptation to the untrained limb. We compare intermanual transfer after sudden and gradual adaptation to different sizes of visual perturbations. We measure the participants’ awareness of the perturbation by means of the PDP. This should address the criticism that previous findings were tainted by systematic reinforcement of explicit strategies. If awareness is involved in intermanual transfer of visuomotor adaptation, we should find the amount of transfer to be correlated with the amount of awareness. Our hypothesis is that we will find intermanual transfer only after sudden adaptation to a large perturbation but not after sudden adaptation to a small perturbation or after gradual adaptation.

Materials and methods

Participants

Twenty-four female and twenty-four male subjects participated in the study and were randomly assigned to four groups. Their ages ranged from 18 to 29 years and were all adults in the country of testing. Participants were gender and age-matched between the four groups and all participants were right-handed as confirmed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory. None of the subjects had any experience in visuomotor adaptation research or exhibited overt sensorimotor deficits besides corrected vision. The experimental protocol was conducted according to the principles expressed in the Declaration of Helsinki and was pre-approved by the ethical committee of the German Sport University Cologne (approval number: 24/2015). All subjects gave written informed consent.

Task

Seated subjects performed reaching movements with a pen on a digitizing tablet (Fig 1). They watched a computer screen through a mirror, such that the virtual image of the screen appeared in the same plane in which subjects performed their movements. The mirror prevented vision of the arm, and the position of the pen was registered and displayed to the participants in real-time as a light-blue cursor on the screen. A yellow starting dot appeared in the center of the virtual display and was then replaced by one of eight possible yellow target dots for a duration of 1000 ms. Target directions were 0 deg, 45 deg, 90 deg,… and 315 deg. The targets were presented in random order and all dots were 5 mm in diameter. The subjects were asked to reach as quickly and accurately as possible from the central starting dot in the direction of the target and to land the cursor on the target. After that the participants actively returned the cursor to the starting dot and movements were continuously performed during an episode of 35 s duration interrupted by rest breaks of 5 s. Thus, stopping at the target didn´t end the trial and was rather short. During some movements the subjects’ task was to perform the same reaching without visual feedback, i.e. no cursor visible outside the starting dot location (radius 10 mm).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Scheme of experimental apparatus.

Shown are tablet (T), screen (S) and mirror (M).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0220748.g001

An overview of the experimental protocols is given in Table 1. After familiarization with veridical feedback all participants conducted baseline episodes without visual feedback as well as baseline episodes with the left and right hand. Depending on group, the perturbation during the following adaptation phase was introduced either suddenly or gradually in steps of 3 deg per episode. The first and second group were gradually (G30) or suddenly (S30) exposed to 30 deg rotated visual feedback, whereas the third and fourth group were gradually (G75) or suddenly (S75) exposed to 75 deg rotated visual feedback. Next came two episodes each of inclusion and exclusion to test for awareness in a process dissociation procedure as in [11,20]. Before inclusion subjects were instructed to ‘use what was learned during adaptation’ and before exclusion subjects were asked to ‘refrain from using what was learned, perform movements as during baseline’ [11]. The order of inclusion and exclusion episodes was randomized between participants. After that, transfer to the left hand was tested during two further episodes. No visual feedback was given during inclusion, exclusion and transfer episodes. In particular, visual feedback was not given during transfer episodes in order to prevent confounding transfer with learning benefits to opposite limb learning (“learning to learn”) [19]. Learning was refreshed during two intermediate episodes each under rotated feedback. The experimental protocol concluded with a de-adaptation phase under veridical feedback.

Data processing

We quantified participants’ reaching performance as movement direction with respect to the target 150 ms after movement onset, i.e., before feedback-based corrections could become effective [24]. In order to allow the comparison of adaptation to the different rotation sizes, we calculated normalized indices for the different parameters of each participant. Adaptation index, transfer index and washout index were determined as (1) with Movement direction equal to mean movement direction of: movements in an adaptation episode for adaptation index for that episode; movements in both transfer episodes for transfer index; movements in the first two washout episodes for washout index. Baseline was the mean movement direction of all baseline episodes of the right hand for adaptation and washout index and of the left hand for transfer index. Note, that we defined the amount of transfer as absolute values of transfer episodes as done previously (e.g. [25]).

To account for differences of the amount of adaptation we also calculated a normalized transfer index as (2) with Movement direction last adaptation episode equal to the mean direction of movements in the last adaptation episode. Movement direction and Baseline were again determined as above.

We further determined awareness and unawareness from reaching performance during exclusion and inclusion episodes. To this end we first calculated exclusion and inclusion indices as (3)

Here, Movement direction was the mean movement direction of exclusion or inclusion episodes for the exclusion and inclusion indices, respectively, and End of adaptation is taken from the last episode of adaptation phase. According to the PDP, an estimate of awareness can be obtained from the difference between exclusion and inclusion performance and an estimate of unawareness can be obtained from the difference between exclusion performance and baseline. Thus, we calculated the awareness index as inclusion minus exclusion index and the unawareness index equals the exclusion index as in our previous work [11].

