Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ttngx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-11T16:37:43.033Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Profiles of language and communication abilities in adolescents with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 November 2022

Lauren D. Poth
Affiliation:
Center for Behavioral Teratology and Department of Psychology, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, USA
Tracy Love
Affiliation:
School of Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, USA
Sarah N. Mattson*
Affiliation:
Center for Behavioral Teratology and Department of Psychology, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA, USA
*
Corresponding author: Sarah N. Mattson, email: sarah.mattson@sdsu.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Language and communication are largely understudied among youth with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders (FASD). Findings have been mixed, and have generally focused on more severely affected (i.e., children with FAS alone) or younger children. This study aimed to elucidate the profiles of language (i.e., receptive, expressive, general language) and communication (i.e., functional, social) abilities in adolescents with FASD.

Method:

Participants aged 12–17 years with (AE = 31) and without (CON = 29) prenatal alcohol exposure were included. Receptive and expressive language were measured by the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals – Fifth Edition (CELF-5). Parents or caregivers completed the Children’s Communication Checklist – Second Edition as a subjective measure of general language skills. Functional communication was measured by the Student Functional Assessment of Verbal Reasoning and Executive Strategies and parents or caregivers completed the Social Skills Improvement System Rating Scales as a measure of social communication. Multivariate analysis of variance determined the overall profiles of language and communication and whether they differed between groups.

Results:

The AE group performed significantly lower than the CON group on receptive language and parent report of general language while groups did not significantly differ on expressive language. Groups did not significantly differ on functional communication while social communication was significantly lower in the AE group.

Conclusions:

Results of this study provide important information regarding the overall profile of basic language abilities and higher-level communication skills of adolescents with FASD. Ultimately, improving communication skills of youth with FASD may translate to better overall functioning.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © INS. Published by Cambridge University Press, 2022

Introduction

Extensive research has investigated the impact of prenatal alcohol exposure (PAE) on several domains of neurobehavioral functioning (Mattson, Bernes, & Doyle, Reference Mattson, Bernes and Doyle2019; Mattson, Crocker, & Nguyen, Reference Mattson, Crocker and Nguyen2011; Mattson & Riley, Reference Mattson and Riley1998; Riley, Infante, & Warren, Reference Riley, Infante and Warren2011). However, investigations into the effects of PAE on language (i.e., receptive and expressive abilities) and communication (i.e., social and functional exchange of information) have been limited despite the clear clinical and functional relevance of such abilities to individuals with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders (FASD). Prevalence estimates of language-related disorders (i.e., expressive language disorder, receptive language disorder, developmental disorder of speech and language) among youth with fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS) alone are significant, ranging from 67.2% to 81.8% (Popova et al., Reference Popova, Lange, Shield, Mihic, Chudley, Mukherjee, Bekmuradov and Rehm2016); thus, the likelihood of elevated rates of language-related disorders are likely to extend to youth across the full spectrum of FASD.

Those studies that have been conducted to explore the effects of PAE on language and communication have primarily focused on children with FAS. Studies show delays in language acquisition (Church & Kaltenbach, Reference Church and Kaltenbach1997) as well as verbal learning and memory deficits in children with FAS (Mattson, Riley, Delis, Stern, & Jones, Reference Mattson, Riley, Delis, Stern and Jones1996) which can significantly impact overall language development. Additionally, children with FAS perform worse than their chronological age on language tests, making fewer grammatically correct and complete sentences (Akbarian, Reference Akbarian1992; Carney & Chermak, Reference Carney and Chermak1991). In another study, 80% of participants with FAS showed impairment on one or more measures of speech, language, voice, or fluency (Iosub, Fuchs, Bingol, & Gromisch, Reference Iosub, Fuchs, Bingol and Gromisch1981). These studies, while useful, were limited in sample size and most did not include controls for comparison.

While few studies have examined language and communication abilities in children with FASD more broadly, some common findings have been shown. Limited studies have shown that PAE disrupts development of language (Mattson & Riley, Reference Mattson and Riley1998) and neuroimaging findings suggest that individuals with FASD likely have alterations in brain areas involved with language (Gautam et al., Reference Gautam, Lebel, Narr, Mattson, May, Adnams, Riley, Jones, Kan and Sowell2015; Sowell et al., Reference Sowell, Johnson, Kan, Lu, Van Horn, Toga and Bookheimer2008; Sowell et al., Reference Sowell, Thompson, Peterson, Mattson, Welcome, Henkenius, Riley, Jernigan and Toga2002; Treit et al., Reference Treit, Lebel, Baugh, Rasmussen, Andrew and Beaulieu2013; Treit et al., Reference Treit, Zhou, Lebel, Rasmussen, Andrew and Beaulieu2014). Further, alcohol-exposed children have demonstrated impaired abilities in basic neuropsychological processes that contribute to language, such as attention and executive function (Kodituwakku & Kodituwakku, Reference Kodituwakku and Kodituwakku2014; Kodituwakku, Reference Kodituwakku2007; Mattson et al., Reference Mattson, Crocker and Nguyen2011; Riley et al., Reference Riley, Infante and Warren2011). Others have shown that children with PAE show deficits in general receptive and expressive language (Akbarian, Reference Akbarian1992; Carney & Chermak, Reference Carney and Chermak1991; Church, Eldis, Blakley, & Bawle, Reference Church, Eldis, Blakley and Bawle1997; Church & Kaltenbach, Reference Church and Kaltenbach1997; Gentry et al., Reference Gentry, Griffith, Dancer, Davis, Eaton and Schulz1998; McGee, Bjorkquist, Riley, & Mattson, Reference McGee, Bjorkquist, Riley and Mattson2009; Wyper & Rasmussen, Reference Wyper and Rasmussen2011). However, the degree of impairment for both aspects of language is not agreed upon and insufficient data exists to define the range of impairments in FASD. More broadly, studies have shown that children across the spectrum of FASD perform worse on narrative analysis (e.g., grammatical errors, semantic elaboration) as compared to typically developing controls (Thorne, Reference Thorne2017; Thorne et al., Reference Thorne, Coggins, Carmichael Olson and Astley2007). One study found a dissociation in language abilities whereby alcohol-exposed children performed better on tests of receptive than expressive language ability, though this difference did not reach statistical significance (McGee et al., Reference McGee, Bjorkquist, Riley and Mattson2009).

Another reason to study language and communication in FASD is the overlap and high rate of co-occurring disorders (e.g., attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), specific learning disorder, oppositional defiant disorder). For example, FASD and ADHD have similar neurobehavioral features and rates of ADHD in FASD are high (Burd et al., Reference Burd, Klug, Martsolf and Kerbeshian2003; Fryer et al., Reference Fryer, McGee, Matt and Mattson2007; Landgren et al., Reference Landgren, Svensson, Strömland and Grönlund2010; Mattson et al., Reference Mattson, Crocker and Nguyen2011; O’Connor & Paley, Reference O'Connor and Paley2009; Rasmussen et al., Reference Rasmussen, Benz, Pei, Andrew, Schuller, Abele-Webster, Alton and Lord2010). Children with ADHD have more language problems than typically developing controls (Sciberras et al., Reference Sciberras, Mueller, Efron, Bisset, Anderson, Schilpzand, Jongeling and Nicholson2014) and thus, it is reasonable to expect language and communication difficulties in individuals with FASD. Investigating language and communication skills of adolescents with FASD in comparison to a heterogeneous group will help clarify patterns and elucidate findings that may be specific to PAE.

