Next Article in Journal
The Effect of Silymarin Flavonolignans and Their Sulfated Conjugates on Platelet Aggregation and Blood Vessels Ex Vivo
Next Article in Special Issue
Dietary Fiber, Whole Grains, and Head and Neck Cancer Prognosis: Findings from a Prospective Cohort Study
Previous Article in Journal
Obesity, a Diet-Induced Inflammatory Disease
Previous Article in Special Issue
Serum ‘Vitamin-Mineral’ Profiles: Associations with Postmenopausal Breast Cancer Risk Including Dietary Patterns and Supplementation. A Case-Control Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Chemosensory Changes from Cancer Treatment and Their Effects on Patients’ Food Behavior: A Scoping Review

1
Food Science Department, College of Natural Sciences, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA 01003, USA
2
Monell Chemical Senses Center, 3500 Market Street, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA
3
The University of Queensland Diamantina Institute, The University of Queensland, Woolloongabba, Queensland 4102, Australia
4
Victorian Comprehensive Cancer Centre, Melbourne 3000, Australia
5
Melbourne School of Population and Global Health, The University of Melbourne, Parkville 3053, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2019, 11(10), 2285; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11102285
Submission received: 28 August 2019 / Revised: 18 September 2019 / Accepted: 18 September 2019 / Published: 24 September 2019
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Nutrition and Cancer: From Prevention to Survivorship)

Abstract

:
Individuals undergoing treatment for cancer can experience changes in taste or smell that are often assumed to affect constructs related to food behavior, although this relationship is rarely measured directly. To ascertain the extent to which measured changes in taste and smell during and after cancer treatment affect food behavior, we conducted a scoping review and completed a comparative analysis for studies that met our criteria, which were: they directly measured cancer patients’ (a) psychophysical response to taste and/or olfactory stimuli, and (b) food behavior (including food enjoyment, food preference, dietary intake) in people affected by cancer. Eleven studies met these criteria and were included in the review. All 11 studies evaluated taste and five also measured smell. A comparative analysis exploring taste and food behavior shows that a reduced sweet taste function (decreased sensitivity) was associated with a reduced intake of a variety of different macro and micro nutrients, reduced appetite, and overall lower energy intake. One out of six studies that measured smell and food measured observed changes in olfactory function following cancer treatment. There were no significant relationships reported between olfactory measures and food behavior. Taste changes that arise from cancer treatment appear to have a direct effect on food behavior, although there is a need for more research using standardized measures and larger sample sizes. A better understanding of taste alterations and their implications for dietary intake and food enjoyment will support optimal nutritional health by identifying strategies to help patients eat well during and after cancer treatment.

1. Introduction

According to the National Cancer Institute, in the United States, one in three adults are diagnosed with cancer (based on 2013–2015 data) [1]. The decline in the cancer death rate (26% reduction between 1991 and 2015) [1], is resulting in a larger number of individuals surviving cancer, with the number of cancer survivors expected to rise to 20.3 million by 2026 [2].
Many people receiving cancer treatment complain of changes to their taste and smell functions, presenting as various manifestations, though routinely described as a loss or alteration of perception [3]. Prevalence of self-reported taste problems among cancer patients ranges from 12% to 84% [4,5,6]. Taste and smell play an important role in food behavior, so changes in taste or smell perception may negatively influence eating behavior [7,8,9]. “Food behavior” is an inclusive term to capture any type of food-related behavior that might have real-world significance for patients’ overall nutritional health [10], such as the desire to eat, food preferences, and food intake, in ways that lead to weight change and poor quality of life [11,12,13,14]. Several studies have investigated cancer-related changes in chemosensation (i.e., taste and smell) or food-related behavior; however, it is rare for a study to use both measures.
There is a wide variability in the prevalence rates for reported taste function (12% to 84% [4,5,6]). There are both clinical and demographic patient characteristics that could have an impact on taste function. The various treatment regimens and cancer types studied, the different methods used to evaluate taste [3,15,16], how “taste” was defined in these studies, and the end points examined, all effect the study results. For example, some authors describe “taste” as a study end point but do not assess taste per se. Other patient characteristics, such as smoking history, alcohol abuse, and genetics, have all known to play a role in individual variability in taste perception.
This review critically evaluates research published from 1982 to 2018 that psychophysically measured taste and smell function and assessed some aspect of food behavior (e.g., appetite, dietary intake, and preference). We reviewed studies that used objective measures of taste and smell function (i.e., using psychophysical methods), as opposed to self-reported changes in taste or smell. The rationale for this selection strategy was based on the observation that patients often do not distinguish between taste and smell or confuse sensory constructs with hedonic constructs, such as appetite or food enjoyment, so self-reported changes are often unrelated to objectively measured taste changes [17,18]. For example, a food may taste the same as it always did but that taste is no longer enjoyed.