We performed a Bayesian statistical analysis. Data and code for this analysis is provided on OSF (https://osf.io/d6swf/). The Bayesian approach is based on specifying a model with specific parameters for where the data came from. The idea is to characterize what we know about the parameters, given the data. For instance, our basic model was that the data was normally distributed around a line and our parameters were the slope and intercept of the line and the variance of the normal distribution. To determine how the data provides information about the parameters, we sample the joint probability distribution of the parameters given the data [26]. This provides a probability associated with every possible value of the parameter instead of simply providing a single best possible estimate. Our approach followed closely that described in [26], a textbook outlining practical Bayesian approaches to problems of linear regression and ANOVA. Our own approach is very close in spirit to performing an ANOVA. We fit a regression model to the mean movement directions of each subject in each episode. The regression model had a term for the episode type (34 types for 48 episodes: baseline no feedback, baseline left, baseline right, 25 adaptation episodes, exclusion, inclusion, transfer, refresh, early washout, and late washout), subject group (S30, G30, S75, and G75), and subject (see S1 Model). It also included terms for the interaction of episode and group and episode and subject. All the coefficients of the regression model were assumed to be normally distributed with a broad, uninformative prior. Noise around the regression model was assumed to have a normal distribution with a standard deviation that varied with subject. The per-subject standard deviation had a gamma distribution whose parameters were sampled hierarchically and had broad priors.

Given the model described above and the data, we now wish to sample the posterior distribution of the parameters. Samples will be more frequent for values of the parameter that are more credible given the data and less frequent for values that are not credible based on the data. To get a good sense of the regions of high credibility, we need a large number of samples (50,000–100,000 are commonly used). The actual sampling was done using a program specially designed for this task. We used JAGS (4.2.0, http://mcmc-jags.sourceforge.net/) called from Matlab using matjags (http://psiexp.ss.uci.edu/research/programs_data/jags/). We used 4 chains, 1,000 adaptation samples, 4,000 burn in samples and 20,000 samples per chain on which we performed our analysis. This gave us a total of 80,000 samples of each parameter. Chains were initialized using the bootstrap approach described in [26]. We analyzed the standard diagnostics described there as well to ensure that chains had converged for all parameters, that the results were consistent across chains, and that the number of effective samples was in the range of 16,000–20,000 for variance parameters and in the range 22,000–70,000 for location parameters. JAGS code for the model as well as Matlab code specifying exactly how JAGS was run, as well as the full posterior sampling are provided online (https://osf.io/d6swf/).

Using the sampled estimates of the regression coefficients, we reconstructed the posterior distribution of the mean movement direction for each episode type for each subject and for each group. One advantage of having sampled the posterior distributions is that we can use these samples to generate posterior distributions of other values of interest. For instance, we can get the posterior distribution of the indices described above: adaptation (for each adaptation episode), awareness, unawareness, transfer and washout. We do this by calculating the index on each sample of the parameters. This gives us 80,000 calculated values of the index that, together, represent a sample from its posterior distribution given the data.

We present our results for all indices both for individual subjects and for the group primarily using the 95% high density interval (HDI). This is a region containing 95% of the posterior distribution where every point in the region has higher probability than any point outside the region (see Figure 25.1 in [26] for an illustration of the HDI). Furthermore, for group comparisons we specified a region of perceived equivalence (ROPE) of -0.05 through 0.05 around a value of 0 difference between the groups. Probabilities of less than 0.01% are reported as negligible. We report the percentage of the HDI that lies within the ROPE as a measure of the probability that the values are equivalent. All analyses were performed using Matlab (Version R2018b).

In addition to the Bayesian statistical analysis, we performed traditional frequentist statistical tests. The description of these analyses as well as their results are provided as S1 Statistics.

Results

Fig 2 shows mean angular movement directions of all experimental phases and groups. (Data is provided as S1 Dataset). Note that exclusion is depicted here before inclusion although, in fact, their order was randomised between participants. We did not expect any order effects since we did not find any in our previous study [11]. However, in G75 and S75 we found more awareness and more transfer in participants doing first exclusion and then inclusion. The results of this analysis are provided as supporting information (S2 Dataset and S1 Results). Movement directions are near zero during baseline (BAS) for all four groups. All groups show adaptation during the adaptation phase (ADAP). The exceptionally large value in the first adaptation episode of S75 is probably due to irregular reaching behavior during this episode in this group (for sample movement paths for each group see S2 Results). While angular movement directions decrease gradually in group G30 and G75, they become abruptly negative in group S30 and S75. The two groups adapting to a rotation of 30 deg reach a similar level of movement directions after the first half of adaptation phase which is maintained throughout the subsequent phases. The two groups adapting to a rotation of 75 deg are moving to different directions at the end of adaptation: adaptation is nearly complete in S75 and much less so in G75. Mean group movement directions by the last episode of adaptation phase were -25.5 ± 1.0 (G30), -24.5 ± 0.7 (S30), -51.5 ± 2.4 (G75) and -70.3 ± 5.0 (S75). The level of adaptation is maintained in the subsequent refresh phases in G30 and S30 while it seems to increase in G75 and decrease in S75 (R). The inclusion phase (IN) shows essentially the same level of adaptation, as well. During the exclusion phase, the S30 and G30 groups continue to perform similarly, while the S75 and G75 groups have an inversion. Despite larger adaptation in the S75 group, it shows a much greater ability to exclude this learning than the G75 group in the exclusion phase (EX). This effect is repeated in the washout phase (DA), where S75 washes out much more quickly than G75. The groups showed different levels of partial transfer in the transfer set (TL).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Mean angular movement errors of all blocks and groups.