Higher-level communication broadly encompasses the many skills (e.g., language, executive function, attention, perspective taking) necessary for the exchange of information; language is one of these skills. For the purpose of this study, social communication is conceptualized as communication used in social situations or when interacting with others while functional communication is communication used to get one’s needs or wants met. In terms of communication and socialization skills, young children with PAE and children with ADHD score lower on measures of adaptive functioning (i.e., socialization, communication, daily living skills) than typically developing controls. However, adolescents with PAE show greater impairment in these abilities than adolescents with ADHD, suggesting an arrest in development of communication and socialization skills in FASD, rather than the delay in development of these abilities seen in ADHD (Crocker et al., Reference Crocker, Vaurio, Riley and Mattson2009; Doyle et al., Reference Doyle, Moore, Coles, Kable, Sowell, Wozniak, Jones, Riley and Mattson2018). Further, children with FASD have social, but dysfunctional, communicative interactions (Akbarian, Reference Akbarian1992).

To be successful in communication, one must invoke language skills, social cognition, and executive function skills. As highlighted above, children with FASD show an array of deficits in these areas, but no clear pattern has emerged (Coggins et al., Reference Coggins, Timler and Olswang2007). During adolescence, academic and social demands are increased and difficulties emerge due to the requirement of independent functioning, decreased adult supervision, and increased peer pressure (Streissguth, Reference Streissguth and West1986). Furthermore, social deficits have been shown to persist across the lifespan of individuals with FASD, and may even worsen with age (Kully-Martens, Denys, Treit, Tamana, & Rasmussen, Reference Kully-Martens, Denys, Treit, Tamana and Rasmussen2012). As previously stated, most studies have either focused on young children with FASD or on individuals with FAS alone. Thus, the adolescent age range is of critical importance for studying language and communication abilities in this population.

Although the aforementioned differences in language and communication have been found, some findings are inconsistent (Flak, Bertrand, Denny, Kesmodel, & Cogswell, Reference Flak, Bertrand, Denny, Kesmodel and Cogswell2014). Some prospective studies have found that children with PAE are more likely to be diagnosed with a language delay (Kuehn et al., Reference Kuehn, Aros, Cassorla, Avaria, Unanue, Henriquez, Kleinsteuber, Conca, Avila, Carter, Conley, Troendle and Mills2012) while others have found no association between language skills and PAE (Davis, Gagnier, Moore, & Todorow, Reference Davis, Gagnier, Moore and Todorow2013). Further, Greene and colleagues (Greene, Ernhart, Martier, Sokol, & Ager, Reference Greene, Ernhart, Martier, Sokol and Ager1990) found little association between PAE and language development among alcohol-exposed individuals without FAS. Another study found that children with FASD performed significantly lower on the language composite of one neuropsychological battery (i.e., NEPSY-II), but did not perform worse on measures of receptive and expressive vocabulary (Nash et al., Reference Nash, Stevens, Rovet, Fantus, Nulman, Sorbara and Koren2013). In contrast, retrospective studies have rather consistently documented language deficits (i.e., grammatical, semantic, pragmatic) among alcohol-exposed youth (Akbarian, Reference Akbarian1992; Carney & Chermak, Reference Carney and Chermak1991; Crocker et al., Reference Crocker, Vaurio, Riley and Mattson2009; Iosub, Fuchs, Bingol, & Gromisch, Reference Iosub, Fuchs, Bingol and Gromisch1981).

Given the gap and noted discrepancies in current literature, this study aimed to examine language and communication abilities in adolescents with FASD. To this end, we sought to establish comprehensive profiles of strengths and weaknesses in language (i.e., receptive, expressive) and communication (i.e., social, functional) of adolescents with FASD and compare these profiles to controls. We hypothesized that: (1) adolescents with FASD will display overall impaired language ability as well as impaired performance on selected measures as compared to controls; specifically, adolescents with FASD will display poorer expressive than receptive language ability; and (2) adolescents with FASD will display impaired performance on measures of functional and social communication as compared to controls. Findings will provide clinically valuable information regarding the language and communication abilities in individuals with FASD and inform future development of interventions to improve communication and related functional deficits of adolescents with FASD.

Method

General methods

Participants (N = 60) were tested individually during a 2-hour testing session. All participants completed a brief hearing screening with a GSI audiometer for pure tone thresholds (20–30 dB, 1000–4000 Hz) at the start of testing to ensure intact hearing at the level of conversational speech. No participants were excluded due to hearing loss. Parents or caregivers completed questionnaires while the participant underwent testing. Informed consent was obtained from parents or caregivers, and informed assent was obtained from participants. Cognitive data (i.e., full scale IQ) were not collected as part of the current study but were available from concurrent, ongoing studies as part of the Collaborative Initiative on Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders, Phase Four (CIFASD-4) multisite study (Mattson et al., Reference Mattson, Foroud, Sowell, Jones, Coles, Fagerlund, Autti-Rämö, May, Adnams, Konovalova, Wetherill, Arenson, Barnett and Riley2010). Financial incentive was provided to caregivers and participants. The Institutional Review Board at San Diego State University approved study procedures. Research was completed in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration.

Participants

All participants were primary English speakers between the ages of 12:0–17:11. Participants were recruited as part of the CIFASD-4. Full scale IQ (FSIQ) was obtained through the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Fifth Edition (ages 12:0–16:11) or the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence – Second Edition (ages 17:0+). Adolescents from all ethnicities, races, and sexes were included in the study based on exclusion and inclusion criteria outlined below. Participants were evaluated for the presence of ADHD symptoms using the ADHD Rating Scale – 5 for Children and Adolescents (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; DuPaul, Power, Anastopoulos, & Reid, Reference DuPaul, Power, Anastopoulos and Reid2016). Testing was completed over three separate days with one of two examiners. All data was re-checked by one of the evaluators (who had not completed the testing) and a third party to ensure accurate data collection. In addition to neurobehavioral testing, all participants were examined by a dysmorphologist for the presence of FAS based upon CIFASD criteria (Jones et al., Reference Jones, Robinson, Bakhireva, Marintcheva, Storojev, Strahova, Sergeevskaya, Budantseva, Mattson, Riley and Chambers2006; Mattson et al., Reference Mattson, Foroud, Sowell, Jones, Coles, Fagerlund, Autti-Rämö, May, Adnams, Konovalova, Wetherill, Arenson, Barnett and Riley2010). Information regarding existing language or speech diagnoses was collected by parent report along with demographic data to determine whether such differences account for group differences in language and communication. In addition, information regarding parental socioeconomic status, including highest level of education and income, was collected.

