Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-dfsvx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-29T04:12:23.882Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bird Brains and Aggro Apes: Questioning the Use of Animals in the Affect Program Theory of Emotion

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2022

Abstract

It is a common assumption amongst theorists that the phenomenon of animal emotion supports the affect program theory of emotion. I argue that this assumption is mistaken by exploring two cases of animal emotion from studies in ethology: aggression in chimpanzees and fear in piping plovers. While the affect program theory fails to account for the cognitive complexity involved in each case, I do not argue for a cognitive theory of emotion. Instead, I suggest that paying attention to animal emotions helps the emotion theorist avoid the dichotomy between the extreme versions of the affect program theory and cognitive theories.

Type
Philosophy of Psychology
Copyright
Copyright © The Philosophy of Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

My thanks to Bart Moffatt, Ben Schulz, Jessica Slind, Katie Plasiance, Ken Waters, Mark Borrello, Susan Hawthorne, and Toben Lafrancois for helpful comments on earlier drafts of this paper.

References

Aureli, Filippo, Preston, Stephanie D., and de Waal, Frans B. M. (1999), “Heart Rate Responses to Social Interactions in Free-moving Rhesus Macaques (Macaca Mulatta): A Pilot Study”, Heart Rate Responses to Social Interactions in Free-moving Rhesus Macaques (Macaca Mulatta): A Pilot Study 113:5965.Google ScholarPubMed
Chevalier-Skolnikoff, Suzanne (1973), “Facial Expression of Emotion in Nonhuman Primates”, in Ekman, Paul (ed.), Darwin and Facial Expressions. New York: Academic Press, 1189.Google Scholar
Damasio, Antonio R. (1994), Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. New York: Putnam.Google Scholar
Darwin, Charles ([1872] 1998), The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Deigh, John (2004), “Primitive Emotions”, in Solomon, Robert C. (ed.), Thinking about Feeling: Contemporary Philosophers on Emotions. New York: Oxford University Press, 927.Google Scholar
DeLancey, Craig (2002), Passionate Engines: What the Emotions Reveal about Mind and Artificial Intelligence. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Waal, Frans B. M. (1996), Good Natured: The Origins of Right and Wrong in Humans and Other Animals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, Irenaus (1973), “The Expressive Behavior of the Deaf-and-Blind-Born”, in Cranach, M. von and Vine, I. (eds.), Social Communication and Movement. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, Paul (1980), “Biological and Cultural Contributions to Body and Facial Movement in the Expression of Emotions”, in Rorty, Amelie O. (ed.), Explaining Emotions. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press, 3772.Google Scholar
Ekman, Paul (1992), “An Argument for Basic Emotions”, An Argument for Basic Emotions 6:169200.Google Scholar
Ekman, Paul (1998), “Universality of Emotional Expression? A Personal History of the Dispute”, in Ekman, P. (ed.), The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. New York: Oxford University Press, 363393.Google Scholar
Ekman, Paul (1999), “Basic Emotions”, in Dalgleish, T. and Power, T. (eds.), The Handbook of Cognition and Emotion. New York: Wiley, 4560.Google Scholar
Fodor, Jerry A. (1983), The Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, Paul E. (1997), What Emotions Really Are: The Problem of Psychological Categories. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, William (1884), “What is an Emotion?”, What is an Emotion? 9:188205.Google Scholar
Jarvis, Erich D., et al. (2005), “Avian Brains and a New Understanding of Vertebrate Evolution”, Avian Brains and a New Understanding of Vertebrate Evolution 6:151159.Google Scholar
Levenson, Robert W. (1992), “Autonomic Nervous System Differences Among Emotions”, Autonomic Nervous System Differences Among Emotions 3:2327.Google Scholar
Miller, R. E., Caul, W. F., and Mirsky, I. R. (1971), “Communication of Affects between Feral and Socially Isolated Monkeys”, Communication of Affects between Feral and Socially Isolated Monkeys 7:231–229.Google Scholar
Panksepp, Jaak (1998), Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Parr, Lisa, Preuschoft, Signe, and de Waal, Frans B. M. (2002), “Afterword: Research on Facial Emotion in Chimpanzees, 75 Years since Kohts”, in Ladygina-Kohts, N. N. and de Waal, Frans B. M. (eds.), Infant Chimpanzee and Human Child: A Classic 1935 Comparative Study of Ape Emotions and Intelligence. New York: Oxford University Press, 411446.Google Scholar
Preuschoft, Signe and van Hoof, Jan A. R. A. M. (1995), “Homologizing Primate Facial Displays: A Critical Review of Methods”, Homologizing Primate Facial Displays: A Critical Review of Methods 65:121137.Google ScholarPubMed
Prinz, Jesse (2004), A Perceptual Theory of Emotion. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ristau, Carolyn A. (1991), “Aspects of the Cognitive Ethology of an Injury-Feigning Bird, the Piping Plover”, in Ristau, Carolyn A. (ed.), Cognitive Ethology: The Minds of Other Animals. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 91126.Google Scholar
Russell, Connie L., Bard, Kim A., and Adamson, Lauren B. (1997), “Social Referencing by Young Chimpanzees (Pan Troglodytes)”, Social Referencing by Young Chimpanzees (Pan Troglodytes) 111:185191.Google Scholar