Shown are baseline (BAS), adaptation (ADAP), exclusion (EX), refresh (R), inclusion (IN), intermanual transfer (TL) and de-adaptation (DA). Lines indicate across-subject means, and the shaded area display standard errors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0220748.g002

Adaptation

To compare learning across groups, we examined the posterior probability distributions for adaptation indices of the group means as well as the posterior probability distributions of the adaptation indexes for individual subjects. Fig 3A depicts the mean of the posterior distribution of the adaptation indices for each group overlaid on its HDI. All groups show adaptation. In addition, adaptation in G75 rises more slowly than G30. This is because adaptation rate in these groups was fixed to 3 deg per movement and so the relative rate in G75 is slower. To compare final adaptation, we calculated the posterior distribution of the difference between the mean of the last 10 episodes for each pair of groups. We stipulated that groups would be considered equivalent if the difference lay in a ROPE of -0.05 to 0.05. Using this procedure, we found a 99.7% chance that final adaptation of G30 was equivalent to that of S30 and a 91% chance that it was equivalent to that of S75. Similarly, there was a 94% chance that final adaptation of S30 and S75 were equivalent. On the other hand, posterior distribution of the difference of final adaptation between all these groups and the G75 group did not overlap at all with the ROPE, indicating less than 0.1% chance that their relative adaptation is similar to that of G75.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Adaptation.

Group means and their HDIs of posterior probability distributions for adaptation indices (A). Average difference from G30 (B) and S30 (C) of the last ten adaptation bins.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0220748.g003

These results indicate that learning at the end of adaptation phase was not complete in group G75. Therefore we performed a comparison of the posterior distributions of the difference of the last adaptation episode, all refresh episodes and also the first washout episode within each group. We again determined effective equality using a ROPE of -0.05 to 0.05. For groups G30, S30 and S75 we found a 39%, 25% or 39% chance that final adaptation was equivalent to refresh, respectively. For G75, however, we found a significant increase of index with a chance of equivalence between final adaptation and refresh being less than 0.1%. Moreover, we compared refresh indices across groups by calculating the posterior distribution of the differences between the estimates of the group indices. This analysis revealed 55%, 57% or 54% chances of equivalence between refresh index of G30, S30 or S75 and that of G75, respectively. These results suggests that learning continued during refresh phase in G75 and final adaptation level approached that of the other groups (for detailed results see S3 Results).

Awareness, unawareness, intermanual transfer and washout

Fig 4A shows the means and HDIs of the posterior probability distributions for the group awareness, unawareness, intermanual transfer and washout indices. We performed group comparisons for these indices as was done for the analysis of refresh indices. We show an overview of the results of all group comparisons in Table 2A. For S75 we found larger awareness and transfer indices and smaller unawareness and washout indices compared to all other groups. Our analysis revealed less than 0.1% chance of equivalence for most of these comparisons. While Fig 4A reveals similar sized indices for G30 and S30, G75 seems to be larger than those two groups in awareness and smaller in unawareness and washout. However, this difference was only significant for washout indices with less than 0.1% chance of equivalence between G75 and both G30 and S30. Even though Fig 4A suggests a larger transfer in S30 than G30, this difference was not significant with a 3% chance of equivalence. Thus, statistical analyses reveal a similar pattern of results for awareness and transfer indices with S75 being larger than the other groups and, similarly, the analyses of unawareness and washout indices yield differences of S75 and G75 (for washout only) to all other groups.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Awareness, unawareness, intermanual transfer and washout.

Group means and their HDIs of posterior probability distributions for awareness, unawareness, intermanual transfer and washout indices (A) as well as for normalized intermanual transfer (B).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0220748.g004

One consideration is that our indices measure transfer on an absolute scale instead of normalizing for the amount of overall learning. We decided to recalculate the amount of intermanual transfer as a proportion of reaching behavior in the last adaptation episode. Means and HDIs of the posterior probability distributions of this normalized transfer index are shown in Fig 4B and results of Bayesian analysis in Table 2B. The results for normalized transfer resemble those of absolute transfer with regard to a larger index in S75 than in the other groups. However, this difference was only significant (less than 0.1% chance of equivalence) between S75 and G30. Again we found no significant difference between the two groups adapting to a small rotation with a 2% chance that normalized transfer index of S30 was equivalent to that of G30.