Participants comprised two groups: PAE (AE; n = 31) and controls (CON; n = 29). Participants in the AE group had heavy PAE, defined as maternal intake of ≥4 drinks per occasion at least once per week, or >13 drinks per week (Jones et al., Reference Jones, Robinson, Bakhireva, Marintcheva, Storojev, Strahova, Sergeevskaya, Budantseva, Mattson, Riley and Chambers2006; Mattson et al., Reference Mattson, Foroud, Sowell, Jones, Coles, Fagerlund, Autti-Rämö, May, Adnams, Konovalova, Wetherill, Arenson, Barnett and Riley2010). Information on maternal alcohol use and other prenatal exposures was obtained through caregiver questionnaires and interview, if available. In cases where direct maternal report was not available, a review of medical, social services, or court records was completed. In these cases, participants were included in the AE group if there was documentation of alcohol abuse or dependence in the biological mother or if exposure was suspected and the child met criteria for FAS. Participants in the CON group were controls with minimal or no PAE and with or without other diagnosed or suspected clinical or behavioral concerns (e.g., ADHD, autism spectrum disorder, oppositionality, depression, anxiety) based on parent report. Minimal exposure is defined as <1 drink per week and never more than 2 drinks per occasion during pregnancy. Further, participants were excluded from the CON group if alcohol exposure information was unavailable or greater than minimal exposure was suspected. Per CIFASD criteria, participants were excluded from the study if they had a medical (e.g., uncorrected hearing or vision loss) or psychiatric illness (e.g., active psychotic episode) that precluded inclusion in the study, or serious head injury with loss of consciousness >30 min (no participants sustained a head injury with loss of consciousness). Individuals with another known cause of mental deficiency (e.g., chromosomal abnormality, neurofibromatosis) were excluded from participation. Information for participants with delayed language was obtained and sub-analyses on those who fall into this subgroup were performed. Demographic information for both groups can be found in Table 1.

Table 1. Demographic information for adolescents with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure (AE) and controls (CON)

Note: * p < .05 level, ADHD = attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder; FAS = fetal alcohol syndrome. FSIQ, an estimate of general intellectual ability, was measured using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Fifth Edition (WISC-V) for participants ages 12:0–16:11 and the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence– Second Edition (WASI-II) for participants ages 17:0+. Research criteria for ADHD was determined using the ADHD Rating Scale – 5 for Children and Adolescents. FAS was determined by presence of two of three key facial features (short palpebral fissures, smooth philtrum, thin vermillion) and either microcephaly or growth deficiency or both. Previous diagnoses (i.e., delayed speech/language, expressive language disorder, auditory processing disorder, specific learning disorder) were based on parent report.

Measures

The following measures were selected to obtain a comprehensive profile of language and communication abilities.

Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-Fifth Edition (CELF-5; Wiig, Semel, & Secord, Reference Wiig, Semel and Secord2013)

The CELF-5 is a comprehensive measure to assess language and communication disorders in children and adolescents. The CELF-5 is widely used within school systems and is recognized as a valid measure of language and communication abilities. The CELF-5 was standardized and normed on a sample of more than 4,500 individuals and demonstrates sound psychometric properties. The Receptive and Expressive Language Index standard scores (M = 100, SD = 15) were used in analyses with lower scores indicating weaker performance.

Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-Third Edition (GFTA-3; Goldman & Fristoe, Reference Goldman and Fristoe2015)

The GFTA-3, a measure of speech sound and articulation abilities, was used to obtain information regarding participants’ speech sound abilities to rule out speech- or articulation-level deficits. The child engages in both spontaneous and imitative sound production to measure the child’s speech abilities. The Sounds-in-Words standard score (M = 100, SD = 15) was used in analyses with lower scores indicating weaker performance.

Children’s Communication Checklist-Second eEdition (CCC-2; Bishop, Reference Bishop2006)

The CCC-2 is a parent- or caregiver-report questionnaire that addresses the child’s language (i.e., speech, syntax, semantics, and coherence) and communication abilities (i.e., initiation, scripted language, context, nonverbal communication, social relations, and interests). It is sensitive to deficits in language and pragmatic communication. As parent-reported and directly measured abilities often do not align among this population (e.g., Gross, Deling, Wozniak & Boys, Reference Gross, Deling, Wozniak and Boys2015; Nguyen et al., Reference Nguyen, Glass, Coles, Kable, May, Kalberg, Sowell, Jones, Riley and Mattson2014; Glass et al., Reference Glass, Graham, Deweese, Jones, Riley and Mattson2014; Rai et al., Reference Rai, Abecassis, Casey, Flaro, Erdodi and Roth2017; Mohamed et al., Reference Mohamed, Carlisle, Livesey and Mukherjee2019), inclusion of parent-reported general language abilities provides a comprehensive profile of overall language abilities and allows for examination of direct versus subjective measures. The General Communication Composite standard score (M = 100, SD = 15) was used in analyses as a measure of general language abilities. This measure is sensitive to general language deficits, with lower scores indicating weaker performance. Parents of participants over the age of 16:11 (n = 3) did not complete this measure due to being out of the normative age range.

Student Functional Assessment of Verbal Reasoning and Executive Strategies (S-FAVRES; MacDonald, Reference MacDonald2013)

The S-FAVRES is a measure of higher-order cognitive and communication skills and was employed to objectively determine the functional communication and interaction abilities of these individuals. Beneficially, this measure is sensitive to higher-order language and communication deficits that may emerge during adolescence and has been standardized on language unimpaired and impaired populations. Constructs tapped by the S-FAVRES include verbal reasoning, social communication, planning, problem solving, and meta-cognition. The S-FAVRES provides four composite normative scores across four tasks: Total Accuracy, Total Time, Total Rationale, Total Reasoning Subskills. The Total Reasoning Subskills standard score (M = 100, SD = 15) was included in analyses as a measure of functional communication with lower scores indicating weaker performance. This subtest requires participants to answer a series of questions regarding their thought processes in solving four real-world problems. As the test authors highlight, the Total Reasoning Subskills measure provides a means to examine reasoning strengths and weaknesses as it is not possible to understand internal reasoning processes in just evaluating time or accuracy in responding. Thus, it allows for an examination of functional communication skills (e.g., identifying facts, filtering out irrelevant facts, creating responses, thinking flexibly).

Social Skills Improvement System Rating Scales (SSIS; Gresham & Elliott, Reference Gresham and Elliott2008)

The SSIS is a parent- or caregiver-report questionnaire that assesses problem behaviors and social skills. The SSIS was selected to obtain a subjective report of an individual’s social communication and interaction abilities. Social communication requires many skills, including appropriate usage of language, executive function, and social cognition. The Social Skills subscale integrates information important for social functioning and interactions including communication, cooperation, assertion, responsibility, empathy, engagement, and self-control. Thus, the SSIS provides a more complete evaluation of one’s social communication abilities above and beyond fundamental language skills alone. The Social Skills standard score (M = 100, SD = 15) was included in analyses with lower scores indicating weaker abilities as rated by caregivers.

Statistical analyses

SPSS statistical software v.26 was used for analyses. Demographic data were analyzed with analysis of variance (ANOVA; for continuous variables) or chi-square (for categorical variables) statistical procedures. Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was used to address the primary hypotheses, as correlations among target dependent variables was expected. Assumptions of homogeneity of variance and covariance were examined with Box’s M and Levene’s test statistics with an alpha level of .001 used to evaluate homogeneity assumptions. An alpha level of .05 was used to determine statistical significance for other analyses and effect sizes were consulted to determine practical significance.

