Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-2lccl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T21:33:47.687Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Social isolation from childhood to mid-adulthood: is there an association with older brain age?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 July 2023

Roy Lay-Yee*
Affiliation:
Centre of Methods and Policy Application in the Social Sciences, and School of Social Sciences, Faculty of Arts, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
Ahmad R. Hariri
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA
Annchen R. Knodt
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA
Ashleigh Barrett-Young
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
Timothy Matthews
Affiliation:
Department of Social Genetic & Developmental Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, London, UK
Barry J. Milne
Affiliation:
Centre of Methods and Policy Application in the Social Sciences, and School of Social Sciences, Faculty of Arts, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand Department of Statistics, Faculty of Science, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
*
Corresponding author: Roy Lay-Yee; Email: r.layyee@auckland.ac.nz
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Older brain age – as estimated from structural MRI data – is known to be associated with detrimental mental and physical health outcomes in older adults. Social isolation, which has similar detrimental effects on health, may be associated with accelerated brain aging though little is known about how different trajectories of social isolation across the life course moderate this association. We examined the associations between social isolation trajectories from age 5 to age 38 and brain age assessed at age 45.

Methods

We previously created a typology of social isolation based on onset during the life course and persistence into adulthood, using group-based trajectory analysis of longitudinal data from a New Zealand birth cohort. The typology comprises four groups: ‘never-isolated’, ‘adult-only’, ‘child-only’, and persistent ‘child-adult’ isolation. A brain age gap estimate (brainAGE) – the difference between predicted age from structural MRI date and chronological age – was derived at age 45. We undertook analyses of brainAGE with trajectory group as the predictor, adjusting for sex, family socio-economic status, and a range of familial and child-behavioral factors.

Results

Older brain age in mid-adulthood was associated with trajectories of social isolation after adjustment for family and child confounders, particularly for the ‘adult-only’ group compared to the ‘never-isolated’ group.

Conclusions

Although our findings are associational, they indicate that preventing social isolation, particularly in mid-adulthood, may help to avert accelerated brain aging associated with negative health outcomes later in life.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Background

Humans need connection and interaction with one another to sustain good health (Berkman, Glass, Brissette, & Seeman, Reference Berkman, Glass, Brissette and Seeman2000; Umberson & Montez, Reference Umberson and Montez2010). A negative indicator of social connection is the degree to which an individual is isolated, i.e. lacking contact with others and being alone most of the time (Cacioppo, Hawkley, Norman, & Berntson, Reference Cacioppo, Hawkley, Norman and Berntson2011; de Jong Gierveld, van Tilburg, & Dykstra, Reference de Jong Gierveld, van Tilburg, Dykstra, Vangelisti and Perlman2006). Social isolation is increasingly recognized as a threat to public health and well-being that requires intervention at a societal level (Holt-Lunstad, Robles, & Sbarra, Reference Holt-Lunstad, Robles and Sbarra2017; Leigh-Hunt et al., Reference Leigh-Hunt, Bagguley, Bash, Turner, Turnbull, Valtorta and Caan2017). This threat has been further highlighted by the impact on mental health of social restrictions deployed to counter the worst of the COVID-19 pandemic (Aleman & Sommer, Reference Aleman and Sommer2022; Bu, Steptoe, & Fancourt, Reference Bu, Steptoe and Fancourt2022; Sommerlad et al., Reference Sommerlad, Marston, Huntley, Livingston, Lewis, Steptoe and Fancourt2022).

But what are the features of social isolation and how might it affect health? The occurrence of social isolation is particularly related to social disadvantage (Rohr et al., Reference Rohr, Wittmann, Engel, Enzenbach, Witte, Villringer and Riedel-Heller2021) and thus to social exclusion (Holt-Lunstad & Steptoe, Reference Holt-Lunstad and Steptoe2022; Umberson & Donnelly, Reference Umberson and Donnelly2023). Social isolation can affect individuals at any age, may occur earlier or later during the life course, and may be transient or persistent. Longitudinal investigations of social isolation from childhood to adulthood (Caspi, Harrington, Moffitt, Milne, & Poulton, Reference Caspi, Harrington, Moffitt, Milne and Poulton2006) are important to understand the development of social isolation and its time-dependent effects. The insights gained from a life-course perspective (Ben-Shlomo & Kuh, Reference Ben-Shlomo and Kuh2002) can then be used to identify earlier influences and indicate possible points for intervention to reduce risk of harm in adulthood (Osborn, Weatherburn, & French, Reference Osborn, Weatherburn and French2021). In children, poor family environment has been associated with social isolation (Repetti, Taylor, & Seeman, Reference Repetti, Taylor and Seeman2002). This is a barrier to successful attachment with parents or care-givers which impacts on typical child development with ongoing consequences (Bowlby, Reference Bowlby1969). Social isolation has been shown to have negative consequences for children's social and emotional functioning (Bukowski & Adams, Reference Bukowski and Adams2005; Coplan, Ooi, Xiao, & Rose-Krasnor, Reference Coplan, Ooi, Xiao and Rose-Krasnor2018; Marryat, Thompson, Minnis, & Wilson, Reference Marryat, Thompson, Minnis and Wilson2014). Further, children who have been socially isolated may carry wide-ranging adverse health effects into adulthood, resulting in worse morbidity and mortality [e.g. cardiovascular disease (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Harrington, Moffitt, Milne and Poulton2006), depression (Danese et al., Reference Danese, Moffitt, Harrington, Milne, Polanczyk, Pariante and Caspi2009), inflammation (Lacey, Kumaria, & Bartley, Reference Lacey, Kumaria and Bartley2014), suicide (Rojas, Reference Rojas2018), and premature death (Holt-Lunstad, Smith, Baker, Harris, & Stephenson, Reference Holt-Lunstad, Smith, Baker, Harris and Stephenson2015)]. Examining trajectories of social isolation from childhood to adulthood may give clues to disentangling the direction of causality and thus enable the potential prevention of poorer health outcomes eventuating from social isolation and its antecedents.

Recent systematic reviews have concluded that positive social connections are associated with slower cognitive decline (Samtani et al., Reference Samtani, Mahalingam, Lam, Lipnicki, Lima-Costa and Blay2022), and that, conversely, a lack of social contact is associated with reduced cognitive function (Kelly et al., Reference Kelly, Duff, Kelly, McHugh Power, Brennan, Lawlor and Loughrey2017), with social isolation specifically related to progressive cognitive decline in older adults (Evans, Martyr, Collins, Brayne, & Clare, Reference Evans, Martyr, Collins, Brayne and Clare2019). There is also evidence that the positive association between social isolation and memory decline is uni-directional rather than reciprocal, i.e. a higher level of social isolation leads to increased memory decline (Read, Comas-Herrera, & Grundy, Reference Read, Comas-Herrera and Grundy2020). Supporting these linkages, pathological findings show associations between cognitive decline and lower social engagement in healthy older people (Biddle et al., Reference Biddle, Uquillas, Jacobs, Zide, Kirn, Rentz and Donovan2019).