Fig 5 gives a clearer view of individual subjects performance for awareness, unawareness, intermanual transfer and washout indices. Here each line represents one subject and the range of the line is the HDI for this individual. The subjects are sorted by their mean score on that parameter and colored according to their group. Subjects in the S75 group are, on the whole, in the upper third of the awareness index across subjects and similarly sorted to the top end of the transfer and normalized transfer distributions. It can be seen that there are no G30 or S30 subjects in the top of the sample in terms of awareness and no G30 subjects past the median in transfer. Moreover, for the unawareness index the four adaptation conditions led to even clearer group selectivity with the subjects in S75 being (with one exception) the least unaware of all subjects and the subjects in G75 tending to have less unawareness than the other two groups. For the washout index distribution according to groups was also achieved in part.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Individual subjects performance for awareness, unawareness, intermanual transfer and washout.

Each line represents one subject and the range of the line is the HDI for that individual. Subjects are sorted by their mean score and coloured according to their group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0220748.g005

The results of group means reveal similar patterns for awareness and intermanual transfer indices. Moreover the results of individual subjects performance of these indices reveal considerable inter-individual differences within groups. To further scrutinize the relation between awareness and intermanual transfer we thus calculated correlations between both parameters within and across all groups. Fig 6 demonstrates that larger awareness of the learned perturbation is related to larger transfer (Fig 6a) as well as to larger normalized transfer (Fig 6b) to the other limb. The HDI for regression coefficients is 0.26 < r < 0.63 or 0.19 < r < 0.65 for correlations between awareness and transfer or normalized transfer, respectively. In addition, we calculated the HDIs of regression coefficients within each group. Results are shown in Table 3. Note that for transfer as well as for normalized transfer correlations are stronger for the two groups adapting to a large rotation size. In addition, we also calculated a correlation between unawareness and transfer. The more awareness a subject has the less unawareness there should be. Accordingly we find a negative correlation between unawareness and transfer (Fig 6c) or, less strong, between unawareness and normalized transfer (Fig 6d). The HDIs of regression coefficients across all groups and within each group are also are shown in Table 3.

thumbnail
Fig 6. Correlations between awareness, unawareness, intermanual transfer and normalized intermanual transfer.

Correlations between the awareness and transfer (A) or normalized transfer (B) indices as well as correlations between the unawareness and transfer (C) and normalized transfer (D) indices for all participants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0220748.g006

thumbnail
Table 3. Results of correlations between awareness or unawareness and intermanual transfer.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0220748.t003

Discussion

One essential question in sensorimotor adaptation research concerns the generalization or transfer of sensorimotor adaptation: The investigation of transfer to different body segments, different movements or different work-spaces can give insight into the nature of motor learning and the brain structures involved. The aim of the present study was to find out whether the amount of intermanual transfer depends on the degree of awareness of the nature of the perturbation. Therefore, four groups (S30, G30, S75, G75) of participants adapted to a visuomotor perturbation. Visual feedback was rotated either by 30 deg (S30, G30) or 75 deg (S75, G75) and this rotation was either introduced suddenly (S30, S75) or gradually in steps of 3 deg (G30, G75). We also measured awareness and unawareness indices after adaptation with a process dissociation procedure and, additionally, the amount of intermanual transfer to the untrained, non-dominant arm and washout performance. Groups with most awareness seemed to show the most transfer. Awareness was larger in S75 than in all other groups and transfer was larger in S75 than in G75 and G30. This pattern held for both the absolute level of transfer and also, although more weakly, when transfer was normalized by the amount of final adaptation. Furthermore, at the individual level, subjects with more awareness tend to showed more transfer.

Intermanual transfer

We found intermanual transfer to be more pronounced after sudden adaptation to a large perturbation than after gradual adaptation to either a large or small perturbation. Moreover, we found a correlation between awareness and intermanual transfer over all groups, even when transfer was normalized to final adaptation level. These findings support our hypothesis that awareness is indeed involved in intermanual transfer of visuomotor adaptation. Specifically, the relation between the amount of awareness and of transfer was more pronounced after adaptation to large perturbations. Poh et al. [19] also revealed a contribution of the explicit process of sensorimotor adaptation to intermanual transfer. However, their reporting method of testing explicit adaptation might have actually caused transfer of explicit learning itself as outlined in the introduction. Thus, using the PDP for measuring awareness we could, for the first time, show the involvement of awareness in intermanual transfer of visuomotor adaptation without creating a situation where testing for awareness might reinforce explicit strategies.

Our results also showed an interesting interaction: abrupt introduction of the perturbation led to more transfer when the perturbation was large but not when it was small. This may explain the apparent contradictions in previous studies, with some showing different intermanual transfer after gradual and sudden adaptation [1,8] but others do not [9,10]. Our findings suggest that the latter did not see an effect of awareness on transfer because small perturbation sizes did not induce awareness even in abrupt perturbations. Even explicit instructions did not lead to increased intermanual transfer in those studies [9,10]. It could be argued, that instructing the participants about the nature of the perturbation should lead to awareness. However, awareness of small visuomotor rotations remains minor (about 25% after 20 deg rotation) even after explicit instructions [11].