First, a MANOVA with group (AE, CON) as the between-subjects variable and language domain (Receptive, Expressive, Speech Sound, General Language) as the within-subject variable was conducted to determine the overall profile of language ability of adolescents with FASD and how this profile differs from controls. Paired samples t-tests investigated the relationship between receptive and expressive language abilities within each group. Next, a MANOVA with group (AE, CON) as the between-subjects factor and social/functional communication (SSIS, S-FAVRES) as the correlated outcome was conducted to examine differences in communication between AE and CON. Group differences on all neuropsychological variables were tested using independent samples t-tests.

Results

Demographic data

Groups did not significantly differ on handedness (p = .139), sex (p = .796), ethnicity (p = .135), race (p = .152), or age (p = .814). As expected, groups significantly differed on FSIQ (p = .049) and presence of ADHD (p < .001). Specifically, the AE group (M = 89.5; SD = 15.02) had significantly lower FSIQ scores than the CON group (M = 98.4; SD = 18.97) and had a significantly higher proportion of participants that met research criteria for ADHD (n = 24; 77%) than the CON group (n = 9; 32%). Due to statistical and methodological limitations discussed elsewhere (Dennis et al., Reference Dennis, Francis, Cirino, Schachar, Barnes and Fletcher2009), FSIQ was not considered as a covariate in subsequent analyses. However, correlation analyses conducted within each group revealed that FSIQ significantly correlated with expressive and receptive language (ps ≤ .001) in both groups. Group performance on all variables is presented in Table 2. Separate correlation analyses for measures used in the language MANOVA and communication MANOVA are presented in Table 3.

Table 2. Group performance on neuropsychological variables

Note: Groups included adolescents with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure (AE) and controls (CON). * p < .05 level, CELF-5 = Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals – Fifth Edition; GFTA-3 = Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation – Third Edition Sounds-In-Speech; S-FAVRES = Student Functional Assessment of Verbal Reasoning and Executive Strategies Total Reasoning Subskills; CCC-2 = Children’s Communication Checklist – Second Edition General Communication Scale; SIDI = Social Interaction Difference Index (SIDI) from the CCC-2; SSIS = Social Skills Improvement System Rating Scales Social Skills Scale; All variables were measured using standard scores. The SIDI is calculated from scaled scores on the CCC-2 with positive scores indicating greater non-pragmatic language impairment and negative scores indicating greater pragmatic and social impairments (scores between −10 and 10 are within the normative range). Number of participants with language impairment was determined by CELF-5 Core Language Index ≤ 1 SD below the mean or 2 or more core subtests ≤ 1 SD below the mean (Leonard, Reference Leonard1998).

Table 3. Correlations among MANOVA-dependent variables for separate language and communication analyses, respectively

Note: * p < .001 level. Receptive and Expressive Language were measured by the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals – Fifth Edition. Speech Sound abilities were measured by the Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation – Third Edition Sounds-In-Speech subtest. General Language was measured by parent report from the Children’s Communication Checklist – Second Edition General Communication Scale. Social Communication was measured by the Social Skills Improvement System Rating Scales Social Skills Scale. Functional Communication was measured by the Student Functional Assessment of Verbal Reasoning and Executive Strategies Total Reasoning Subskills.

Groups did not significantly differ on number of participants with delayed speech and language acquisition (p = .245). Of those participants with delayed speech and language acquisition, groups did not significantly differ on participants that received early intervention services (p = .665). In addition, groups did not significantly differ on number of participants with existing expressive language disorder (p = .185), auditory processing disorder (p = .156), or specific learning disorder (p = .693) diagnoses based on parent report. Groups also did not significantly differ on parental education (p = .738) or family income (p = .066). As such, these variables were not considered in the following analyses.

Language and communication data

A MANOVA with group (AE, CON) as the between-subjects variable and domain (Receptive, Expressive, Speech Sound, General Language) as the within-subject variable was conducted to determine the overall profile of language ability of adolescents with FASD and how this profile differs from controls. Results are presented in Table 4. Box’s M test of homogeneity of covariance (p < .001) was statistically significant and Levene’s homogeneity test (all ps > .026) was not statistically significant. Using Wilk’s criterion (Λ) as the omnibus test statistic, the combined dependent variables resulted in a significant main effect of group [F(4, 48) =3.135, p = .023, partial η2 = .207].

Table 4. MANOVA results for language profile by group

Note: Groups included adolescents with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure (AE) and controls (CON). * p < .05 level, df = degrees of freedom. Standard scores from each measure were included in analyses. Receptive Language and Expressive Language were measured by the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals – Fifth Edition. Speech Sound abilities were measured by the Sounds-In-Words subtest from Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation – Third Edition. General Language was measured by parent report from the General Communication Scale from the Children’s Communication Checklist – Second Edition.

To probe the statistically significant multivariate effects, univariate ANOVAs were conducted on each individual dependent variable (Receptive, Expressive, Speech Sound, General Language). Overall, the AE group performed below the CON group on all measures and significantly differed from the CON group on receptive language and parent-reported general language abilities (see Figure 1). Expressive and receptive language scores were not significantly different within the AE group (t(29) =−1.469, p = .153) or the CON group (t(28) = .146, p = .885). For receptive language, there was a significant main effect for group [F(1, 51) = 6.117, p = .017, partial η2 = .107] and receptive language scores were significantly higher for the CON group (M = 96.5) relative to the AE group (M = 87.8). For parent report of general language, there was a significant main effect for group [F(1, 51) = 10.365, p = .002, partial η2 = .169]. Parents rated the general language skills of the CON group (M = 98.7) higher relative to the AE group (M = 79.0). The main effect for group was not statistically significant for expressive language [F(1, 51) = 2.831, p = .099, partial η2 = .053] or speech sound abilities [F(1, 51) =1.386, p = .245, partial η2 = .026].

Figure 1. Profile of language abilities by group. Groups included adolescents with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure (AE) and controls (CON). Groups significantly differed on receptive language and parent report of general language.

Note: * p < .05 level. Receptive Language and Expressive Language were measured the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals – Fifth Edition. Speech Sound abilities were measured by the Sounds-In-Words subtest from Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation – Third Edition. General Language was measured by parent report from the General Communication Scale from the Children’s Communication Checklist – Second Edition.

A MANOVA with group (AE, CON) as the between-subjects factor and social/functional communication (SSIS, S-FAVRES) as the correlated outcome was conducted to examine differences in communication between AE and CON. Results are presented in Table 5. Box’s M test of homogeneity of covariance (p = .213) and Levene’s homogeneity test (all ps > .025) were not statistically significant. Using Wilk’s criterion (Λ) as the omnibus test statistic, the combined dependent variables resulted in a significant main effect for group [F(2, 54) = 4.5000, p = .016, partial η2 = .143].

Table 5. MANOVA results for communication profile by group

Note: Groups included adolescents with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure (AE) and controls (CON). * p < .05 level, df = degrees of freedom. Social Communication was measured by the Social Skills standard score from the Social Skills Improvement System Rating Scales completed by parents. Functional Communication was measured by the Total Reasoning Subskills standard score from the Student Functional Assessment of Verbal Reasoning and Executive Strategies test.

To probe the statistically significant multivariate effects, univariate ANOVAs were conducted on each individual dependent variable (SSIS, S-FAVRES). Overall, groups did not differ on functional communication performance [F(1, 55) = 2.374, p = .129, partial η2 = .041], but parents rated the social communication abilities of the AE group as significantly worse than the CON group [F(1, 55) = 9.167, p = .004, partial η2 = .143] (see Figure 2). Parents rated the CON group (M = 92.4) as having significantly higher (i.e., stronger) social communication abilities relative to the AE group (M = 77.1).