While the mechanisms underlying these associations between social integration and health have not been fully determined, behavioral, psychological, and physiological pathways likely play a role (Berkman et al., Reference Berkman, Glass, Brissette and Seeman2000). For example, being part of a social network tends to promote positive health behaviors, enables access to support and other resources, buffers against stress, and provides a sense of well-being and meaning (Di Marco et al., Reference Di Marco, Marzo, Munoz-Ruiz, Ikram, Kivipelto, Ruefenacht and Ventikos2014; Umberson, Crosnoe, & Reczek, Reference Umberson, Crosnoe and Reczek2010). Being actively engaged in social relations plays a foundational role in developing brain reserve (tolerance of the brain's structure to damage) and cognitive reserve (resilience of the brain's function to structural deficits), which act as protective factors against cognitive decline (Barnett, Salmond, Jones, & Sahakian, Reference Barnett, Salmond, Jones and Sahakian2006). The brain's plasticity enables its re-modeling and adaptation in response to internal or external stimuli (Jellinger & Attems, Reference Jellinger and Attems2013).

Neurophysiological mechanisms – including measures of brain health – have also been implicated in the relationship between social ties and health (Eisenberger & Cole, Reference Eisenberger and Cole2012). The relationship between social isolation and cognitive decline may be mediated by brain age, a measure of brain health. Greater risk of cognitive decline is indicated by deleterious neurochemical changes in the brain that may accompany the aging process (Cleeland, Pipingas, Scholey, & White, Reference Cleeland, Pipingas, Scholey and White2019). Both structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) results have been found to mirror age-related changes in neurochemistry (Eavani et al., Reference Eavani, Habes, Satterthwaite, An, Hsieh, Honnorat and Davatzikos2018). MRI measures of brain structure can be extracted and analyzed to estimate biological brain age (Franke & Gaser, Reference Franke and Gaser2019; MacDonald & Pike, Reference MacDonald and Pike2021). The difference between estimated brain age and actual chronological age [i.e. brain age gap estimate (brainAGE)] can then indicate whether an individual's brain has aged more or less than their peers, calibrated against a population benchmark, reflecting overall brain health and the degree of any underlying neuroanatomical abnormalities present (Liem et al., Reference Liem, Varoquaux, Kynast, Beyer, Kharabian Masouleh, Huntenburg and Margulies2017; Smith, Vidaurre, Alfaro-Almagro, Nichols, & Miller, Reference Smith, Vidaurre, Alfaro-Almagro, Nichols and Miller2019). Brain age has been shown to predict accelerated cognitive decline (Franke & Gaser, Reference Franke and Gaser2012). In particular, older brain age at midlife has been associated with cognitive decline from childhood (Elliott et al., Reference Elliott, Belsky, Knodt, Ireland, Melzer, Poulton and Hariri2021) as well as cognitive impairment in later life (Zheng et al., Reference Zheng, Habes, Gonzales, Pomponio, Nasrallah, Khan and Hou2022).

Social isolation has been suggested as a predictor of brain age, e.g. socially isolated individuals showed older estimated brain age relative to controls in a large population-based study (de Lange et al., Reference de Lange, Kaufmann, Quintana, Winterton, Andreassen, Westlye and Ebmeier2021). However, little is known whether the timing (i.e. life stage of onset) or duration (i.e. persistence across life stages) of social isolation have an impact on brain age. For instance, it is not known whether (i) child-onset social isolation has long-term negative effects on adult brain structure and thus on cognitive function, (2) more proximal (i.e. adult) experiences of social isolation impact cognitive decline, or (iii) prolonged exposure to social isolation has cumulative effects on health (e.g. Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Harrington, Moffitt, Milne and Poulton2006).

To understand the impact of timing and duration on cognitive function, this study investigates whether trajectories of social isolation from childhood to midlife are related to estimated brain age gap, a known biomarker of cognitive decline. If so, then the relationship between social isolation and the deterioration of cognitive function may – at least in part – be mediated by deleterious changes in brain structure that accelerate brain aging. We have recently identified four distinct trajectory groups of social isolation using longitudinal data (never-isolated, child-only, adult-only, or child-adult), and have demonstrated that these have different risk factor profiles by family and child characteristics (Lay-Yee et al., Reference Lay-Yee, Matthews, Moffitt, Poulton, Caspi and Milne2021). Here, we aim to investigate the degree to which these trajectory groups across a long stretch of the lifespan (from childhood to age 38) are related to brain age in mid-adulthood (assessed at chronological age 45). Specifically, we investigate two research questions:

  1. (1) How does the course of social isolation – i.e. membership of trajectory groups: ‘never-isolated’, ‘child-only’, ‘adult-only’, or persistent ‘child-adult’ isolation – impact brain age (at age 45)? We hypothesize that patterns of social isolation in children and adults affect adult brain age, that the adverse effect of social isolation is greater the earlier it occurs during the life course (Vidal-Pineiro et al., Reference Vidal-Pineiro, Wang, Krogsrud, Amlien, Baaré, Bartres-Faz and Fjell2021), and that any adverse effects accumulate, i.e. there is an effect of persistence (Rudenstine & Galea, Reference Rudenstine and Galea2015). We expect that adult brain age will be older in the isolated groups compared to the ‘never-isolated’ group, greater in ‘child-only’ v. ‘adult-only’ groups, and greatest in the ‘child-adult’ group.

  2. (2) Can the association between social isolation and brain age, uncovered in (1), be explained by risk factors associated with social isolation also being associated with brain age? We hypothesize that the association between social isolation and brain age will still hold after controlling for factors known to be associated with social isolation.

Methods

Data source

We used data from the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study (DMHDS), an ongoing longitudinal investigation of the health and behavior of a complete birth cohort of consecutive births (N = 1037) over a one-year period from 1 April 1972 to 31 March 1973 in Dunedin, New Zealand (Poulton, Guiney, Ramrakha, & Moffitt, Reference Poulton, Guiney, Ramrakha and Moffitt2022; Poulton, Moffitt, & Silva, Reference Poulton, Moffitt and Silva2015). The DMHDS has been approved by the New Zealand Health and Disability Ethics Committee, and informed consent was obtained from participants. To date, assessments have been carried out at ages 3, 5, 7, 9, 11, 13, 15, 18, 21, 26, 32, 38, and 45. We focus on assessments of outcome at age 45 in 2017–2019 when 94% (n = 938) of living cohort members participated in the study. The study sample (n = 855) includes participants who had data collected on social isolation at ages 5–11 and 26–38, as well as on brain age in mid-adulthood (at age 45).