The amount of intermanual transfer cannot be fully explained by the amount of awareness in the present study. Even after gradual adaptation to a 30 deg rotation with awareness of about 15% transfer reaches about 30% and normalized transfer reaches about 35%. Moreover, while there is no doubt that awareness and transfer are correlated across individuals, these correlations are not strong. The HDI for the correlation coefficient was 0.26 < r < 0.63 and 0.19 < r < 0.65 for transfer and for normalized transfer, respectively. Thus, it is likely that the implicit process of adaptation is also involved in transfer to the other hand as previously suggested [19]. It is equally possible that other factors are involved such as the degree of handedness [27,28].

It is also conceivable that the amount of intermanual transfer in the present study is confounded by group differences of time spend at plateau during learning [29]. While most groups in our study reach a similar plateau early in adaptation and remain there, the G75 does not reach a plateau at all and has less adaptation than the other groups for the last ten episodes. It has been suggested that extended training should lead to greater transfer [29] although these results have not been consistent in the literature [30]. However, because our study included groups with prolonged training at the plateau that did not show strong transfer (the S30 and G30 groups), we do not believe that the result in the G75 group is strongly affected by failing to reach plateau. Thus, we suggest that awareness and not time spent at plateau drive the amount of intermanual transfer.

In order to further confirm this, we tested specifically for normalized transfer in addition to absolute transfer. Even though our results are not as strong for normalized transfer, the general pattern of the results is the same: awareness correlated to both our transfer measures.

Given that we only tested intermanual transfer from the dominant, right to the non-dominant, left arm as done by Malfait and Ostry [8] our findings might be restricted to this direction of transfer. While asymmetry of intermanual transfer of visuomotor adaptation was first proposed by Sainburg and Wang [25] an inconsistent picture has emerged since then: Some studies showed transfer only from the dominant to the non-dominant arm after dynamic adaptation [31,32] and others showed transfer in both directions after dynamic [33] or visuomotor adaptation in elderly participants [34]. Importantly, Poh et al. [19] neither found an asymmetry of total transfer nor of explicit transfer in a visuomotor adaptation experiment. For our understanding of directionality of intermanual transfer a comprehensive study should be conducted controlling for type (visuomotor/dynamic), workspaces used, type of feedback (online/terminal) and perturbation size since they all differed during aforementioned investigations.

Awareness and unawareness

The degree of awareness of the nature of the perturbation did clearly depend on the perturbations´ magnitude during sudden adaptation. Hence awareness is driven by the size of target error, i.e. the perceived error between cursor and target. This result is consistent with our previous work showing lager awareness after adaptation to a 60 deg than to a 20 deg rotation of visual feedback [11]. For the first time, the degree of awareness was actually measured after adaptation to a gradually introduced perturbation. As suggested earlier [11,3537] we indeed find very low awareness but large unawareness indices after gradual adaptation.

Performance in aftereffect tests such as pointing directions at the beginning of a washout block is thought to reflect the actual recalibration of sensory-to-motor transformation rules [38,39]. Compared to sudden adaptation, performance in aftereffect tests was previously shown to be improved after gradual saccade [40] and prism adaptation [1], after gradual adaptation to a visual gain [41] and to a 90 deg rotation [42]. Adaptation to smaller visual rotations (30 deg and 60 deg), however, did not lead to a difference of aftereffects between gradual or sudden adaptation [37,43]. This is in line with the present findings. Our analyses reveal a difference of washout indices after gradual or sudden adaptation to a 75 deg but not after gradual or sudden adaptation to a 30 deg rotation. In addition, we find a similar pattern of results for washout and for unawareness indices. Therefore, we can conclude that the amount of recalibration does not depend on the learning condition such as gradual versus sudden, but is negatively related to the amount of awareness gained during adaptation.

It can be argued that gaining awareness does not directly indicate the explicit process of adaptation since it does not ensure the use of explicit strategies [44]. In that respect the inappropriate use of the term awareness should first be mentioned. Some authors relate to awareness of a perturbation, i.e. the notion that something has changed [36,45], while we specifically refer to awareness of the nature of the perturbation as also done in previous other work [9,46].

Second, the mental construct of awareness might very well be more than just the participants’ report of planned movement directions and the PDP measure and the trial-by-trial reporting might reflect two distinct components of the explicit process. Then again, characterization of implicit and explicit processes over a range of task conditions lead to similar results independent of which method was used for measuring the two processes: The explicit process increases with increasing rotation size and the implicit process takes time to develop during adaptation measured by reporting [18,47] and, likewise, by PDP [11,12]. Further research should be conducted to directly compare the explicit process of sensorimotor adaptation measured by prediction task methods such as reporting of aiming directions [18] or by the process dissociation procedure [11].

Neuronal correlates of intermanual transfer

It has been suggested that intermanual transfer of sensorimotor adaptation may involve several aspects of a neural representation, some of which are effector dependent and others are effector independent [48]. It can be speculated that cognitive processes such as the explicit process or awareness of a perturbation do not directly relate to a motor action of a specific limb and that they might, thus, relate to an effector independent neural representation. It has been suggested that the explicit process plays a role in an early stage of the adaptation process [46]. Since several imaging studies on sensorimotor adaptation found early learning to engage prefrontal brain regions (e.g. [4951]), especially the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [52] this brain area might be related to the explicit process of adaptation. Considering our present findings of intermanual transfer being related to awareness this area may also be related to the transfer of adaptation.