Figure 2. Profile of communication abilities by group. Groups included adolescents with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure (AE) and controls (CON). Groups significantly differed in social communication.

Note: * p < .05 level. Groups significantly differed on social communication. Functional Communication was measured by the Total Reasoning Subskills standard score from the Student Functional Assessment of Verbal Reasoning and Executive Strategies test. Social Communication was measured by the Social Skills standard score from the Social Skills Improvement System Rating Scales completed by parents.

Post hoc analyses

Follow-up analyses examined whether inclusion of a heterogeneous CON group influenced results, specifically the contribution of ADHD to results. Participants within the CON group that met research criteria for ADHD (n = 9) were excluded and analyses were re-examined. Results for the language MANOVA remained except the group difference in expressive language became significant [F(1, 44) = 5.140, p = .028, partial η2 = .105]. Expressive language scores were significantly higher (i.e., stronger) for the CON group (M = 100.2) relative to the AE group (M = 90.3). Results for the communication MANOVA remained except the group difference in functional communication became significant [F(1, 47) = 4.864, p = .032, partial η2 = .094]. Functional communication scores were significantly higher (i.e., stronger) for the CON group (M = 89.9) relative to the AE group (M = 79.0).

Discussion

Overall, this study aimed to identify the profiles of language (receptive, expressive, speech sound abilities, general language) and communication (functional, social) abilities among adolescents with heavy PAE and how these abilities differ from controls. In terms of language abilities, our hypotheses were partially supported as the AE group performed below the CON group on all measures examined (Receptive Language, Expressive Language, Speech Sound, General Language) although these differences were only statistically significant on receptive language and parent report of general language. Furthermore, receptive language did not significantly differ from expressive language abilities within either group. These findings differ from previous studies that showed significant differences in both receptive and expressive language between alcohol-exposed participants and controls as well as stronger receptive than expressive language skills (Akbarian, Reference Akbarian1992; Carney & Chermak, Reference Carney and Chermak1991; Church et. al, Reference Church, Eldis, Blakley and Bawle1997; Church & Kaltenbach, Reference Church and Kaltenbach1997; Gentry et al., Reference Gentry, Griffith, Dancer, Davis, Eaton and Schulz1998; McGee et al., Reference McGee, Bjorkquist, Riley and Mattson2009; Wyper & Rasmussen, Reference Wyper and Rasmussen2011).

The patterns of language performance in previous studies and the current study suggest interesting developmental trends although longitudinal studies are needed to confirm cross-sectional observations. The lack of group differences on expressive language in the current study may be attributable to the inclusion of a heterogeneous comparison group as follow-up analyses showed that in removing the CON participants who met research criteria for ADHD, groups significantly differed on expressive language abilities as well, though there were still no within-group differences between expressive and receptive language. That is, differences in expressive language abilities between groups may be attributed to factors other than PAE. Furthermore, the current study investigated these abilities among adolescents whereas previous studies have focused on younger children (e.g., 3–9 years of age) with PAE. Longitudinal studies among non-exposed individuals with developmental language disorders show a pattern of impaired but stable language abilities over time (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Beitchman, Young, Escobar, Atkinson, Wilson, Brownlie, Douglas, Taback, Lam and Wang1999) although certain skills appear to plateau as individuals continue to fall behind as compared to their peers (Stothard et al., Reference Stothard, Snowling, Bishop, Chipchase and Kaplan1998). Taken together, results from the current study and those from previous studies highlight the need for longitudinal studies among individuals with PAE to clarify the developmental trajectories of these important language skills to help clarify noted inconsistencies in younger children with FASD.

In addition to the differences in statistical significance, examination of effect sizes (see Table 4) suggests that receptive language (partial η2 = .107) is more strongly related to group membership than expressive language (partial η2 = .053) highlighting the importance of receptive language abilities among adolescents with PAE. Notably, the prognosis for receptive language disorder is worse than that for expressive language disorder and early and ongoing intervention is key in addressing these difficulties (Clark et al., Reference Clark, O’Hare, Watson, Cohen, Cowie, Elton and Seckl2007). Results demonstrate that language and communication difficulties remain in adolescence suggesting that individuals with PAE do not “grow out” of these deficits. In addition, individuals with a pure language disorder in the absence of speech or articulation impairments are at a high risk of developing psychiatric illness (Prizant et al., Reference Prizant, Audet, Burke, Hummel, Maher and Theadore1990) likely due to these underlying difficulties not being identified or intervened upon. Findings from the current study show intact speech sound abilities among adolescents with PAE highlighting the risk of these individuals developing psychiatric or functional issues due to underlying language disorders and potential for these language disorders to go unidentified.

The clinical implications of language disorders are clear. Language disorders can impact long-term functioning in terms of academic functioning, occupational functioning, and socialization skills (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Among individuals without a history of PAE, early language disorders predict significant social maladaptation in later life as well as increased risk for psychiatric disorders (Clegg et al., Reference Clegg, Hollis, Mawhood and Rutter2005). Current results highlight the importance of early identification of language difficulties among youth with PAE to provide intervention as early as possible to improve deficits still evident in adolescence. Future research will necessarily benefit from focusing on the long-term implications for mental health among adolescents with FASD and to what extent language and communication deficits contribute to poor mental health outcomes later in life.

Given the implications for impaired receptive language, findings suggest interventions targeted at improving receptive language abilities may be most beneficial in ameliorating language deficits among youth with PAE given the statistical significance and effect size of receptive language on group membership. Among other neurodevelopmental disorders, limited research has investigated the responsiveness of language disorders to targeted intervention and findings suggest minimal improvement in receptive language abilities although earlier and longer term (i.e., more than 8 weeks) therapy provided better clinical outcomes (Law, Garrett, & Nye, Reference Law, Garrett and Nye2004). As such, current results highlight the importance of early identification of language difficulties among youth with PAE to provide intervention as early as possible to improve deficits still evident in adolescence. It will be important to elucidate the underlying cognitive factors (e.g., working memory, attention) that contribute to observed receptive language difficulties to allow for targeted intervention and inform appropriate treatment.

Regarding communication abilities, our hypotheses were once again partially supported as groups did not significantly differ on a direct measure of functional communication (the S-FAVRES) but did significantly differ on a measure of social communication (SSIS). However, the average functional communication score of the AE group fell more than one standard deviation below average (M = 79.0) while the CON group was within the low end of the average range (M = 85.8) and parents of exposed youth reported difficulties in social communication on the SSIS. In addition, follow-up analyses that removed CON participants meeting research criteria for ADHD lead to an increase in functional communication scores in the CON group (M = 89.9) and the difference between functional communication scores of the AE and CON groups becoming significant. The current study may have been underpowered to detect a statistically significant difference in functional communication with a heterogeneous control group though findings may suggest other explanatory factors are contributing to communication differences. Indeed, it is important to acknowledge the widespread impact ADHD can have on other cognitive skills, such as executive function, that may be impacting on functional communication. However, follow-up analyses highlight the significant impairment in communication among adolescents with PAE. Importantly, the S-FAVRES was designed to discriminate typical or average performance from below average performance. Based on descriptors provided by test manufacturers, the AE group performed in the below average range while the CON group performed in the low average range (MacDonald, Reference MacDonald2013) highlighting the clinical significance of communication difficulties among the AE group. Social and functional communication abilities of the AE group were generally comparable and did not significantly differ.

