Dement Neurocogn Disord. 2024 Apr;23(2):107-114. English.
Published online Apr 12, 2024.
© 2024 Korean Dementia Association
Original Article

Gender Differences in Items of the Instrumental Activities of Daily Living in Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease Dementia

Hui Jin Ryu,1 and Yeonsil Moon1,2
    • 1Department of Neurology, Konkuk University Medical Center, Seoul, Korea.
    • 2Research Institute of Medical Science, Konkuk University School of Medicine, Seoul, Korea.
Received January 26, 2024; Revised March 12, 2024; Accepted March 26, 2024.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/) which permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

Background and Purpose

Each item in the instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) questionnaire has differential importance to an individual’s life functioning based on gender. However, IADL has mostly been utilized for its total score alone, without gender specificity. We identify the impact of each item on the transition from amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) to Alzheimer’s disease dementia (ADD), and determine if the impact of each item differs by gender.

Methods

Subjects were aMCI or ADD with a global clinical dementia rating of 0.5 or 1. The sample size was 146 men and 154 women. We used logistic regression analysis to determine the effect of each item of IADL on the transition from aMCI to ADD.

Results

The odds ratio (OR) for “remembering recent events” had similar values: 27.2 for men, and 27.7 for women. Gender difference was identified in the item with the highest OR value. For women, the “using transportation” item was 63.3, and for men, “conducting financial affairs” was overwhelmingly high at 89.1.

Conclusions

Functional decline on items with relatively higher ORs may indicate higher probability of a transition from aMCI to ADD. The OR of “conducting financial affairs” was relatively higher for both genders. In terms of gender differences, “conducting home repair” for men, and “using transportation” for women, have relatively higher impact. This study demonstrates that during the transition from aMCI to ADD, each item of IADL shows a staggered decline in functioning, and that this decline is gender-specific.

Keywords
Neurocognitive Disorders; Dementia; Sex Factors; Mild Cognitive Impairment; Activities of Daily Living

INTRODUCTION

The activities of daily living (ADL) is a concept that encompasses the fundamental skills needed for independent personal management at both basic and complex levels. In the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, significant impairment in ADL independence is one of the key criteria for the diagnosis of dementia.1 The ADL can be categorized into the basic ADL (BADL), such as bathing, dressing, and toileting, and the instrumental ADL (IADL), which are more complex activities, such as managing finances, and engaging in hobbies.2, 3, 4

While most items of the BADL are primarily related to self-care abilities and are rarely affected by gender, some items of the IADL are based on the social and cultural roles of the individual, and it is difficult to exclude the influence of gender. As one of the traditional IADL questionnaires, the early version of the Lawton IADL,4 the items “food preparation”, “housekeeping”, and “laundry” were designed for women subjects only. The Korean version of the Lawton IADL5 was designed so that men would also respond to these items. On the other hand, the Korean IADL (K-IADL),6, 7 which was developed by modifying the Lawton IADL to suit the Korean context, is designed to allow both men and women to respond to all items, while adding an “irrelevant” option for each item to reflect individual differences, including gender-related roles. Selecting the “irrelevant” option based on individual characteristics means that the weight of each item in the total score will differ from individual to individual. In addition, if a particular item has a significantly lower response by gender due to the functional roles of men and women in a culture, it may be necessary to make adjustments to the question based on gender. Therefore, it is important to look closely at the degree of response to the question by gender.

On the other hand, the IADL addresses functional skills that are based on more complex cognitive abilities, compared to the BADL. Maintaining the independence of the IADL in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) often requires greater effort or compensatory strategies.1 The Petersen criteria for MCI states that all subtypes of clinical MCI must not meet the criteria for dementia, but that mild impairment in functional activities is possible.8 Some studies have shown that a mild decline in the IADL is also seen in the MCI stage.9, 10 One paper comparing normal control and MCI showed group differences in conversation, household activities, medication, social functioning, telephone, and organization.11 The finding that the IADL can show deterioration as early as MCI suggests that the IADL may provide useful information in identifying early dementia.12 Therefore, in clinical practice, the IADL has been utilized for diagnosing early dementia, clinical follow-up, and treatment evaluation.