Imaging of intermanual transfer of visuomotor adaptation previously revealed activation in the temporal cortex, the middle occipital gyrus and the right medial frontal gyrus [50]. However, adaptation to a 30 deg rotation was used in that study, which is largely related to the unaware, implicit process of adaptation as suggested previously [11,12] and confirmed by the present data. Accordingly, Anguera et al. [50] observed no areas of overlap comparing activation at transfer and early adaptation but partial overlap at transfer and late adaptation. These findings suggest, in combination with our data, that the above mentioned areas might be more related to the transfer of the implicit process of adaptation. Future research should be conducted to scrutinize whether brain areas related to the unaware or implicit process of adaptation are also related to the unaware or implicit process of intermanual transfer. For example, indirect evidence from patient studies supports the involvement of the cerebellum mainly in the implicit process during adaptation itself. For instance, cerebellar patients show decreased aftereffects (e.g. [5356]) or impaired gradual adaptation associated with less awareness [57,58]. However, transcranial direct current stimulation on the trained or untrained hemisphere of the right cerebellum caused faster adaptation, but did not affect intermanual transfer [29].

Conclusions

In conclusion, the results of this study show for the first time that the size of intermanual transfer relates to the extent of awareness of the learned visuomotor perturbation. Our findings can further help explain the disagreement regarding the amount of intermanual transfer after gradual and sudden adaptation by proposing that some studies chose perturbation sizes which left participants adapting to the suddenly introduced distortion just as unaware as those in the gradual group. The amount of transfer cannot be fully explained by awareness in the present study suggesting the involvement of additional factors like the implicit process of adaptation or handedness. Moreover, our findings confirm the idea that participants adapting to gradually introduced perturbations are usually not aware and that the size of aftereffects is negatively related to the amount of awareness gained during adaptation. Our results also underscore the complexity of the implicit and explicit processes of sensorimotor adaptation and emphasize the importance of directly comparing the different tasks of measuring the explicit process or awareness. To sum up, our results can further help explaining the effect of awareness on sensorimotor adaptation and its transfer to the other limb.

Supporting information

S1 Model. Model used for the Bayesian analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0220748.s001

(PDF)

S1 Dataset. Mean movement directions of each episode (baseline, adaptation, awareness test, refresh, intermanual transfer and deadaptation) for each participant of each group (G30, S30, G75, S75).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0220748.s003

(CSV)

S2 Dataset. Order of exclusion and inclusion of awareness test for each subject; 1 means exclusion first and 0 means inclusion first.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0220748.s004

(CSV)

S1 Results. Results of the analysis of the order effect of exclusion and inclusion phase.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0220748.s005

(PDF)

S2 Results. Original registrations of movement paths of the first adaptation episode produced by a typical participant of each group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0220748.s006

(PDF)

S3 Results. Results of the analysis of the refresh phase.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0220748.s007

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Mats Harmuth for his help with data collection.