2. Methods

2.1. Study Selection

Our focus on taste and food behavior in people with cancer posed several challenges in systematically reviewing the literature because no standard search terms returned appropriate studies. We therefore took a two-pronged approach: First, we started with the most current published papers and traced citations back to 1961, the earliest paper reporting on taste psychophysics of cancer patients. This paper from 1961 served as the starting point of the period covered by the review. Second, we included the broad terms “taste” and “smell” (i.e., taste, smell, olfaction, chemosensory), “objective evaluation” (i.e., threshold, detection threshold, recognition threshold, sensitivity, intensity, identification), “food behavior” (i.e., dietary food intake, food preference, food behavior, appetite, food liking), “human,” and “cancer” or “chemotherapy” and read the abstracts to identify relevant papers. Papers were removed that did not use methods that objectively measured taste and/or smell function or did not include measures of food behavior. Papers were not excluded based on study design (e.g., cohort or longitudinal), cancer diagnosis, treatment type, or time since cancer diagnosis or treatment. Papers that met the above criteria were removed if articles were not written in English, included children (<18 years old), or were not original research publications. Database searches were carried out in PubMed, Web of Science, Google Scholar, and from the University of Pennsylvania and University of Massachusetts Libraries. Two authors reviewed the abstracts of the unique original research articles from the resulting literature searches (n = 44), and additional articles were identified through reference searches and searches of similar articles, with 11 articles fulfilling the criteria.

2.2. Data Extraction

Data from selected articles were manually extracted, including sample size, experimental design, methods of sensory measurement, food behavior metrics, and summary statistical statistics.

2.3. Statistical Approach for Comparative Analysis

Three of the eleven articles were included in the comparative analysis due to eight of the eleven articles having insufficient data available for extraction. Means and standard deviations of the comparison groups, test statistics, and other metrics for the comparative analysis were obtained where possible. We calculated the effect sizes with the d statistic using previous methods [19].

3. Results

3.1. Study Characteristics and Design

The characteristics of the 11 studies included in the review and comparative analysis are summarized in Table 1. The studies included had a combined total of 578 participants, comprising 380 people with cancer and 198 controls. Five studies enrolled patients with more than one cancer type and six enrolled patients with a single cancer type. Cancer diagnoses varied across studies, including breast, esophagogastric, lung, ovarian, prostate, colon, multiple myeloma, lymphoma, and testicular cancer, with one study not reporting a cancer type.

3.2. Methods of Taste and Smell Assessment

A variety of objective measures were used to evaluate taste and smell perception. For taste, procedures included detection threshold (DT) and recognition threshold (RT). DT is the lowest concentration that a taste stimulus can be detected compared to the background (i.e., different than water), with RT referring to the lowest concentration a specific taste can be recognized (e.g., bitter). Other taste methods included taste identification tasks evaluating the suprathreshold stimuli (e.g., International Organization for Standardization (ISO) 3972:2011) intensity rating of suprathreshold stimuli (concentration above DT), and ranking tasks. Of note, the detection threshold occurs at a lower concentration than the recognition threshold. Most studies reported stimuli being presented at room temperature with participants having time between stimuli where water was available for rinsing. The studies also differed in the way investigators presented smell and taste stimuli to subjects. For taste, stimuli were presented to either the whole mouth (whole-mouth rinses, sprays) or only parts of the mouth (filter paper disks, taste strips). For smell, most of the studies used Sniffin’ Sticks (a set of pen-like devices that dispense an odor) to make three measures of olfaction function: identification, sensitivity (DT), and discrimination [29]. Table 2 summarizes the methods used to evaluate taste and smell. For a general review of methods evaluating taste and smell function in oncology populations, see References [30,31,32,33]; for more methodological detail, see References [34,35]; and for clinical applications for dietitians, see Boltong and Campbell [36].
A variety of different methods were used to assess food behavior across studies. Eight studies included measures of dietary intake, assessed using either a dietary recall (n = 2), a food frequency questionnaire (n = 5), or a 3-day weighed food technique (n = 1). Participants answered questions regarding symptoms related to appetite (n = 5); however, only two studies analyzed the relationship between chemosensory measures and appetite. Four studies measured food preferences using responses to food pictures with different macronutrient content.