Limitations/future directions

Findings from the present study should be considered within the context of several limitations. First of all, while several interesting results were shown, we were limited in our sample size which may have restricted statistically significant results. As such, we may have not had adequate statistical power to detect relations with smaller effect sizes. Given observed effect sizes ranging from .041 < partial η 2 < .169 for MANOVA, post hoc power analyses showed required sample sizes ranging from 69 to 270 in order to detect significant special effects of interest. Despite this limitation, several significant results were revealed, and effect size estimates provide additional information for potentially significant findings. Similarly, we were limited in the adolescent age range we were able to investigate (i.e., 12–17 years) based on normative age ranges for the chosen measures. Future studies should expand upon this age range and consider longitudinal examination to elucidate developmental trajectories of language and communication abilities.

Other possible limitations include the assessments chosen to measure study constructs. For example, the S-FAVRES was included as an objective measure of functional communication. While standardized on language impaired (i.e., adolescents with traumatic brain injury) and unimpaired participants (MacDonald, Reference MacDonald2013), it may be more sensitive to attention difficulties and effort as group differences were not evident with a heterogeneous control group. Despite these limitations, the AE group performed below the average range while the CON group was within the average range, particularly when removing participants meeting research criteria for ADHD. As highlighted above, the purpose of the S-FAVRES is to identify individuals with below average communication skills and results suggest possible use in clinical settings to delineate cognitive-communication difficulties experienced by adolescents with PAE. Nevertheless, future studies with larger sample sizes will help clarify this pattern. Concern may also exist in using parent-report questionnaires as parents or caregivers may be similarly impaired in language abilities. A reading level roughly equivalent to the fifth grade is required for parent-report questionnaires (Bishop, Reference Bishop2006; Gresham & Elliott, Reference Gresham and Elliott2008) and follow-up analyses showed that caregiver education level did not significantly predict CCC-2 or SSIS scores (ps ≥ .139). Therefore, it is unlikely that caregiver education levels significantly influenced results. However, inclusion of a direct measure of social communication may be helpful to clarify or validate parent-reported concerns. Along those lines, it is important to note that the AE group demonstrated comparable parental education and income to the control group suggesting that these individuals may not be experiencing psychosocial difficulties often noted in FASD. As such, adolescents with greater clinical complexities may demonstrate even more difficulties than current findings suggest.

Additional confounds that are inherent to PAE should be considered. Other psychiatric disorders are highly prevalent among individuals with PAE and may have contributed to our findings (e.g., ADHD, depression). Nonetheless, inclusion of a heterogeneous control group provides additional support for our findings above and beyond comorbid diagnoses. We also considered the contribution of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) to our findings as the AE group had significantly more (p = .004) participants with an existing ASD diagnosis (n = 10; 32.3%) than the CON group (n = 1; 3.4%) per parent report. However, we did not specifically recruit participants with an ASD diagnosis and did not verify the accuracy of the diagnosis. Exploratory analyses showed that within the AE group, participants with and without an existing ASD diagnosis, based on parent report, did not significantly differ on any language or communication variables (ps ≥ .087) from the larger group. Future studies should consider inclusion of an ASD comparison group to investigate these relations between language and communication and clarify the profile of abilities between youth with FASD and those with ASD. In addition, information regarding ongoing medication usage was not available, though all participants were asked to refrain from medication usage on the days of testing. However, potential cumulative effects due to medication usage cannot be excluded.

Other confounds include maternal use of other substances (e.g., cocaine, nicotine, marijuana) during pregnancy. Due to the retrospective nature of this study, specific information regarding smoking or other drug use is unknown and as such we cannot account for these potentially confounding variables. We require evidence of alcohol as the primary substance of exposure for inclusion in the alcohol-exposed group, though we cannot rule out the effects of other drugs of abuse. Future studies would benefit from investigating the potential contribution of other substances to patterns of language and communication impairment among these youth. Finally, differences may be explained by overall performance differences (e.g., IQ) between groups as the AE group performed below the CON group on most measures though the average FSIQ score for both groups fell in the average range. Given methodological and statistical limitations, we did not test FSIQ as a covariate (Dennis et al., Reference Dennis, Francis, Cirino, Schachar, Barnes and Fletcher2009). Other studies have suggested that IQ does not fully account for communication deficits among alcohol-exposed individuals (Doyle et al., Reference Doyle, Coles, Kable, May, Sowell, Jones, Riley and Mattson2019), although other aspects of cognitive functioning not included in the current study may play an important role in mediating the relation between PAE, language, and communication.