While the overall score is important in assessing an individual’s IADL, the individual items of the IADL are also important. Furthermore, each IADL item may have different importance for practical interventions to improve a patient’s life functioning. Previous studies have reported that among IADL items, telephone use, use of transport, medication intake, and financial management are strong predictors of developing dementia.13, 14, 15 A related study found that among Telephone, Transportation, Medications, and Finances, Finances had the highest odds ratio (OR) for predicting dementia 10 years later.16

On the other hand, when it comes to the gendered nature of IADL, most existing studies have analyzed IADL without distinguishing between genders. Research on the gender-specificity of IADL items is rare, and findings on the risk of dementia by gender in IADL are inconsistent. While some studies have shown that IADL and dementia risk do not differ by gender,17 others have reported that poor IADL functioning is associated with increased dementia risk in men, but not in women.18 These studies analyzed gender differences in IADL, but they are limited, in that they do not report the gender-specific characteristics of the individual IADL.

In this study, we aimed to find out how amnestic MCI (aMCI) and Alzheimer’s disease dementia (ADD) differ based on gender in the IADL items, as follows: first, we determine if there is a difference between men and women in the percentage checked as unrelated for each item. Second, for each item, we determine if there is a gender difference in the optimal cut-off score that separates ADD from aMCI. Lastly, we identify the impact of each item on the transition from aMCI to ADD, and compare them to determine if the impact of the items differs by gender.

METHODS

Study participants

Subjects were patients diagnosed with aMCI or ADD with a global clinical dementia rating (CDR) of 0.5 or 1 who visited the Department of Neurology at Konkuk University Medical Center between October 2021 and July 2023, and who underwent neuropsychological evaluation. The clinical assessment for dementia diagnosis was made by a neurologist, while the Korean version of the Mini Mental State Examination, 2nd edition (K-MMSE-2)19, 20 and the CDR21 were administered by a neuropsychologist. The final sample size, excluding those with no education, was 300, of which 146 were men, and 154 were women. For the clinical diagnosis of aMCI, the Petersen criteria8 were used. The clinical diagnosis for ADD was based on the criteria of the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association workgroups, published in 2011.22

Materials

The K-IADL was used to evaluate the IADL. The K-IADL was developed and standardized in 2002, and re-standardized in 2018.6, 7 This is an 11-item caregiver-reported questionnaire. The 11 items consist of the following topics: 1. shopping; 2. using transportation; 3. conducting financial affairs; 4. housekeeping; 5. preparing food; 6. using the telephone; 7. taking medicine; 8. remembering recent events; 9. enjoying hobbies; 10. watching TV; 11. conducting home repair. Each item has five choices: “possible alone” (0 point), “need some help” (1 point), “need a lot of help” (2 points), “impossible alone” (3 points), “irrelevant” (exclude). The K-IADL score is obtained by dividing the sum of the item scores by the number of items assessed, excluding those rated as irrelevant. Therefore, the score ranges (0–3). In analyzing each item, irrelevant rates were excluded from the final analysis. In addition to the K-IADL, the K-MMSE-2 was administered for the assessment of general cognition, and the CDR and CDR-sum of boxes (CDR-SB) were performed for dementia severity evaluation.

Procedures

During the detailed neuropsychological assessment of patients, the K-IADL was provided along with other questionnaires to be filled out by caregivers. Since the study is data-gathering research, the review exemption was approved by the Konkuk University Medical Center Institutional Review Board (IRB No. KUMC 2024-01-052).