3.3. Impact of Taste Changes on Food Intake and Preferences

When comparing the experimental results between taste and food behavior for each type of study design, i.e., cross-sectional and prospective longitudinal, the results were mixed. Of the cross-sectional cohort studies (including one combined cross-sectional and longitudinal study), several reported a significant relationship between taste function and food behaviors. Investigators reported that cancer patients with a loss of appetite were more likely to prefer lower sweetness levels and a reduced intake of sweet foods compared with patients without a reduced appetite [21]. In another study, changes in bitter DT correlated with the avoidance of meat, chocolate, and tea [21]. In a third study, changes in bitter DT correlated with the avoidance of meat, chocolate, and tea; however, the direction (i.e., increased or decreased bitter sensitivity) was not reported [23]. In a fourth study, patients with a higher sucrose DT consumed less protein, carbohydrate, zinc, and fewer calories. In the same study, patients with a higher urea RT reported consuming fewer calories and less protein, carbohydrate, and fat. Sucrose and urea thresholds were also associated with energy intake: a greater percentage of patients who did not meet energy requirements had thresholds above the median for sucrose (DT and RT) and urea (RT) [24], meaning they found it harder to recognize sweet and bitter tastes. The one cross-sectional cohort study did not report a significant relationship between taste function and food behavior in those who had cancer but were in remission and no longer undergoing treatment (cancer survivors; 1–7 years post-treatment), even though changes in taste function were apparent in this group [18].
Among the seven prospective longitudinal studies (including one cross-sectional longitudinal study using a combined approach), three reported a significant relationship between taste and food behavior. In one of these three studies, the reduced ability to identify taste solutions was associated with decreased energy intake during the beginning of the third chemotherapy treatment cycle [25]. In a second study, patients who became more sensitive to sucrose between the start of chemotherapy and after two cycles of chemotherapy consumed fewer grams of protein, animal protein, fat, and iron compared to those who became less sensitive to sucrose [26]. The third study also found a relationship between sweet sensitivity and food behavior, reporting that decreased sucrose sensitivity was associated with reduced appetite prior to treatment [20].
Of the four prospective longitudinal studies that found no taste effects on food behavior, one showed that changes in sensitivity (as measured by the ability to detect electrical stimulation of the tongue) was not associated with changes in chemotherapy patients’ protein or energy intake [22]. In two studies, recognition thresholds did not correlate with food preferences [28] or with the liking of macronutrient categories [17]. The fourth study measured recognition thresholds and collected data on food frequency intake and food preferences but did not report on associations between objective taste and food measures [27].
We computed effect sizes from the studies listed in Table 1. Where possible, we computed these values and report them as d (standardized mean difference; Table 3) to assess the magnitude of taste changes, allowing for the comparison of findings across studies. However, 5 of the 11 studies reported no significant effects, and 3 of the 6 studies with significant effects reported too little information to allow us to compute effect sizes. Therefore, only three of the studies were included in the effect size calculations, and thus conclusions should be drawn cautiously. However, the effect sizes revealed that sweet taste has the most empirical evidence for involvement with food behavior, with greater evidence supporting this link. There was some but limited evidence that bitter taste was involved with measures of nutrient intake. Almost all studies measured taste changes for four taste qualities, sweet (n = 10), sour (n = 8), bitter (n = 10), and salty (n = 8), with fewer reporting responses to umami (n = 3); but positive results and reportable effect sizes were mostly for sweet taste.

3.4. Impact of Smell Changes on Food Intake and Preferences

Five studies measured smell function; three of the five studies reported no significant change among subjects affected by cancer [18,22,28]. One longitudinal study reported significant differences (poorer smell function) for overall olfactory scores and for threshold olfactory scores (but not discrimination or identification tasks) during chemotherapy compared with controls [17]. A prospective cohort study of testicular cancer patients reported lower smell thresholds (greater smell sensitivity) compared with controls at baseline, with no differences at any other time point (through 1-year post-treatment) [27]. Another longitudinal study reported just under 50% (13 of 27) of patients had changed smell detection thresholds (compared to baseline): almost equal numbers of participants experienced increased sensitivity (n = 6) and decreased sensitivity (n = 7) [22]; no other analysis was reported. No comparative analysis was performed as none of these studies report an instance where smell alterations were significantly related to any measures of food behaviors.