Description of variables

Social isolation

Social isolation in childhood at ages 5, 7, 9, and 11, and again in adulthood at ages 26, 32, and 38, has been used to statistically derive four trajectory groups: never-isolated (neither isolated as a child nor as an adult); child-only (isolated as a child but not as an adult), adult-only (first became isolated as an adult), and persistent child-adult isolation (isolated both as a child and as an adult) (Lay-Yee et al., Reference Lay-Yee, Matthews, Moffitt, Poulton, Caspi and Milne2021; Nagin, Reference Nagin2005). Membership of these groups represents 30-year trajectories in change of social isolation status.

Child isolation was assessed by a collection of measures from ages 5 to 11 (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Harrington, Moffitt, Milne and Poulton2006; Danese et al., Reference Danese, Moffitt, Harrington, Milne, Polanczyk, Pariante and Caspi2009). When a Study member was 5, 7, 9, and 11 years old, their parent and teacher completed the Rutter Child Scale (Elander & Rutter, Reference Elander and Rutter1996), reporting on two items that measure peer problems: ‘tends to do things on his/her own; is rather solitary’ and ‘not much liked by other children’. At each age, scores on these two scale items (0 = doesn't apply, 1 = applies somewhat, 2 = certainly applies) were averaged across the two reporting sources (i.e. parent and teacher).

Adult isolation was assessed using informant report at ages 26, 32, and 38, whereby up to three informants whom the Study member nominated as ‘knowing them well’ was mailed a questionnaire. At each age, over 90% of Study members had reports from at least two informants, and over 60% had reports from all three informants. At each age, scores on the item ‘seems lonely’ (0 = not a problem, 1 = bit of a problem, 2 = yes, a problem) were averaged across informants.

Brain age

The brain age gap estimate (brainAGE) was derived from multiple measures of brain structure – i.e. cortical thickness, surface area, and volumes of subcortical gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid – taken from MRI, based on an algorithm developed by Liem et al. (Reference Liem, Varoquaux, Kynast, Beyer, Kharabian Masouleh, Huntenburg and Margulies2017). The specific method has been described in detail elsewhere (Elliott et al., Reference Elliott, Belsky, Knodt, Ireland, Melzer, Poulton and Hariri2021). Study members were scanned using a MAGNETOM Skyra 3 T scanner (Siemens Healthcare, Erlangen, Germany) between August 2016 and April 2019. High resolution T1-weighted images, three-dimensional fluid-attenuated inversion recovery images, and a gradient echo field map were obtained. Structural MRI data were analyzed using the Human Connectome Project (HCP) minimal preprocessing pipeline (Glasser et al., Reference Glasser, Sotiropoulos, Wilson, Coalson, Fischl and Andersson2013). BrainAGE was quantified as the difference between estimated brain age and exact chronological age (birth to MRI scan date).

Confounders (childhood)

A complex interplay of factors may be involved in the relationship between social isolation and brain age (de Lange et al., Reference de Lange, Kaufmann, Quintana, Winterton, Andreassen, Westlye and Ebmeier2021; Richmond-Rakerd et al., Reference Richmond-Rakerd, Caspi, Ambler, d'Arbeloff, de Bruine, Elliott and Moffitt2021). We have found a number of familial and child-behavioral covariates to be associated with social isolation trajectory group membership (Lay-Yee et al., Reference Lay-Yee, Matthews, Moffitt, Poulton, Caspi and Milne2021). We adjusted multivariable analyses for these potential confounders of the association between social isolation and brain age. We also adjusted these analyses for socio-demographic factors identified as potentially important from the literature: sex (Umberson, Lin, & Cha, Reference Umberson, Lin and Cha2022) and family socio-economic status (Schneider, Richard, Younger, & Freeman, Reference Schneider, Richard, Younger and Freeman2000).

Socio-demographic factors

  1. i Sex was defined as binary at birth: male/female.

  2. ii Family socio-economic status was measured using the Elley-Irving scale of occupational status (Elley & Irving, Reference Elley and Irving1972), estimated as the average of the higher level of either parent assessed at birth and at ages 3, 5, 7, 9, 11, 13, and 15 years, with scores grouped into low, medium, and high categories.

Family factors

  1. i A teen-aged mother was defined as being aged 18 or under and coded in a binary variable as ‘yes’ or ‘no’.

  2. ii We assessed whether the child had a single parent for at least one year up to age 11 as a binary variable coded ‘yes’ or ‘no’.

  3. iii We measured change in residence as the number of times that this occurred up to age 11.

  4. iv An index of maltreatment was formed by combining measures of atypical maternal behavior (age 3), harsh discipline (ages 7 and 9), disruptive caregiver changes from birth to age 11, exposure to physical abuse from birth to age 11, and exposure to sexual abuse from birth to age 11 (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, McClay, Moffitt, Mill, Martin, Craig and Poulton2002).

Child-behavioral factors

  1. i Self-control, the ability to regulate emotions and behavior, was assessed using a scale combining measures of lack of control (ages 3 and 5), impulsivity (ages 5, 7, 9, and 11), and hyperactivity (ages 5, 7, 9, and 11) (Moffitt et al., Reference Moffitt, Arseneault, Belsky, Dickson, Hancox, Harrington and Caspi2011). The scale was split into quintiles for the purposes of analysis (quintile 1 = highest self-control; quintile 5 = lowest self-control).

  2. ii When a Study member was 5, 7, 9, and 11 years old, their parent and teacher completed the Rutter Behavior Scales (Elander & Rutter, Reference Elander and Rutter1996). Scores on the worried/fearful scale, a measure of childhood internalizing symptoms, were averaged across rater and age and split into quintiles for analysis and interpretation (note that items relating to social isolation were not used in the construction of the worry/fearful scale).

Data analysis

We previously derived a typology comprising four groups according to the onset and duration of social isolation: never-isolated (n = 710, 71.6% of the cohort), child-only (n = 142, 14.3%), adult-only (n = 100, 10.1%), and child-adult (n = 40, 4.0%) (Lay-Yee et al., Reference Lay-Yee, Matthews, Moffitt, Poulton, Caspi and Milne2021). Here, we employed that same typology of trajectory groups to examine its relationship to adult brain age (at chronological age 45).

Firstly, we ascertained the mean brainAGE (with standard deviations) for each trajectory group and across categories within potential confounders, and then tested to see if there were bivariate associations (p < 0.05). Secondly, we used linear regression analysis to ascertain whether brainAGE was predicted by different trajectory group membership, while controlling for childhood factors previously shown to be important risk factors for social isolation, i.e. having a teenaged mother, having a single parent, changes in residence, maltreatment, self-control, and worry/fearfulness (Lay-Yee et al., Reference Lay-Yee, Matthews, Moffitt, Poulton, Caspi and Milne2021); as well as sex and family socio-economic status. We report model-adjusted marginal means (with standard errors) of brainAGE by trajectory group and confounders, and beta coefficients (with 95% confidence intervals and p values) to indicate the importance of trajectory group. To check that the differences in trajectory group size were not affecting the results of our regression analysis, we carried out online Supplementary analysis based on the average scores across the four measures in childhood and the three measures in adulthood, converted to z scores for ease of interpretation. Analyses were carried out using Stata 16.0 (StataCorp, 2019).