Conclusions

The current study is the first known comprehensive investigation of language and communication abilities of adolescents with PAE. In terms of language, the alcohol-exposed group performed below the control group on all variables though only significantly differed in receptive language skills and parent report of general language. These findings have significant clinical implications as receptive language disorders are often difficult to identify and treat. While adolescents with PAE did not differ from heterogeneous controls on a measure of functional communication, the average scores of the alcohol-exposed group fell in the below average range identifying these participants as experiencing clinically significant communication difficulties. Furthermore, parents of adolescents with PAE reported significantly impaired social communication skills as compared to controls. The clinical implications of language and communication difficulties are clear. As highlighted above, intact speech sound abilities among this population increases the risk for long-term functional impairment in the presence of underlying language and communication disorders due to lack of identification (Prizant et al., Reference Prizant, Audet, Burke, Hummel, Maher and Theadore1990). Results again highlight the need for early identification in combination with integrated and multidisciplinary treatment to improve academic, social, and overall wellbeing of youth with PAE.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the families who graciously participate in our studies. All or part of this work was done in conjunction with the Collaborative Initiative on Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders (CIFASD), which is funded by grants from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA). Additional information about CIFASD can be found at www.cifasd.org. Research described in this paper was supported by NIAAA grants U01 AA014834 and F31 AA02525.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Akbarian, G. G. (1992). Communication effects of prenatal alcohol exposure. Journal of Communication Disorders, 25, 221240. doi: 10.1016/0021-9924(92)90017-Q Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, DSM-5. 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. (2006). Children’s Communication Checklist-2 U.S. Edition. New York, NY: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Burd, L., Klug, M. G., Martsolf, J. T., & Kerbeshian, J. (2003). Fetal alcohol syndrome: Neuropsychiatric phenomics. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 25(6), 697705. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2003.07.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carney, L. J., & Chermak, G. D. (1991). Performance of American Indian children with fetal alcohol syndrome on the test of language development. Journal of Communication Disorders, 24, 123134. doi: 10.1016/0021-9924(91)90016-c CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Church, M. W., Eldis, F., Blakley, B. W., & Bawle, E. V. (1997). Hearing, language, speech, vestibular, and dentofacial disorders in fetal alcohol syndrome. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 21, 227237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Church, M. W., & Kaltenbach, J. A. (1997). Hearing, speech, language, and vestibular disorders in the fetal alcohol syndrome: A literature review. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 21, 495512. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1997.tb03796.x Google ScholarPubMed
Clark, A., O’Hare, A., Watson, J., Cohen, W., Cowie, H., Elton, R., Nasir, J., & Seckl, J. (2007). Severe receptive language disorder in childhood - familial aspects and long-term outcomes: Results from a Scottish study. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 92, 614619. doi: 10.1136/adc.2006.101758 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clegg, J., Hollis, C., Mawhood, L., & Rutter, M. (2005). Developmental language disorders - a follow-up in later adult life. Cognitive, language and psychosocial outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 128149. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00342.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coggins, T. E., Timler, G. R., & Olswang, L. B. (2007). A state of double jeopardy: Impact of prenatal alcohol exposure and adverse environments on the social communicative abilities of school-age children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorder. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38, 117127. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2007/012)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crocker, N., Vaurio, L., Riley, E. P., & Mattson, S. N. (2009). Comparison of adaptive behavior in children with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 33, 20152023. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.01040.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, K. M., Gagnier, K. R., Moore, T. E., & Todorow, M. (2013). Cognitive aspects of fetal alcohol spectrum disorder. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 4, 8192. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1202 Google ScholarPubMed
Dennis, M., Francis, D. J., Cirino, P. T., Schachar, R., Barnes, M. A. & Fletcher, J. M. (2009). Why IQ is not a covariate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of the International Neuropsychology Society, 15, 331343. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709090481 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doyle, L. R., Coles, C. D., Kable, J. A., May, P. A., Sowell, E. R., Jones, K. L., Riley, E. P., Mattson, S. N., & the CIFASD (2019). Relation between adaptive function and IQ among youth with histories of heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. Birth Defects Research, 110. doi: 10.1002/bdr2.1463 Google ScholarPubMed
Doyle, L. R., Moore, E. M., Coles, C. D., Kable, J. A., Sowell, E. R., Wozniak, J. R., Jones, K. L., Riley, E. P., Mattson, S. N., & the CIFASD (2018). Executive functioning correlates with communication abilities in youth with histories of heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 24, 112. doi: 10.1017/S1355617718000772 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DuPaul, G. J., Power, T. J., Anastopoulos, A. D., & Reid, R. (2016). ADHD rating scale - 5 for children and adolescents: Checklists, norms, and clinical interpretation. New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Flak, A. L., Bertrand, J., Denny, C. H., Kesmodel, U. S., & Cogswell, M. E. (2014). The association of mild, moderate, and binge prenatal alcohol exposure and child neuropsychological outcomes: A meta-analysis. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 38, 214226. doi: 10.1111/acer.12214 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fryer, S. L., McGee, C. L., Matt, G. E., & Mattson, S. N. (2007). Evaluation of psychopathological conditions in children with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. Pediatrics, 119(3), E733E741. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1606 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gautam, P., Lebel, C., Narr, K. L., Mattson, S. N., May, P. A., Adnams, C. M., Riley, E. P., Jones, K. L., Kan, E. C., & Sowell, E. R. (2015). Volume changes and brain-behavior relationships in white matter and subcortical gray matter in children with prenatal alcohol exposure. Human Brain Mapping, 36, 23182329. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22772 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentry, B., Griffith, L., Dancer, J., Davis, P., Eaton, B., & Schulz, E. (1998). Prenatal alcohol exposure and communication, behavior, and nonverbal intelligence of 3 school-age children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 86, 10891090. doi: 10.2466/pms.1998.86.3.1089 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glass, L., Graham, D. M., Deweese, B. N., Jones, K. L., Riley, E. P., & Mattson, S. N. (2014). Correspondence of parent report and laboratory measures of inattention and hyperactivity in children with prenatal alcohol exposure. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 42, 4350. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2014.01.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldman, R., & Fristoe, M. (2015). Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation 3 (GFTA-3). Bloomington, MN: Pearson.Google Scholar
Greene, T., Ernhart, C. B., Martier, S. S., Sokol, R. J., & Ager, J. W., Jr. (1990). Prenatal alcohol exposure and language development. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 14, 937945. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1990.tb01842.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gresham, F., & Elliott, S. N. (2008). Social Skills Improvement System (SSIS) Rating Scales. Circle Pines, MN: AGS/Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
Gross, A. C., Deling, L. A., Wozniak, J. R., & Boys, C. J. (2015). Objective measures of executive functioning are highly discrepant with parent-report in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychology, 21, 531538. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2014.911271 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iosub, S., Fuchs, M., Bingol, N., & Gromisch, D. S. (1981). Fetal alcohol syndrome revisited. Pediatrics, 68, 475479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, C. J., Beitchman, J. H., Young, A., Escobar, M., Atkinson, L., Wilson, B., Brownlie, E. B., Douglas, L., Taback, N., Lam, I., & Wang, M. (1999). Fourteen-year follow-up of children with and without speech/language impairments: Speech/language stability and outcomes. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 744760. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4203.744 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, K. L., Robinson, L. K., Bakhireva, L. N., Marintcheva, G., Storojev, V., Strahova, A., Sergeevskaya, S., Budantseva, S., Mattson, S. N., Riley, E. P., & Chambers, C. D. (2006). Accuracy of the diagnosis of physical features of fetal alcohol syndrome by pediatricians after specialized training. Pediatrics, 118, E1734E1738. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1037 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kodituwakku, P., & Kodituwakku, E. (2014). Cognitive and behavioral profiles of children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, 1, 149160. doi: 10.1007/s40474-014-0022-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kodituwakku, P. W. (2007). Defining the behavioral phenotype in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: A review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 31, 192201. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuehn, D., Aros, S., Cassorla, F., Avaria, M., Unanue, N., Henriquez, C., Kleinsteuber, K., Conca, B., Avila, A., Carter, T. C., Conley, M. R., Troendle, J., & Mills, J. L. (2012). A prospective cohort study of the prevalence of growth, facial, and central nervous system abnormalities in children with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 36, 18111819. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2012.01794.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kully-Martens, K., Denys, K., Treit, S., Tamana, S., & Rasmussen, C. (2012). A review of social skills deficits in individuals with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders and prenatal alcohol exposure: Profiles, mechanisms, and interventions. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 36, 568576. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01661.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landgren, M., Svensson, L., Strömland, K., & Grönlund, M. A. (2010). Prenatal alcohol exposure and neurodevelopmental disorders in children adopted from eastern Europe. Pediatrics, 125(5), e11781185. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-0712 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Law, J., Garrett, Z., & Nye, C. (2004). The efficacy of treatment for children with developmental speech and language delay/disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 924943. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/069)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L. (1998). Children with Specific Language Impairment (Language, Speech and Communication series). Camridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