4. Discussion

4.1. Taste but Not Smell May Influence Food Intake and Enjoyment

The studies reviewed here suggest that cancer patients may suffer from reduced food intake and appetite, which are associated with alterations in taste perception. Specifically, reduced taste perception may result in a decreased intake of fat and protein, leading to an overall decrease in energy consumption (Table 3). These data suggest that patients suffering from reduced taste sensitivity (i.e., higher DT) experience greater incidences of food avoidance. Among the 11 research articles reviewed, six reported no significant relationship between measures of taste (or smell) and food-related behaviors; none of which supported a relationship between smell function and food behavior.

4.2. The Special Role of Sweet Taste Changes and Food Behavior in Cancer Patients

We performed a comparative analysis and learned that changes in sweet and, to a lesser extent, bitter perception were more common than changes to salt or sour perception in cancer patients, and that these changes in sweet taste perception were often tied to differences in food behaviors (Table 3). The effect sizes, reported in Table 3, allow for the comparison across the studies reporting a significant relationship between taste and food behavior. The greatest effect size reported was between sweet sensitivity and zinc intake, with individuals having a greater sensitivity for sweetness (i.e., low DT) associated with consuming more zinc, compared to those with a lower sensitivity for sweetness (i.e., high DT). For bitterness, the highest effect size was observed for the daily consumption of calories, with individuals with a higher DT (lower sensitivity) consuming on average 631 fewer calories compared to individuals with a lower DT (greater sensitivity). Reduced sweet taste function (decreased sensitivity) was associated with a reduced intake of a variety of different macro and micro nutrients, reduced appetite, and overall lower energy intake. This suggests that reduced taste function, specifically for sweetness, reduces patient’s food intake.
There are a few mechanisms in which cancer treatment may affect taste and ultimately food behavior. It is plausible that sweet taste receptors or cells are more vulnerable to cancer treatment or that neural response to sweet ligands is altered as a result of cancer treatment. Another consideration is that taste receptors are also expressed in the gut [37] and may be affected in a similar mechanism as oral taste cells during chemotherapy treatment. Therefore, changes in food behavior may be a result of gastrointestinal symptoms that co-occur with taste alterations [38,39]. While caution is warranted in drawing conclusions from this comparative analysis because of the small number of studies, it is clear that these perceptual changes warrant more study.
Taste and smell loss related to cancer treatment is thought to arise as a result of the cancer treatment [40]; yet individuals may experience taste and smell changes prior to treatment as a result of cancer [32]. Each type of cancer treatment may affect taste and smell through different mechanisms [16]. For example, taste loss during chemotherapy may be due to the disruption of taste cell renewal [41,42], whereas surgery in the oral cavity could damage chemosensory nerves [43].