Results

Bivariate associations

Table 1 shows brainAGE stratified by the trajectory groups, indicating very strong associations though without any covariate adjustment. There are clear differences between group means, particularly between the ‘adult only’ (mean 1.78, s.d. 7.71, p = 0.012) and persistent ‘child-adult’ (mean 3.47, s.d. 8.85, p = 0.006) groups and the ‘never-isolated’ group (mean −0.58, s.d. 7.91). Results suggest that there is a sizeable 4-year difference between brainAGE for the persistent ‘child-adult’ group and the ‘never-isolated’ group, using our trajectory classification. Family circumstances in childhood showing an association with brainAGE were socio-economic status, change in residence, and maltreatment. Sex was also significantly related to brainAGE though child behavioral factors were not.

Table 1. Brain age gap estimate (brainAGE) at age 45 years by ‘trajectory’ group and family-child factors

* p < 0.05.

a Positive values indicate older and negative values younger estimated brain age compared with chronological age.

b One-way analysis of variance of brain age gap by trajectory group and covariates respectively.

Covariate-adjusted associations

Table 2 shows that social isolation retained an effect (now attenuated) on brainAGE independent of early family and child factors, particularly in the case of ‘adult-only’ isolation (marginal mean 1.73, s.e. 0.89, p = 0.024). It should be noted that persistent ‘child-adult’ isolation had the highest mean brainAGE (2.29 years), but the difference between the persistent ‘child-adult’ and ‘never-isolated’ groups was not statistically significant.

Table 2. Relationship between ‘trajectory’ group and brain age gap estimate (brainAGE) at age 45 years

* p < 0.05.

a Positive values indicate older and negative values younger estimated brain age compared with chronological age.

b Multi-way analysis of variance model, controlled for sex, socio-economic status, teenaged mother, single parent, change in residence, maltreatment, self-control, and worry/fearfulness.

Supplementary analysis

Differences in trajectory group size did not affect the results of our regression analysis. Re-analysis using continuous isolation scores (rather than groups) showed that adult isolation was important and child isolation was not, corroborating our original analysis of trajectory groups. There was no interaction indicating that there was no extra effect of having high scores on both adult and child isolation (see online Supplementary Table S1).

Conclusions

This paper presents the findings of a longitudinal study of social isolation from a New Zealand population-representative birth cohort followed from birth to mid-adulthood. We used four trajectory groups, identified in an earlier study (Lay-Yee et al., Reference Lay-Yee, Matthews, Moffitt, Poulton, Caspi and Milne2021), that mapped well to the onset and persistence of social isolation: never-isolated, child-only, adult-only, and child-adult. The trajectory groups were shown not to differ by sex, while the child-adult group was found to consistently have the worst profile across both family-environment and child-behavior factors. Our research questions aimed to test if membership of these trajectory groups was associated with older brain age at age 45, and, if so, whether any association was upheld after controlling for relevant family and child confounders.

Principal findings

Our findings corroborate and add to the evidence that there is a positive association between adult social isolation and brain age (e.g. de Lange et al., Reference de Lange, Kaufmann, Quintana, Winterton, Andreassen, Westlye and Ebmeier2021). We show, in analyses adjusted for confounders, that individuals with adult-only social isolation had mean estimated brain age that was 1.73 years older than individuals who never experienced social isolation. The pattern of relationship between social isolation trajectory groups and brain age suggested in unadjusted analysis disappeared in the presence of confounders. The mean estimated brain age of the persistently isolated (child-adult) group differed from that of the never-isolated group in unadjusted analyses (3.47 years older) but was attenuated and non-significant in adjusted analyses (2.29 years older). The mean estimated brain age for the child-only group did not differ from that for the never-isolated group, either in unadjusted or adjusted analyses. In general, it appeared that the experience of social isolation in adulthood – with or without childhood isolation – had the greatest impact on estimated brain age (Matthews et al., Reference Matthews, Qualter, Bryan, Caspi, Danese, Moffitt and Arseneault2022).

Implications

The theoretical significance of this study lies in its focus on the longitudinal effects of social isolation on an adult outcome (de Jong Gierveld et al., Reference de Jong Gierveld, van Tilburg, Dykstra, Vangelisti and Perlman2006), specifically brain age. Our finding suggesting that adult isolation has a greater influence on brain age than persistent (child-adult) isolation requires further investigation, as it seems to run counter to the hypothesis that long-term exposure to social isolation leads to chronic biological embedding, and, in turn, to older brain age. An alternative explanation may be reverse causation – i.e. older brain age leads to social isolation – which could be tested in future with repeated measurements of both social isolation and brain age. Our finding that child-only isolation was not related to older brain age is also unexpected and against our hypothesis that earlier exposure would have a greater effect on outcome. We speculate that the child's brain may be resilient to or has the capacity to recover from being socially isolated, particularly if isolation is not experienced again as an adult. Nevertheless, the current study helps to elucidate the contribution of social isolation to older brain age (while controlling for confounders located in childhood) and adds novel evidence as to its detrimental effect on brain aging. Social isolation, defined as an extreme lack of social ties, is a relational phenomenon that may become biologically embedded in the person who experiences it, i.e. the brain's structure and function are able to be shaped by social relations (Bzdok & Dunbar, Reference Bzdok and Dunbar2022; Morese & Palermo, Reference Morese and Palermo2022). For example, social isolation reduces access to the stimulation that is required to build stocks of cognitive reserve, thus diminishing the buffer against cognitive decline (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Llewellyn, Matthews, Woods, Brayne and Clare2018). Potential neurophysiological mechanisms driving the association between social isolation and brain health include effects on brain chemistry, oxidative stress, immune response, inflammation, impaired myelination, synapse loss, and allostatic load (Drinkwater, Davies, & Spires-Jones, Reference Drinkwater, Davies and Spires-Jones2021; Eisenberger & Cole, Reference Eisenberger and Cole2012). At a molecular level, the disruption of gene regulation and expression in the brain may be underpinning these physiological effects of social isolation (Arzate-Mejía, Lottenbach, Schindler, Jawaid, & Mansuy, Reference Arzate-Mejía, Lottenbach, Schindler, Jawaid and Mansuy2020).