Statistical analysis

To examine demographic differences in the gender group, Student’s t-test was used for age, education, K-MMSE-2, CDR, CDR-SB, and K-IADL. Chi-square analysis was used for gender comparison of the proportion of “irrelevant” responses for each item. For each item, receiver operating characteristic curve analysis was performed to determine the optimal cut-off score distinguishing ADD from aMCI, and optimal cutoff scores are presented. The effects of individual K-IADL items were analyzed based on gender, using the logistic regression method for the aMCI and ADD groups. The diagnosis group was set as the dependent variable, and the score of each item categorizing it into two based on the optimal cut-off score was set as an independent variable. Age and education variables were included as covariates. In the analysis, a p-value<0.05 was considered statistically significant. The data were analyzed using IBM SPSS 25.0 statistical software (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA).

RESULTS

In the analysis of differences by gender group, there were no significant differences in age, CDR, CDR-SB, and K-IADL score, but there were significant differences in education and K-MMSE-2 score (Table 1). The CDRs for the aMCI group were all 0.5, while those for the ADD group were either 0.5 or 1.

Table 1
Demographic characteristics of the participants

When we analyzed the “irrelevant” response for each item, men had significantly higher percentages of “irrelevant” responses than women for four items: “shopping”, “conducting financial affairs”, “housekeeping”, and “preparing food”. Women had a significantly higher percentage of “conducting home repair” responses than men (Table 2). We found that almost all items had less than 50% “irrelevant” responses, except for “preparing food” and “conducting home repair”. Items “using the telephone”, “taking medicine”, “remembering recent events”, and “watching TV” had less than 2% “irrelevant” responses for both men and women subjects. Items “using transportation” and “enjoying hobbies” had less than 10%. In particular, the item “remembering recent events” had zero “irrelevant” responses across all participants. However, in both diagnostic groups, “food preparation” for men and “home repair” for women had less than 80% “irrelevant” rate.

Table 2
Gender-specific “irrelevant” choice percentage per item

The optimal cut-off score distinguishing between aMCI and ADD was determined by maximizing both the sensitivity and specificity for each item (Table 3). The optimal cut-off score was found to be 1 for almost all items in both men and women, while 2 for the item “recent memory” in both genders. Additionally, the optimal cut-off of the total K-IADL score for men was 0.52 (area under the curve [AUC], 0.964; sensitivity, 0.923; specificity, 0.911), while for women, it was 0.58 (AUC, 0.975; sensitivity, 0.942; specificity, 0.911).

When logistic regression was performed for each gender, the ORs of all items were significant, and the influence of each item differed between men and women (Table 4). In common for men and women, “shopping” had a relatively lower OR, while “remembering recent events” and “conducting financial affairs” had a relatively higher OR. The OR for the item “remembering recent events” had similar values: 27.2 for men, and 27.7 for women. In terms of differences between men and women, the lowest ORs were “shopping” for men, and “taking medicine” for women, respectively. In particular, gender difference was identified in the item with the highest OR value. For women, the “using transportation” item was 63.3, and for men, “conducting financial affairs” was overwhelmingly high at 89.1.

Table 4
Gender comparison of the impact of individual K-IADL items on diagnosis

DISCUSSION

Regarding “irrelevant” choices for men and women, in the K-IADL development paper, only the “preparing food” item was the highest at 75.0% only for men, and the rest of the items did not exceed 35% for both men and women.6 In contrast, in this study, “preparing food” for men and “conducting home repair” for women had more than 80% “irrelevant” choices. Compared to a study 20 years ago, we believe that the social environment has changed, such that older women are significantly less likely to do home repairs themselves. The existence of these distinct gender-specific “irrelevant” items has the effect of making them act as gender-specific substitutes. On the other hand, when an activity is performed on an “irrelevant” item of the opposite gender, the weight of each item changes in calculating the total K-IADL score. If gender-specific “irrelevant” items are evident in future large-scale studies, as in this study, it may be necessary to separate the K-IADL items by gender in the future.

For both men and women, unlike the other items, the optimal cutoff score for the item “remembering recent events” was 2, indicating some level of impairment on this item already at the aMCI stage. The converse is that IADL items other than memory-related IADL are generally less likely to show functional decline in the aMCI stage. Thus, for individuals with AD-related MCI or dementia, non-memory IADL problems may indicate more severe functional decline than memory-related IADL, suggesting a more rapid transition to dementia.