4.3. Contrasting Study Designs, Patient Populations, and Methodologies

Cancer treatment is continually making advancements, so as technology and precision medicine advances, toxicities may differ. For example, in the treatment of head and neck cancers, proton radiation has a smaller radiation field, and thus may better preserve gustatory tissue when compared to conventional photon radiation treatment [44]. While it is likely that treatment characteristics and cancer diagnosis may differentially effect taste perception, conclusions cannot be drawn here about the effects of specific treatments on taste and food behavior because of both the diversity of treatments and the limited treatment information provided. Other aspects that could explain the conflicting results are the mix of study designs (see Table 1) and patient population. Diverse patient populations were used across studies, with six studies studied a single cancer type, three studies involved patients from two or more cancer type sub groups, and two studies did not disclose cancer diagnosis. Clinical and demographic characteristics, such as gender, age, smoking history, upper airway infections, and drug use, among other factors, are known to impact chemosensory function; however, these data were reported infrequently and could have had a significant impact on the study outcomes and their interpretations. Moreover, occurrence and severity of treatment-related symptoms, such as dysphagia, dry mouth, and nausea, among others, can negatively impact food behavior [36]. More work is needed to fully understand the implications of co-occurrence of these symptoms on food behavior and overall nutrition. Data and analyses were reported for the study population, regardless of the number of treatment types or cancer diagnoses, making it difficult to conduct a meaningful analysis to examine these clinical characteristics and their association with or influence on taste alterations and food behavior.
As summarized in Table 2, different methods were used to evaluate chemosensory function, which included variations in the taste stimulus delivery method. The authors of several studies did not report key information about the testing procedure, such as the concentration of the taste stimuli, and these methodological details may have contributed to the range of study results. These different methods allow stimuli to contact different areas of the oral cavity and tongue (e.g., sensitivity on the posterior versus anterior tongue), and the delivery method may influence taste perception [45,46]. Because different areas are innervated by different cranial nerves, they may be differentially impacted by treatment [43,47]. Food-related measures also varied across studies. For those evaluating intake, all studies used either food-frequency questionnaires or dietary recalls (24-h or 3-day recalls), with the exception of one weighed food study [23]. Evaluation of food preferences in the four studies used pictures of foods that were representative of portion sizes and different macronutrients [17,18,27,28], developed as a measure of food preference based on taste and macronutrient categories [48]. Studies using this method had mixed results for changes in food preferences during treatment [17,28,49], and were not associated with objective taste and smell measures [17,18,27,49]. These methods are not without limitations and may not accurately represent daily intake. Furthermore, food intake was not always normalized for body weight or accounted for recent weight history. This can be challenging as recommended caloric intake during cancer treatment may not be reflective their current body weight, with clinicians modifying patients’ diet to promote weight loss or maintain weight. Overall, it is recommended that more objective measures be used to quantify food intake [50], notwithstanding the practical barriers and participant burden inherent in doing so.

4.4. Individual Differences and Genetic Background

Some of the different findings among studies might be due to variation arising from individual differences in taste perception. A well-studied phenomenon is the wide variation across individuals in response to the bitterness of propylthiouracil (PTU or PROP) and phenylthiocarbamide (PTC) [51,52,53]. Some of the variation in response to these bitter compounds and others is linked to genetic differences in bitter taste receptor genes (TAS2Rs) [54,55,56,57,58,59,60]. Individual differences in perception are associated with differences in the liking and intake of foods [60,61,62,63,64]. This points to the importance of individual differences in perception that may result from genetic differences [59,65] (see Hayes et al. [66]) or dietary exposure [67,68,69,70], and is why longitudinal studies are preferred, where the patients act as their own control. Indeed, there appears to be a link between the genetics of a bitter taste receptor (TAS2R38) and the risk of developing cancer; however, the direction of this relationship is unclear [71,72,73]; although the direct pathophysiology is not known, it is likely driven by factors other than differences in vegetable and fruit intake [74]. Taken together, greater attention to differences across individuals arising from taste genotypes is warranted regarding taste studies.

4.5. Translation to Real-World Food Interactions and Clinical Applications

The goal of this review was to determine whether alterations in chemosensory function are associated with changes in food behavior, including consumption, preference, and enjoyment, in cancer patients. However, we must consider how changes in taste thresholds (DT or RT) translate to how we experience food. Threshold measures are variable and subject to false positives [75], and do not correlate with suprathreshold ratings or hedonic response [33,76,77]. However, the intensity and hedonic response of suprathreshold stimuli has been associated with the intake of a variety of foods and beverages [67,68,78,79]. Of the eleven studies included in this review, three presented taste stimuli at suprathreshold levels [17,21,25], of these studies, two reported a significant relationship with reduced energy intake [21,25], and another reported no relationship with food preference [17]. Selection of the psychophysical sensory methods is important and the translation of these measures to food behavior and nutritional status must be taken into consideration. More studies are needed to evaluate taste function above the detection threshold and how changes in taste perception mediate dietary intake and food behavior.
Patients should receive information on taste changes prior to treatment; this requires the proper training of clinicians such that they are equipped with validated evaluation measures. However, there is no such rapid detection or diagnosis tool that can be administered in a clinical setting. Currently, there are no clinical guidelines on the assessment or strategies for managing or improving taste problems. Future work will help to identify whether patient or clinical characteristics are associated with an increased risk for developing taste alterations, with more work needed to explore the long-term effects of cancer treatment, whether taste fully recovers, or whether there are long-lasting and persistent alterations. Another area that needs attention is the taxonomies used to describe alterations in taste function [15], with disparities being present in the use of the classifications across disciplines. Overall, more work is needed make diagnostic tools available for clinicians and information available for patients, along with identifying strategies for the treatment of taste disorders.