MacDonald, S. (2013). Student Functional Assessment of Verbal Reasoning and Executive Strategies. Ontario, Canada: CCD Publishing.Google Scholar
Mattson, S. N., Bernes, G. A., & Doyle, L. R. (2019). Fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: A review of neurobehavioral deficits associated with prenatal alcohol exposure. Alcoholism: Clnical and Experimental Research, 43, 10461062. doi: 10.1111/acer.14040 Google ScholarPubMed
Mattson, S. N., Crocker, N., & Nguyen, T. T. (2011). Fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: Neuropsychological and behavioral features. Neuropsychology Review, 21, 81101. doi: 10.1007/s11065-011-9167-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mattson, S. N., Foroud, T., Sowell, E. R., Jones, K. L., Coles, C. D., Fagerlund, Å., Autti-Rämö, I., May, P. A., Adnams, C. A., Konovalova, V., Wetherill, L., Arenson, A. D., Barnett, W. K., Riley, E. P., & the CIFASD. (2010). Collaborative Initiative on Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders: Methodology of clinical projects. Alcohol, 44, 635641. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2009.08.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mattson, S. N., & Riley, E. P. (1998). A review of the neurobehavioral deficits in children with fetal alcohol syndrome or prenatal exposure to alcohol. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 22, 279294. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1998.tb03651.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mattson, S. N., Riley, E. P., Delis, D. C., Stern, C., & Jones, K. L. (1996). Verbal learning and memory in children with fetal alcohol syndrome. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 20, 810816. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1996.tb05256.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGee, C. L., Bjorkquist, O. A., Riley, E. P., & Mattson, S. N. (2009). Impaired language performance in young children with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 31, 7175. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2008.09.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mohamed, Z., Carlisle, A. C. S., Livesey, A. C., & Mukherjee, R. A. S. (2019). Comparisons of the BRIEF parental report and neuropsychological clinical tests of executive function in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: Data from the UK national specialist clinic. Child Neuropsychology, 25, 648663. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2018.1516202 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nash, K., Stevens, S., Rovet, J., Fantus, E., Nulman, I., Sorbara, D., & Koren, G. (2013). Towards identifying a characteristic neuropsychological profile for fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. 1. Analysis of the Motherisk FASD clinic. Journal of Population Therapeutics and Clinical Pharmacology, 20, e4452.Google ScholarPubMed
Nguyen, T. T., Glass, L., Coles, C. D., Kable, J. A., May, P. A., Kalberg, W. O., Sowell, E. R, Jones, K. L., Riley, E. P., Mattson, S. N., & the CIFASD (2014). The clinical utility and specificity of parent report of executive function among children with prenatal alcohol exposure. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20. doi: 10.1017/S1355617714000599 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Connor, M. J., & Paley, B. (2009). Psychiatric conditions associated with prenatal alcohol exposure. Developmental Disabilities Research Review, 15(3), 225234. doi: 10.1002/ddrr.74 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Popova, S., Lange, S., Shield, K., Mihic, A., Chudley, A. E., Mukherjee, R. A. S., Bekmuradov, D., & Rehm, J. (2016). Comorbidity of fetal alcohol spectrum disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet, 387, 978987. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)01345-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prizant, B. M., Audet, L. R., Burke, G. M., Hummel, L. J., Maher, S. R., & Theadore, G. (1990). Communication disorders and emotional/behavioral disorders in children and adolescents. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 179192. doi: 10.1044/jshd.5502.179 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rai, J. K., Abecassis, M., Casey, J. E., Flaro, L., Erdodi, L. A., & Roth, R. M. (2017). Parent rating of executive function in fetal alcohol spectrum disorder: A review of the literature and new data on Aboriginal Canadian children. Child Neuropsychology, 23, 713732. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2016.1191628 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rasmussen, C., Benz, J., Pei, J., Andrew, G., Schuller, G., Abele-Webster, L., Alton, C., & Lord, L. (2010). The impact of an ADHD co-morbidity on the diagnosis of FASD. The Canadian Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 17(1), e165176.Google Scholar
Riley, E. P., Infante, M. A., & Warren, K. R. (2011). Fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: An overview. Neuropsychology Review, 21, 7380. doi: 10.1007/s11065-011-9166-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sciberras, E., Mueller, K. L., Efron, D., Bisset, M., Anderson, V., Schilpzand, E. J., Jongeling, B., & Nicholson, J. M. (2014). Language problems in children with ADHD: A community-based study. Pediatrics, 133(5), 793800. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-3355 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sowell, E. R., Johnson, A., Kan, E., Lu, L. H., Van Horn, J. D., Toga, A. W., O'Connor, M. J., & Bookheimer, S. Y. (2008). Mapping white matter integrity and neurobehavioral correlates in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 13131319. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5067-07.2008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Peterson, B. S., Mattson, S. N., Welcome, S. E., Henkenius, A. L., Riley, E. P., Jernigan, T. L., & Toga, A. W. (2002). Mapping cortical gray matter asymmetry patterns in adolescents with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. NeuroImage, 17, 18071819. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1328 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stothard, S. E., Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M., Chipchase, B. B., & Kaplan, C. A. (1998). Language-impaired preschoolers: A follow-up into adolescence. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 407418. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4102.407 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Streissguth, A. P. (1986). The behavioral teratology of alcohol: Performance, behavioral, and intellectual deficits in prenatally exposed children. In West, J. R. (Ed.), Alcohol and Brain Development (pp. 344). New York, NY: Oxford.Google Scholar
Thorne, J. C. (2017). Accentuate the negative: Grammatical errors during narrative production as a clinical marker of central nervous system abnormality in school-aged children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60, 35233537. doi: 10.1044/2017_JSLHR-L-17-0128 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thorne, J. C., Coggins, T. E., Carmichael Olson, H., & Astley, S. (2007). Exploring the utility of narrative analysis in diagnostic decision making: Picture-bound reference, elaboration, and fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 459474. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/032)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treit, S., Lebel, C., Baugh, L., Rasmussen, C., Andrew, G., & Beaulieu, C. (2013). Longitudinal MRI reveals altered trajectory of brain development during childhood and adolescence in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 1009810109. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5004-12.2013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treit, S., Zhou, D., Lebel, C., Rasmussen, C., Andrew, G., & Beaulieu, C. (2014). Longitudinal MRI reveals impaired cortical thinning in children and adolescents prenatally exposed to alcohol. Human Brain Mapping, 35, 48924903. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22520 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiig, E.H., Semel, E., & Secord, W.A. (2013). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-Fifth Edition (CELF-5). Bloomington, MN: Pearson.Google Scholar
Wyper, K. R., & Rasmussen, C. R. (2011). Language impairments in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Journal of Population Therapeutics and Clinical Pharmacology, 18, E364E376.Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic information for adolescents with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure (AE) and controls (CON)

Figure 1

Table 2. Group performance on neuropsychological variables

Figure 2

Table 3. Correlations among MANOVA-dependent variables for separate language and communication analyses, respectively

Figure 3

Table 4. MANOVA results for language profile by group

Figure 4

Figure 1. Profile of language abilities by group. Groups included adolescents with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure (AE) and controls (CON). Groups significantly differed on receptive language and parent report of general language.Note: * p < .05 level. Receptive Language and Expressive Language were measured the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals – Fifth Edition. Speech Sound abilities were measured by the Sounds-In-Words subtest from Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation – Third Edition. General Language was measured by parent report from the General Communication Scale from the Children’s Communication Checklist – Second Edition.

Figure 5

Table 5. MANOVA results for communication profile by group

Figure 6

Figure 2. Profile of communication abilities by group. Groups included adolescents with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure (AE) and controls (CON). Groups significantly differed in social communication.Note: * p < .05 level. Groups significantly differed on social communication. Functional Communication was measured by the Total Reasoning Subskills standard score from the Student Functional Assessment of Verbal Reasoning and Executive Strategies test. Social Communication was measured by the Social Skills standard score from the Social Skills Improvement System Rating Scales completed by parents.