Even if the temporal relationships reported here are not causal, they suggest that social isolation at different life stages – particularly in mid-adulthood – is associated with older brain age. The association is strong enough for even mild levels of cognitive impairment to be linked to social isolation (Ishikawa, Davis, Chen, & Lim, Reference Ishikawa, Davis, Chen and Lim2022). Brain age, a global measure of brain health, may be mediating the effect of social isolation on cognitive decline, given that both social ties and brain health have been associated with cognitive function. For example, it has been found that gray matter volume, a specific measure of brain health, partly mediates the longitudinal association between social isolation and cognitive function (Shen et al., Reference Shen, Rolls, Cheng, Kang, Dong, Xie and Feng2022). Finally, we may speculate that there are implications for understanding dementia, a pathological form of cognitive dysfunction, given that a lack of social contact has been associated with elevated dementia risk (see reviews by: Desai, John, Stott, & Charlesworth, Reference Desai, John, Stott and Charlesworth2020; Kuiper et al., Reference Kuiper, Zuidersma, Oude Voshaar, Zuidema, van den Heuvel, Stolk and Smidt2015; Penninkilampi, Casey, Singh, & Brodaty, Reference Penninkilampi, Casey, Singh and Brodaty2018; Ren et al., Reference Ren, Savadlou, Park, Siska, Epp and Sargin2023; and Sommerlad, Ruegger, Singh-Manoux, Lewis, & Livingston, Reference Sommerlad, Ruegger, Singh-Manoux, Lewis and Livingston2018), and that older brain age has been considered to be a biomarker for (Gaser, Franke, Klöppel, Koutsouleris, & Sauer, Reference Gaser, Franke, Klöppel, Koutsouleris and Sauer2013; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Knol, Tiulpin, Dubost, de Bruijne, Vernooij and Roshchupkin2019) and midlife antecedent of (Reuben et al., Reference Reuben, Moffitt, Abraham, Ambler, Elliott, Hariri and Caspi2022) dementia.

In terms of policy and practice, understanding the influence of life-course differences in onset and persistence of social isolation on brain health should inform screening and intervention strategies. The evidence on interventions is currently sparse and the effectiveness of interventions is likely to vary by life stage. Our study suggests that reducing social isolation could be used to improve brain health and subsequently prevent, arrest, or delay cognitive decline. Timely detection of cognitive decline, before the appearance of significant clinical symptoms, is important as interventions will be more likely to be effective earlier in the life course. Further, our findings may inform the specification of interventions to suit sub-populations or individuals at different life stages (early, mid, and later) and in recognizing vulnerable groups (by sex, social disadvantage, or family and childhood factors) that might benefit from public services. In particular, our finding of an adult effect and tentatively a persistent effect of social isolation on brain age may indicate points for intervention. For example, providing early support for vulnerable adults in midlife may be crucial to preventing the onset (and to interrupting the persistence) of social isolation, and, in turn, slowing brain aging and reducing the occurrence of cognitive decline in later adulthood. In fact, a longitudinal intervention study of healthy adults already has reported that mental training improved cognitive and social skills and that this was correlated with MRI-based measures of increased cortical thickness (Valk et al., Reference Valk, Bernhardt, Trautwein, Böckler, Kanske, Guizard and Singer2017). Our finding that child-only social isolation did not contribute to older brain age bodes well for intervention strategy: the child brain appears to be robust and it is not too late to intervene in adolescence or adulthood.

Strengths and limitations

The main strengths of this study are the longitudinal assessment of social isolation from childhood to adulthood using trajectory modeling, and the measurement of brain structure for a large general population sample about which there have been detailed life-course phenotypic information collected. This enables not only the analysis of social isolation onset but also of any persistence across the life course. We were able to use an existing typology of four trajectory groups derived from child and adult measures of social isolation.

Limitations involve, first, that our heuristic social isolation measures were not designed specifically to measure social isolation and only cover one aspect of social isolation, i.e. social disconnectedness (Cornwell & Waite, Reference Cornwell and Waite2009), and did not assess loneliness, i.e. how the experience was perceived by participants (Perlman & Peplau, Reference Perlman, Peplau, Duck and Gilmour1981). Social isolation and loneliness have been shown to be distinct entities that are not highly correlated and may have independent effects on health outcomes (Perissinotto & Covinsky, Reference Perissinotto and Covinsky2014; Valtorta, Kanaan, Gilbody, & Hanratty, Reference Valtorta, Kanaan, Gilbody and Hanratty2016). A recent review concluded that both social isolation and loneliness affect cognitive health, though their effects may be mediated differently, i.e. by a lack of stimulation (in the former case) and depression (in the latter) (Cardona & Andrés, Reference Cardona and Andrés2023). Our social isolation measures have been associated with cardiovascular disease (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Harrington, Moffitt, Milne and Poulton2006), various age-related diseases (Danese et al., Reference Danese, Moffitt, Harrington, Milne, Polanczyk, Pariante and Caspi2009), and chronic inflammation (Lacey et al., Reference Lacey, Kumaria and Bartley2014), while our derived trajectory groups have been related to depression and suicide (Lay-Yee et al., Reference Lay-Yee, Matthews, Moffitt, Poulton, Caspi and Milne2023), and retinal neuronal thickness (Barrett-Young et al., Reference Barrett-Young, Ambler, Cheyne, Guiney, Kokaua, Tham and Poulton2023). Second, we have not accounted for confounding by factors contemporaneous with adulthood. Third, while we accounted for likely confounding factors from childhood, we cannot infer causality and so cannot rule out reciprocal effects, i.e. social isolation is associated with, but may not necessarily directly cause, changes in brain structure, while reverse causation may also operate. Fourth, while the estimated mean brain age gap for the child-adult group was the largest of all trajectory groups, even after controlling for potential confounding variables that were elevated in this group (Lay-Yee et al., Reference Lay-Yee, Matthews, Moffitt, Poulton, Caspi and Milne2021), small numbers (n = 31 in the child-adult group) may have hampered the detection of an association between persistent social isolation and brain age. Fifthly, we did not have the requisite data to cover adolescence, an important life stage. Finally, the findings derive from analyses of a predominantly ethnically European cohort in New Zealand so may not be generalizable to all populations.

Future research

This paper examines risk factor levels for groupings by social isolation that have an intrinsic longitudinal dimension. More sophisticated longitudinal analyses with the requisite data offer the possibility of assessing changes in brain age in tandem with changes in social isolation, especially in the transition from childhood to adulthood. Sharp inflection points during the life course – perhaps indicating sensitive or critical periods – could thus be identified where interventions could reasonably be targeted to good effect. Considering later circumstances that might be more relevant to an adult outcome would clarify the effect of social isolation trajectories. Further studies are required to examine the causal mechanism whereby social isolation affects brain health outcomes (Cené et al., Reference Cené, Beckie, Sims, Suglia, Aggarwal and Moise2022) or whether the relationship is stronger in the reverse direction.

Conclusion

This study shows that social isolation, particularly when occurring in adulthood, is associated with older estimated brain age in mid-adulthood. There are implications for the screening for and treatment of cognitive decline. Points of intervention during the life course are suggested by our findings, addressing the onset and persistence of social isolation to improve brain health, and thereby contributing to the prevention or mitigation of cognitive decline and its private and public burdens.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291723001964

Acknowledgements

This research has been made possible by a generous bequest from Dr Jeanette Crossley – we honor her lived experience, her courage in adversity, and her compassion in helping others. We are also grateful to the trustees – Dr Alison Oosterman and Jamie Thompson – for their faith in our work and standing beside us on our journey. We thank our colleagues Prof Terrie Moffitt and Prof Avshalom Caspi (Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC, United States), and Prof Richie Poulton (Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand) for their expert oversight of this research. We thank the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development (DMHD) Study members, their families and friends, teachers and peer informants for their long-term involvement, and Study Founder, Dr Phil A Silva, and DMHD Research Unit staff. The DMHD Research Unit is based at University of Otago within the tribal area of Ngāi Tahu whom we acknowledge as first peoples, tangata whenua (people of this land).