References

  1. 1. Michel C, Pisella L, Prablanc C, Rode G, Rossetti Y. Enhancing visuomotor adaptation by reducing error signals: single-step (aware) versus multiple-step (unaware) exposure to wedge prisms. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19: 341–350. pmid:17280521
  2. 2. Weeks DL, Wallace SA, Anderson DI. Training with an upper-limb prosthetic simulator to enhance transfer of skill across limbs. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2003;84: 437–443. pmid:12638114
  3. 3. Romkema S, Bongers RM, van der Sluis CK. Intermanual Transfer in Training With an Upper-Limb Myoelectric Prosthesis Simulator: A Mechanistic, Randomized, Pretest-Posttest Study. Phys Ther. 2013;93: 22–31. pmid:22976445
  4. 4. Romkema S, Bongers RM, van der Sluis CK. Influence of the type of training task on intermanual transfer effects in upper-limb prosthesis training: A randomized pre-posttest study. Philip BA, editor. PLoS One. 2017;12: e0188362. pmid:29190727
  5. 5. Cunningham HA, Welch RB. Multiple concurrent visual-motor mappings: implications for models of adaptation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1994;20: 987–99. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7964533 pmid:7964533
  6. 6. Cohen MM. Continuous versus terminal visual feedback in prism aftereffects. Percept Mot Skills. 1967;24: 1295–302. pmid:5623034
  7. 7. Imamizu H, Shimojo S. The locus of visual-motor learning at the task or manipulator level: implications from intermanual transfer. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1995;21: 719–33. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7643045 pmid:7643045
  8. 8. Malfait N, Ostry DJ. Is interlimb transfer of force-field adaptation a cognitive response to the sudden introduction of load? J Neurosci. 2004;24: 8084–8089. pmid:15371509
  9. 9. Wang J, Joshi M, Lei Y. The extent of interlimb transfer following adaptation to a novel visuomotor condition does not depend on awareness of the condition. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106: 259–264. pmid:21562196
  10. 10. Taylor JA, Wojaczynski GJ, Ivry RB. Trial-by-trial analysis of intermanual transfer during visuomotor adaptation. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106: 3157–3172. pmid:21917998
  11. 11. Werner S, van Aken BC, Hulst T, Frens MA, van der Geest JN, Strüder HK, et al. Awareness of sensorimotor adaptation to visual rotations of different size. PLoS One. 2015;10: e0123321. pmid:25894396
  12. 12. Neville K-M, Cressman EK. The influence of awareness on explicit and implicit contributions to visuomotor adaptation over time. Exp Brain Res. 2018;236: 2047–2059. pmid:29744566
  13. 13. Eriksen CW. Discrimination and learning without awareness: a methodological survey and evaluation. Psychol Rev. 1960;67: 279–300. pmid:13697142
  14. 14. Nisbett RE, Wilson TD. Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychol Rev. 1977; 231–259.
  15. 15. Shanks DR, Rowland LA, Ranger MS. Attentional load and implicit sequence learning. Psychol Res. 2005;69: 369–382. pmid:15856286
  16. 16. Heuer H, Hegele M. Adaptation to visuomotor rotations in younger and older adults. Psychol Aging. 2008;23: 190–202. pmid:18361666
  17. 17. Hegele M, Heuer H. Implicit and explicit components of dual adaptation to visuomotor rotations. Conscious Cogn. 2010;19: 906–917. pmid:20537562
  18. 18. Taylor JA, Krakauer JW, Ivry RB. Explicit and implicit contributions to learning in a sensorimotor adaptation task. J Neurosci. 2014;34: 3023–3032. pmid:24553942
  19. 19. Poh E, Carroll TJ, Taylor JA. Effect of coordinate frame compatibility on the transfer of implicit and explicit learning across limbs. J Neurophysiol. 2016;116: 1239–1249. pmid:27334955
  20. 20. Jacoby L. A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional use of memory. J Mem Lang. 1991; 513–541.
  21. 21. Cleeremans A. Consciousness: the radical plasticity thesis. Prog Brain Res. 2008;168: 19–33. pmid:18166383
  22. 22. Destrebecqz A, Cleeremans A. Can sequence learning be implicit? New evidence with the process dissociation procedure. Psychon Bull {&} Rev. 2001;8: 343–350.
  23. 23. Karabanov A, Ullén F. Implicit and explicit learning of temporal sequences studied with the process dissociation procedure. J Neurophysiol. 2008;100: 733–739. pmid:18497356
  24. 24. Cressman EK, Franks IM, Enns JT, Chua R. No automatic pilot for visually guided aiming based on colour. Exp brain Res. 2006;171: 174–83. pmid:16307249
  25. 25. Sainburg RL, Wang J. Interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations: independence of direction and final position information. Exp Brain Res. 2002;145: 437–447. pmid:12172655
  26. 26. Kruschke JK. Doing Bayesian Data Analysis: A Tutorial with R, JAGS, and Stan [Internet]. Elsevier LTD, Oxford; 2015.
  27. 27. Chase C, Seidler R. Degree of handedness affects intermanual transfer of skill learning. Exp Brain Res. 2008;190: 317–328. pmid:18592225
  28. 28. Lefumat HZ, Vercher J-L, Miall RC, Cole J, Buloup F, Bringoux L, et al. To transfer or not to transfer? Kinematics and laterality quotient predict interlimb transfer of motor learning. J Neurophysiol. 2015;114: 2764–2774. pmid:26334018
  29. 29. Block H, Celnik P. Stimulating the Cerebellum Affects Visuomotor Adaptation but not Intermanual Transfer of Learning. The Cerebellum. 2013;12: 781–793. pmid:23625383