To the best of our knowledge, there have been no studies on how individual IADL items differentially affect the development of dementia by gender. In the present study, we aimed to determine whether the effect of each item of the K-IADL on the transition from aMCI to ADD, based on the cut-off separating aMCI from ADD, differs by gender. We used the relative magnitudes of the ORs obtained through logistic regression to determine each item’s influence. The occurrence of functional decline on items with relatively higher ORs may indicate higher probability of a transition from aMCI to ADD. For men, the ORs for “conducting financial affairs” and “conducting home repair” were about 1.4 and 3.3 times higher, respectively, than for “remembering recent events”; for women, the ORs for “conducting financial affairs” and “using transportation” were about 2.1 and 2.3 times higher, respectively, than for “remembering recent events”. Also, the highest OR (“conducting financial affairs” for men, “using transportation” for women) was about 7.0 times that of the lowest (“shopping” for men, “taking medicine” for women), which was similar for men and women. The item “conducting financial affairs” had a relatively higher impact for both men and women. In terms of gender differences, the items “conducting home repair” for men and “using transportation” for women can be seen to have a relatively higher impact. In the end, for men, the most influential IADL item is “conducting financial affairs” with an OR of about 90, while for women, both “using transportation” and “conducting financial affairs” need to be emphasized at the same time. On the other hand, functional decline on items with relatively lower ORs may suggest a mix of the aMCI and ADD stages. For both men and women, the item “shopping” falls into this category. The items “watching TV” for men and “taking medicine” for women were relatively lower. These results provide gender-specific information about which IADL items to selectively observe and work to maintain focus on at each stage of cognitive decline.

Based on the above results, we can visualize how K-IADL items typically affect the aMCI and ADD stage (Fig. 1). Already in the aMCI phase, a degradation of the “remembering recent events” function is observed. In the transition to the ADD stage, functional decline is observed in “shopping” and “watching TV” for men, and “taking medicine” and “shopping” for women. Declines in “conducting home repair” and “conducting financial affairs” for men and “conducting financial affairs” and “using transportation” for women may indicate an almost certain transition to dementia. Given that men are generally more likely to be responsible for home repairs than women, and that older women are more likely to be socially active than older men, the results of this study are a relatively good reflection of everyday life for men and women in general. This suggests that when diagnosing dementia for an individual patient, it is important to pay attention to which IADL items are important to the individual’s daily life. We believe that such a diagram has the advantage of making caregivers and the patients in the early stages of decline aware of functions that should be clearly observed.

Fig. 1
Gender-specific flow of symptom reporting for key K-IADL items.
aMCI: amnestic mild cognitive impairment, ADD: Alzheimer’s disease dementia, K-IADL: Korean-instrumental activities of daily living.

There are some limitations to this study. First, the number of subjects in the study is rather small to generalize about gender differences. Second, the geographic diversity of the subjects included in the study should be ensured to generalize the differences between genders. This study is limited by the fact that it only includes patients from a single hospital. Third, further research is needed to see if similar results are found for dementia groups other than ADD. Lastly, this study categorized gender by biological sex. With the recent individualization of gender roles, the importance of social gender as a gender role in addition to biological distinction is becoming more apparent. Therefore, we believe that social gender needs to be considered in IADL analysis in the future.

This study demonstrates that during the transition from aMCI to ADD, each item of the IADL shows a staggered decline in functioning, and that this decline is gender-specific. The similarities and differences between men and women identified in this study can be used as a useful resource for gender-specific approaches to diagnosing and preventing dementia, identifying the extent of functional decline, educating caregivers, and rehabilitation in various settings.

Notes

Conflict of Interest:The authors have no financial conflicts of interest.

Author Contributions:

  • Conceptualization: Ryu HJ, Moon Y.

  • Writing - original draft: Ryu HJ.

  • Writing - review & editing: Ryu HJ, Moon Y.