Financial support

The Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development (DMHD) Research Unit is supported by the NZ Health Research Council and has also received funding from the NZ Ministry of Business, Innovation and Employment. The DMHD Research Unit receives current funding from: NZ Health Research Council (program grant 16-604); US National Institute on Aging (grants RO1AG069939, R01AG032282, and R01AG049789); and UK Medical Research Council (grant MR/P005918/1). For the purpose of this specific project, RLY and BJM were funded by the Jeanette Crossley Foundation, a private charity.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical standards

The DMHDS has been approved by the NZ Health and Disability Ethics Committee, Ministry of Health, and written informed consent was obtained from participants.

References

Aleman, A., & Sommer, I. (2022). The silent danger of social distancing. Psychological Medicine, 52(4), 789790. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720002597CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arzate-Mejía, R. G., Lottenbach, Z., Schindler, V., Jawaid, A., & Mansuy, I. M. (2020). Long-term impact of social isolation and molecular underpinnings. Frontiers in Genetics, 11, 589621. doi: 10.3389/fgene.2020.589621CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnett, J., Salmond, C., Jones, P., & Sahakian, B. (2006). Cognitive reserve in neuropsychiatry. Psychological Medicine, 36(8), 10531064. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706007501CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett-Young, A., Ambler, A., Cheyne, K., Guiney, H., Kokaua, J., Tham, Y. C., … Poulton, R. (2023). Childhood social isolation as a predictor of retinal neuronal thickness in middle age: A lifecourse birth cohort study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 85(3), 238249. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000001177CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ben-Shlomo, Y., & Kuh, D. (2002). A life course approach to chronic disease epidemiology: Conceptual models, empirical challenges and interdisciplinary perspectives. International Journal of Epidemiology, 31(2), 285293. doi: 10.1093/ije/31.2.285CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berkman, L. F., Glass, T., Brissette, I., & Seeman, T. E. (2000). From social integration to health – Durkheim in the new millennium. Social Science & Medicine, 51, 843857. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(00)00065-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biddle, K. D., Uquillas, F. D. O., Jacobs, H. I., Zide, B., Kirn, D. R., Rentz, D. M., … Donovan, N. J. (2019). Social engagement and amyloid-Β-related cognitive decline in cognitively normal older adults. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 27, 12471256. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2019.05.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bu, F., Steptoe, A., & Fancourt, D. (2022). Longitudinal changes in home confinement and mental health implications: A 17-month follow-up study in England during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychological Medicine, 53(9), 19. doi: 10.1017/S0033291722000605Google ScholarPubMed
Bukowski, W. M., & Adams, R. (2005). Peer relationships and psychopathology: Markers, moderators, mediators, mechanisms, and meanings. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34, 310. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3401_1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bzdok, D., & Dunbar, R. (2022). Social isolation and the brain in the pandemic era. Nature Human Behaviour, 6(10), 13331343. doi: 10.1038/s41562-022-01453-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., Norman, G. J., & Berntson, G. G. (2011). Social isolation. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1231, 1722. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06028.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cardona, M., & Andrés, P. (2023). Are social isolation and loneliness associated with cognitive decline in ageing? Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 15, 1075563. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2023.1075563CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Harrington, H., Moffitt, T. E., Milne, B. J., & Poulton, R. (2006). Socially isolated children 20 years later: Risk of cardiovascular disease. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 160(8), 805811. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.160.8.805CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., … Poulton, R. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science (New York, N.Y.), 297(5582), 851854. doi: 10.1126/science.1072290CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cené, C. W., Beckie, T. M., Sims, M., Suglia, S. F., Aggarwal, B., Moise, N., … American Heart Association Social Determinants of Health Committee of the Council on Epidemiology and Prevention and Council on Quality of Care and Outcomes Research; Prevention Science Committee of the Council on Epidemiology and Prevention and Council on Cardiovascular and Stroke Nursing; Council on Arteriosclerosis, Thrombosis and Vascular Biology; and Stroke Council. (2022). Effects of objective and perceived social isolation on cardiovascular and brain health: A scientific statement from the American Heart Association. Journal of the American Heart Association, 11(16), e026493. doi: 10.1161/JAHA.122.026493CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cleeland, C., Pipingas, A., Scholey, A., & White, D. (2019). Neurochemical changes in the aging brain: A systematic review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 98, 306319. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.01.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coplan, R. J., Ooi, L. L., Xiao, B., & Rose-Krasnor, L. (2018). Assessment and implications of social withdrawal in early childhood. Social Development, 27, 125139. doi: 10.1111/sode.12258CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cornwell, E. Y., & Waite, L. J. (2009). Social disconnectedness, perceived isolation, and health among older adults. Journal of Health & Social Behavior, 50(1), 3148. doi: 10.1177/002214650905000103CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danese, A., Moffitt, T. E., Harrington, H., Milne, B. J., Polanczyk, G., Pariante, C. M., … Caspi, A. (2009). Adverse childhood experiences and adult risk factors for age-related disease: Depression, inflammation, and clustering of metabolic risk markers. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 163(12), 11351143. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.214CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Jong Gierveld, J., van Tilburg, T. G., & Dykstra, P. A. (2006). Loneliness and social isolation. In Vangelisti, A. & Perlman, D. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of personal relationships (2nd ed.), (pp. 485500). Cambridge UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Lange, A. G., Kaufmann, T., Quintana, D. S., Winterton, A., Andreassen, O. A., Westlye, L. T., & Ebmeier, K. P. (2021). Prominent health problems, socioeconomic deprivation, and higher brain age in lonely and isolated individuals: A population-based study. Behavioural Brain Research, 414, 113510. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2021.113510CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desai, R., John, A., Stott, J., & Charlesworth, G. (2020). Living alone and risk of dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Research Reviews, 62, 101122. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2020.101122CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Di Marco, L. Y., Marzo, A., Munoz-Ruiz, M., Ikram, M. A., Kivipelto, M., Ruefenacht, D., … Ventikos, Y. A. (2014). Modifiable lifestyle factors in dementia: A systematic review of longitudinal observational cohort studies. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 42, 119135. doi: 10.3233/JAD-132225CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drinkwater, E., Davies, C., & Spires-Jones, T. L. (2021). Potential neurobiological links between social isolation and Alzheimer's disease risk. European Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 670959. doi: 10.1111/ejn.15373Google Scholar