  30. 30. Lei Y, Wang J. Prolonged training does not result in a greater extent of interlimb transfer following visuomotor adaptation. Brain Cogn. 2014;91: 95–99. pmid:25282328
  31. 31. Criscimagna-Hemminger SE, Donchin O, Gazzaniga MS, Shadmehr R. Learned Dynamics of Reaching Movements Generalize From Dominant to Nondominant Arm. J Neurophysiol. 2003;89: 168–176. pmid:12522169
  32. 32. Wang J, Sainburg RL. Interlimb Transfer of Novel Inertial Dynamics Is Asymmetrical. J Neurophysiol. 2004;92: 349–360. pmid:15028745
  33. 33. Stockinger C, Thürer B, Focke A, Stein T. Intermanual transfer characteristics of dynamic learning: direction, coordinate frame, and consolidation of interlimb generalization. J Neurophysiol. 2015;114: 3166–3176. pmid:26424581
  34. 34. Wang J, Przybyla A, Wuebbenhorst K, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL. Aging reduces asymmetries in interlimb transfer of visuomotor adaptation. Exp Brain Res. 2011;210: 283–290. pmid:21424842
  35. 35. Criscimagna-Hemminger SE, Bastian AJ, Shadmehr R. Size of error affects cerebellar contributions to motor learning. J Neurophysiol. 2010;103: 2275–2284. pmid:20164398
  36. 36. Galea JM, Sami SA, Albert NB, Miall RC. Secondary tasks impair adaptation to step- and gradual-visual displacements. Exp brain Res. 2010;202: 473–484. pmid:20101396
  37. 37. Klassen J, Tong C, Flanagan JR. Learning and recall of incremental kinematic and dynamic sensorimotor transformations. Exp brain Res. 2005;164: 250–259. pmid:15947919
  38. 38. Baraduc P, Wolpert DM. Adaptation to a Visuomotor Shift Depends on the Starting Posture. J Neurophysiol. 2002;88: 973–981. pmid:12163546
  39. 39. Buch ER, Young S, Contreras-Vidal JL. Visuomotor adaptation in normal aging. Learn {&} Mem (Cold Spring Harb NY). 2003;10: 55–63. pmid:12551964
  40. 40. Wong AL, Shelhamer M. Saccade adaptation improves in response to a gradually introduced stimulus perturbation. Neurosci Lett. 2011;500: 207–211. pmid:21741440
  41. 41. Ingram HA, van Donkelaar P, Cole J, Vercher JL, Gauthier GM, Miall RC. The role of proprioception and attention in a visuomotor adaptation task. Exp brain Res. 2000;132: 114–126. pmid:10836641
  42. 42. Kagerer FA, Contreras-Vidal JL, Stelmach GE. Adaptation to gradual as compared with sudden visuo-motor distortions. Exp brain Res. 1997;115: 557–561. pmid:9262212
  43. 43. Werner S, Schorn CF, Bock O, Theysohn N, Timmann D. Neural correlates of adaptation to gradual and to sudden visuomotor distortions in humans. Exp Brain Res. Springer Verlag; 2014;232: 1145–1156.
  44. 44. Morehead JR, Qasim SE, Crossley MJ, Ivry R. Savings upon Re-Aiming in Visuomotor Adaptation. J Neurosci. 2015;35: 14386–96. pmid:26490874
  45. 45. Taylor JA, Ivry RB. Flexible cognitive strategies during motor learning. PLoS Comput Biol. 2011;7: e1001096. pmid:21390266
  46. 46. Benson BL, Anguera JA, Seidler RD. A spatial explicit strategy reduces error but interferes with sensorimotor adaptation. J Neurophysiol. 2011;105: 2843–2851. pmid:21451054
  47. 47. Bond KM, Taylor JA. Flexible explicit but rigid implicit learning in a visuomotor adaptation task. J Neurophysiol. 2015;113: 3836–3849. pmid:25855690
  48. 48. Wang J, Lei Y, Binder JR. Performing a reaching task with one arm while adapting to a visuomotor rotation with the other can lead to complete transfer of motor learning across the arms. J Neurophysiol. 2015;113: 2302–2308. pmid:25632082
  49. 49. Clower DM, Hoffman JM, Votaw JR, Faber TL, Woods RP, Alexander GE. Role of posterior parietal cortex in the recalibration of visually guided reaching. Nature. 1996;383: 618–21. pmid:8857536
  50. 50. Anguera JA, Russell CA, Noll DC, Seidler RD. Neural correlates associated with intermanual transfer of sensorimotor adaptation. Brain Res. 2007;1185: 136–151. pmid:17996854
  51. 51. Inoue M, Uchimura M, Karibe A, O’Shea J, Rossetti Y, Kitazawa S. Three timescales in prism adaptation. J Neurophysiol. 2015;113: 328–38. pmid:25298383
  52. 52. Anguera JA, Reuter-Lorenz PA, Willingham DT, Seidler RD. Contributions of Spatial Working Memory to Visuomotor Learning. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010;22: 1917–1930. pmid:19803691
  53. 53. Morton SM, Bastian AJ. Prism adaptation during walking generalizes to reaching and requires the cerebellum. J Neurophysiol. 2004;92: 2497–509. pmid:15190088
  54. 54. Smith MA, Shadmehr R. Intact ability to learn internal models of arm dynamics in Huntington’s disease but not cerebellar degeneration. J Neurophysiol. 2005;93: 2809–21. pmid:15625094
  55. 55. Werner S, Bock O, Gizewski ER, Schoch B, Timmann D. Visuomotor adaptive improvement and aftereffects are impaired differentially following cerebellar lesions in SCA and PICA territory. Exp Brain Res. 2010;201: 429–439. pmid:19885654
  56. 56. Donchin O, Rabe K, Diedrichsen J, Lally N, Schoch B, Gizewski ER, et al. Cerebellar regions involved in adaptation to force field and visuomotor perturbation. J Neurophysiol. 2012;107: 134–47. pmid:21975446
  57. 57. Gibo TL, Criscimagna-Hemminger SE, Okamura AM, Bastian AJ. Cerebellar motor learning: are environment dynamics more important than error size? J Neurophysiol. 2013;110: 322–33. pmid:23596337
  58. 58. Schlerf JE, Xu J, Klemfuss NM, Griffiths TL, Ivry RB. Individuals with cerebellar degeneration show similar adaptation deficits with large and small visuomotor errors. J Neurophysiol. 2013;109: 1164–73. pmid:23197450