References

    1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed (DSM-5). Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.
    1. Katz S. Assessing self-maintenance: activities of daily living, mobility, and instrumental activities of daily living. J Am Geriatr Soc 1983;31:721–727.
    1. Hindmarch I, Lehfeld H, de Jongh P, Erzigkeit H. The Bayer activities of daily living scale (B-ADL). Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 1998;9 Suppl 2:20–26.
    1. Lawton MP, Brody EM. Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist 1969;9:179–186.
    1. Kim SY, Won JW, Cho KW. The validity and reliability of Korean version of Lawton IADL index. J Korean Geriatr Soc 2005;9:23–29.
    1. Kang SJ, Choi SH, Lee BH, Kwon JC, Na DL, Han SH, et al. The reliability and validity of the Korean instrumental activities of daily living (K-IADL). J Korean Neurol Assoc 2002;20:8–14.
    1. Chin J, Park J, Yang SJ, Yeom J, Ahn Y, Baek MJ, et al. Re-standardization of the Korean-instrumental activities of daily living (K-IADL): clinical usefulness for various neurodegenerative diseases. Dement Neurocogn Disord 2018;17:11–22.
    1. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med 2004;256:183–194.
    1. Cloutier S, Chertkow H, Kergoat MJ, Gélinas I, Gauthier S, Belleville S. Trajectories of decline on instrumental activities of daily living prior to dementia in persons with mild cognitive impairment. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2021;36:314–323.
    1. Wadley VG, Okonkwo O, Crowe M, Ross-Meadows LA. Mild cognitive impairment and everyday function: evidence of reduced speed in performing instrumental activities of daily living. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2008;16:416–424.
    1. Schmitter-Edgecombe M, Woo E, Greeley DR. Characterizing multiple memory deficits and their relation to everyday functioning in individuals with mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology 2009;23:168–177.
    1. Barberger-Gateau P, Fabrigoule C, Helmer C, Rouch I, Dartigues JF. Functional impairment in instrumental activities of daily living: an early clinical sign of dementia? J Am Geriatr Soc 1999;47:456–462.
    1. Barberger-Gateau P, Dartigues JF, Letenneur L. Four instrumental activities of daily living score as a predictor of one-year incident dementia. Age Ageing 1993;22:457–463.
    1. Barberger-Gateau P, Fabrigoule C, Rouch I, Letenneur L, Dartigues JF. Neuropsychological correlates of self-reported performance in instrumental activities of daily living and prediction of dementia. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 1999;54:293–303.
    1. Gold DA. An examination of instrumental activities of daily living assessment in older adults and mild cognitive impairment. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2012;34:11–34.
    1. Pérès K, Helmer C, Amieva H, Orgogozo JM, Rouch I, Dartigues JF, et al. Natural history of decline in instrumental activities of daily living performance over the 10 years preceding the clinical diagnosis of dementia: a prospective population-based study. J Am Geriatr Soc 2008;56:37–44.
    1. Abdulrahman H, Richard E, van Gool WA, Moll van Charante EP, van Dalen JW. Sex differences in the relation between subjective memory complaints, impairments in instrumental activities of daily living, and risk of dementia. J Alzheimers Dis 2022;85:283–294.
    1. Pérès K, Helmer C, Amieva H, Matharan F, Carcaillon L, Jacqmin-Gadda H, et al. Gender differences in the prodromal signs of dementia: memory complaint and IADL-restriction. a prospective population-based cohort. J Alzheimers Dis 2011;27:39–47.
    1. Kang Y, Jahng S, Kim SY. Korean Dementia Association. In: Korean-Mini Mental State Examination. 2nd Edition (K-MMSE-2). Seoul: Hakjisa Publisher; 2020.
    1. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, White T, Messer MA. In: MMSE-2 User’s Manual. Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2010.
    1. Morris JC. The clinical dementia rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology 1993;43:2412–2414.
    1. McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, Jack CR Jr, Kawas CH, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 2011;7:263–269.

Metrics
Share
Figures

1 / 1

Tables

1 / 4

PERMALINK