Eavani, H., Habes, M., Satterthwaite, T. D., An, Y., Hsieh, M. K., Honnorat, N., … Davatzikos, C. (2018). Heterogeneity of structural and functional imaging patterns of advanced brain aging revealed via machine learning methods. Neurobiology of Aging, 71, 4150. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.06.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberger, N. I., & Cole, S. W. (2012). Social neuroscience and health: Neurophysiological mechanisms linking social ties with physical health. Nature Neuroscience, 15(5), 669674. doi: 10.1038/nn.3086CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elander, J., & Rutter, M. (1996). Use and development of the Rutter parents' and teachers' scale. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 6(2), 6378.3.3.CO;2-M>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elley, W. B., & Irving, J. C. (1972). A socio-economic index for New Zealand based on levels of education and income from the 1966 census. New Zealand Journal of Educational Studies, 7, 153167.Google Scholar
Elliott, M. L., Belsky, D. W., Knodt, A. R., Ireland, D., Melzer, T. R., Poulton, R., … Hariri, A. R. (2021). Brain-age in midlife is associated with accelerated biological aging and cognitive decline in a longitudinal birth cohort. Molecular Psychiatry, 26(8), 38293838. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0626-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, I. E., Llewellyn, D. J., Matthews, F. E., Woods, R. T., Brayne, C., Clare, L., & CFAS-Wales research team. (2018). Social isolation, cognitive reserve, and cognition in healthy older people. PLoS ONE, 13(8), E0201008. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.02010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, I. E., Martyr, A., Collins, R., Brayne, C., & Clare, L. (2019). Social isolation and cognitive function in later life: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 70(s1), S119S144. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180501CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franke, K., & Gaser, C. (2012). Longitudinal changes in individual BrainAGE in healthy aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Gerontopsychology and Geriatric Psychiatry, 25(4), 235245. doi: 10.1024/1662-9647/a000074Google Scholar
Franke, K., & Gaser, C. (2019). Ten years of BrainAGE as a neuroimaging biomarker of brain aging: What insights have we gained? Frontiers in Neurology, 10, 789. doi: 10.3389/fneur.201900789CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaser, C., Franke, K., Klöppel, S., Koutsouleris, N., & Sauer, H. (2013). BrainAGE in mild cognitive impaired patients: Predicting the conversion to Alzheimer's disease. PLoS ONE, 8(6), e67346. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067346CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glasser, M. F., Sotiropoulos, S. N., Wilson, J. A., Coalson, T. S., Fischl, B., Andersson, J. L., … WU-Minn HCP Consortium. (2013). The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. NeuroImage, 80, 105124. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.127CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holt-Lunstad, J., Robles, T. F., & Sbarra, D. A. (2017). Advancing social connection as a public health priority in the United States. The American Psychologist, 72(6), 517530. doi: 10.1037/amp0000103CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., Baker, M., Harris, T., & Stephenson, D. (2015). Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: A meta-analytic review. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(2), 227237. doi: 10.1177/1745691614568352CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holt-Lunstad, J., & Steptoe, A. (2022). Social isolation: An underappreciated determinant of physical health. Current Opinion in Psychology, 43, 232237. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.07.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ishikawa, K. M., Davis, J., Chen, J. J., & Lim, E. (2022). The prevalence of mild cognitive impairment by aspects of social isolation. PloS ONE, 17(6), e0269795. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0269795CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jellinger, K. A., & Attems, J. (2013). Neuropathological approaches to cerebral aging and neuroplasticity. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 15(1), 2943. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2013.15.1/kjellingerCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelly, M. E., Duff, H., Kelly, S., McHugh Power, J. E., Brennan, S., Lawlor, B. A., & Loughrey, D. G. (2017). The impact of social activities, social networks, social support and social relationships on the cognitive function of healthy older adults: A systematic review. Systematic Reviews, 6(1), 259. doi: 10.1186/s13643-017-0632-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuiper, J. S., Zuidersma, M., Oude Voshaar, R. C., Zuidema, S. U., van den Heuvel, E. R., Stolk, R. P., & Smidt, N. (2015). Social relationships and risk of dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. Ageing Research Reviews, 22, 3957. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2015.04.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lacey, R. E., Kumaria, M., & Bartley, M. (2014). Social isolation in childhood and adult inflammation: Evidence from the National Child Development Study. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 50, 8594. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.08.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lay-Yee, R., Matthews, T., Moffitt, T., Poulton, R., Caspi, A., & Milne, B. (2023). Are trajectories of social isolation from childhood to mid-adulthood associated with adult depression or suicide outcomes. Social Psychiatry & Psychiatric Epidemiology, 58, 373382. doi: 10.1007/s00127-022-02389-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lay-Yee, R., Matthews, T., Moffitt, T. E., Poulton, R., Caspi, A., & Milne, B. J. (2021). Do socially isolated children become socially isolated adults? Advances in Life Course Research, 50, 100419. doi: 10.1016/j.alcr.2021.100419CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leigh-Hunt, N., Bagguley, D., Bash, K., Turner, V., Turnbull, S., Valtorta, N., & Caan, W. (2017). Overview of systematic reviews on the public health consequences of social isolation and loneliness. Public Health, 152(157e), 171. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2017.07.035CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liem, F., Varoquaux, G., Kynast, J., Beyer, F., Kharabian Masouleh, S., Huntenburg, J. M., … Margulies, D. S. (2017). Predicting brain-age from multimodal imaging data captures cognitive impairment. NeuroImage, 148, 179188. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacDonald, M. E., & Pike, G. B. (2021). MRI of healthy brain aging: A review. NMR in Biomedicine, 34(9), e4564. doi: 10.1002/nbm.4564CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marryat, L., Thompson, L., Minnis, H., & Wilson, P. (2014). Associations between social isolation, pro-social behaviour and emotional development in preschool aged children: A population based survey of kindergarten staff. BMC Psychology, 2, 44. doi: 10.1186/s40359-014-0044-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, T., Qualter, P., Bryan, B. T., Caspi, A., Danese, A., Moffitt, T. E., … Arseneault, L. (2022). The developmental course of loneliness in adolescence: Implications for mental health, educational attainment, and psychosocial functioning. Development and Psychopathology 35(2), 537546. doi: 10.1017/S0954579421001632CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., … Caspi, A. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 26932698. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010076108CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morese, R., & Palermo, S. (2022). Feelings of loneliness and isolation: Social brain and social cognition in the elderly and Alzheimer's disease. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 14, 896218. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.896218CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nagin, D. S. (2005). Group-based modeling of development. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osborn, T., Weatherburn, P., & French, R. S. (2021). Interventions to address loneliness and social isolation in young people: A systematic review of the evidence on acceptability and effectiveness. Journal of Adolescence, 93, 5379. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2021.09.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penninkilampi, R., Casey, A.-N., Singh, M. F., & Brodaty, H. (2018). The association between social engagement, loneliness, and risk of dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 66(4), 115. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180439CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perissinotto, C. M., & Covinsky, K. E. (2014). Living alone, socially isolated or lonely – what are we measuring? Journal of General Internal Medicine, 11, 14291431. doi: 10.1007/s11606-014-2977-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perlman, D., & Peplau, L. A. (1981). Toward a social psychology of loneliness. In Duck, S. & Gilmour, R. (Eds.), Personal relationships in disorder (pp. 3156). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Poulton, R., Guiney, H., Ramrakha, S., & Moffitt, T. E. (2022). The Dunedin study after half a century: Reflections on the past, and course for the future. Journal of the Royal Society of New Zealand 53(4), 446465. doi: 10.1080/03036758.2022.2114508CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poulton, R., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (2015). The Dunedin multidisciplinary health and development study: Overview of the first 40 years, with an eye to the future. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 50(5), 679693. doi: 10.1007/s00127-015-1048-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Read, S., Comas-Herrera, A., & Grundy, E. (2020). Social isolation and memory decline in later-life. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 75(2), 367376. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbz152CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ren, Y., Savadlou, A., Park, S., Siska, P., Epp, J. R., & Sargin, D. (2023). The impact of loneliness and social isolation on the development of cognitive decline and Alzheimer's disease. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 69, 101061. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2023.101061CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Repetti, R. L., Taylor, S. E., & Seeman, T. E. (2002). Risky families: Family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), 330366. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.128.2.330CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reuben, A., Moffitt, T. E., Abraham, W. C., Ambler, A., Elliott, M. L., Hariri, A. R., … Caspi, A. (2022). Improving risk indexes for Alzheimer's disease and related dementias for use in midlife. Brain Communications, 4(5), fcac223. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcac223CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richmond-Rakerd, L. S., Caspi, A., Ambler, A., d'Arbeloff, T., de Bruine, M., Elliott, M., … Moffitt, T. E. (2021). Childhood self-control forecasts the pace of midlife aging and preparedness for old age. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(3), e2010211118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2010211118CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rohr, S., Wittmann, F., Engel, C., Enzenbach, C., Witte, A. V., Villringer, A., … Riedel-Heller, S. G. (2021). Social factors and the prevalence of social isolation in a population-based adult cohort. Social Psychiatry & Psychiatric Epidemiology, 57(10), 19591968. doi: 10.1007/s00127-021-02174-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rojas, Y. (2018). Long-term suicidogenic effect of being mostly alone as a child in a Stockholm birth cohort - restating the role of social isolation in suicide. Suicidology Online, 9(5), 113. http://www.suicidology-online.com/pdf/SOL-2018-9-5.pdf.Google Scholar
Rudenstine, S., & Galea, S. (2015). Preventing brain disorders: A framework for action. Social Psychiatry & Psychiatric Epidemiology, 50, 833841. doi: 10.1007/s00127-015-1007-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samtani, S., Mahalingam, G., Lam, B., Lipnicki, D. M., Lima-Costa, M. F., Blay, S. L., … SHARED consortium for the Cohort Studies of Memory in an International Consortium (COSMIC). (2022). Associations between social connections and cognition: A global collaborative individual participant data meta-analysis. The Lancet. Healthy Longevity 3(11), 740753. doi: 10.1016/S2666-7568(22)00199-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, B. H., Richard, J. F., Younger, A. J., & Freeman, P. (2000). A longitudinal exploration of the continuity of children's social participation and social withdrawal across socioeconomic status levels and social settings. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30(4), 497519. doi: 10.1002/1099-0992(200007/08)30:4<497::AID-EJSP4>3.0.CO;2-03.0.CO;2-0>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shen, C., Rolls, E., Cheng, W., Kang, J., Dong, G., Xie, C., … Feng, J. (2022). Associations of social isolation and loneliness with later dementia. Neurology, 99(2), e164e175. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000200583CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S. M., Vidaurre, D., Alfaro-Almagro, F., Nichols, T. E., & Miller, K. L. (2019). Estimation of brain age delta from brain imaging. NeuroImage, 200, 528539. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.06.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sommerlad, A., Marston, L., Huntley, J., Livingston, G., Lewis, G., Steptoe, A., & Fancourt, D. (2022). Social relationships and depression during the COVID-19 lockdown: Longitudinal analysis of the COVID-19 Social Study. Psychological Medicine, 52(15), 33813390. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721000039CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sommerlad, A., Ruegger, J., Singh-Manoux, A., Lewis, G., & Livingston, G. (2018). Marriage and risk of dementia: Systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 89, 231238. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2017-316274CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
StataCorp (2019). Stata statistical software: Release 16. College Station, TX: StataCorp LLC.Google Scholar
Umberson, D., Crosnoe, R., & Reczek, C. (2010). Social relationships and health behavior across life course. Annual Review of Sociology, 36, 139157. doi: 10.1146/annurev-soc-070308-120011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Umberson, D., & Donnelly, R. (2023). Social isolation: An unequally distributed health hazard. Annual Review of Sociology, 49, 121. doi: 10.1146/annurev-soc-031021-012001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Umberson, D., Lin, Z., & Cha, H. (2022). Gender and social isolation across the life course. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 63(3), 319335. doi: 10.1177/00221465221109634CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Umberson, D., & Montez, J. K. (2010). Social relationships and health: A flashpoint for health policy. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 51(Suppl), S54S66. doi: 10.1177/0022146510383501CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valk, S. L., Bernhardt, B. C., Trautwein, F. M., Böckler, A., Kanske, P., Guizard, N., … Singer, T. (2017). Structural plasticity of the social brain: Differential change after socio-affective and cognitive mental training. Science Advances, 3(10), e1700489. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1700489CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valtorta, N. K., Kanaan, M., Gilbody, S., & Hanratty, B. (2016). Loneliness, social isolation and social relationships: What are we measuring? A novel framework for classifying and comparing tools. BMJ Open, 6(4), e010799. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-010799CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vidal-Pineiro, D., Wang, Y., Krogsrud, S. K., Amlien, I. K., Baaré, W. F. C., Bartres-Faz, D., … Fjell, A. (2021). Individual variations in ‘brain age’ relate to early-life factors more than to longitudinal brain change. eLife, 10, e69995. doi: 10.7554/eLife.69995CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, J., Knol, M. J., Tiulpin, A., Dubost, F., de Bruijne, M., Vernooij, M. W., … Roshchupkin, G. V. (2019). Gray matter age prediction as a biomarker for risk of dementia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(42), 2121321218. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1902376116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zheng, Y., Habes, M., Gonzales, M., Pomponio, R., Nasrallah, I., Khan, S., … Hou, L. (2022). Mid-life epigenetic age, neuroimaging brain age, and cognitive function: Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study. Aging, 14(4), 16911712. doi: 10.18632/aging.203918CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Brain age gap estimate (brainAGE) at age 45 years by ‘trajectory’ group and family-child factors

Figure 1

Table 2. Relationship between ‘trajectory’ group and brain age gap estimate (brainAGE) at age 45 years

Supplementary material: File

Lay-Yee et al. supplementary material
Download undefined(File)
File 13.5 KB