5. Conclusions

Taste and smell function related to food behavior in people affected by cancer is understudied and the mix of positive and negative results may be due to the mix of measurement methods and experimental designs [3,15,32,80]. However, we can say that among the studies with statistically significant associations between chemosensory function and food behavior (5 out of the 11 reviewed here), reduced taste function, particularly for sweet stimuli, was associated with a reduced appetite, avoidance of certain foods (e.g., meat), and reduced consumption (e.g., overall calories and protein). Additional studies that use a consistent methodology will better elucidate the duration and extent to which taste and smell alterations impact patient outcome.
The results of laboratory studies investigating taste and food behavior may have practical implications. Clinicians lack standardized tools with a high clinical utility to routinely assess and treat taste and smell disorders in oncology patients. A clearer understanding of the effect of taste and smell alterations on elements of food behavior may support the design and testing of clinical strategies and interventional approaches aimed at mitigating the deleterious effects of chemosensory dysfunction as a result of cancer treatment.

Author Contributions

The authors’ responsibilities were as follows—A.A.N.: conception and drafting of the manuscript; A.A.N. and D.R.R.: search and review of the literature; D.R.R. and L.-D.H.: data extraction, comparative analysis, and statistical expertise; A.A.N., L.-D.H., A.B., and D.R.R.: interpretation of the data; A.A.N., A.B., and D.R.R.: critical revision for important intellectual content; all authors: final approval of the manuscript.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Acknowledgments

Thank you to Anna Lysenko-Brockman and Elena Lysenko for their efforts in translation. Michael G. Tordoff is acknowledged for his assistance with effect size calculations. A.N. was supported by the NIH National Research Service Award T32 (DC000014) Fellowship. The authors would like to thank Cordelia Running and Paule Valery Joseph for their constructive criticism of the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors have no conflict of interest to report.

Abbreviations

DTDetection threshold
MFPRTMacronutrient and food preference rating task
RTRecognition threshold
SNUTNutritional assessment system habits and nutrient intake (Spanish)

References

  1. Noone, A.M.; Howlader, N.; Krapcho, M.; Miller, D.; Brest, A.; Yu, M.; Ruhl, J.; Tatalovich, Z.; Mariotto, A.; Lewis, D.R.; et al. SEER Cancer Statistics Review, 1975–2015; National Cancer Institute: Bethesda, MD, USA, 2018.
  2. National Cancer Institute. Cancer Statistics. Available online: https://www.cancer.gov/about-cancer/understanding/statistics (accessed on 29 October 2018).
  3. Cohen, J.; Wakefield, C.E.; Laing, D.G. Smell and taste disorders resulting from cancer and chemotherapy. Curr. Pharm. Des. 2016, 22, 2253–2263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Wagland, R.; Richardson, A.; Armes, J.; Hankins, M.; Lennan, E.; Griffiths, P. Treatment-related problems experienced by cancer patients undergoing chemotherapy: A scoping review. Eur. J. Cancer Care 2015, 24, 605–617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Hovan, A.J.; Williams, P.M.; Stevenson-Moore, P.; Wahlin, Y.B.; Ohrn, K.E.; Elting, L.S.; Spijkervet, F.K.; Brennan, M.T. A systematic review of dysgeusia induced by cancer therapies. Support. Care Cancer 2010, 18, 1081–1087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Gamper, E.-M.; Giesinger, J.M.; Oberguggenberger, A.; Kemmler, G.; Wintner, L.M.; Gattringer, K.; Sperner-Unterweger, B.; Holzner, B.; Zabernigg, A. Taste alterations in breast and gynaecological cancer patients receiving chemotherapy: Prevalence, course of severity, and quality of life correlates. Acta Oncol. 2012, 51, 490–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Rolls, B.J. Do chemosensory changes influence food intake in the elderly? Physiol. Behav. 1999, 66, 193–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Schiffman, S.; Graham, B. Taste and smell perception affect appetite and immunity in the elderly. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2000, 54, S54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Mattes, R.D.; Cowart, B.J.; Schiavo, M.A.; Arnold, C.; Garrison, B.; Kare, M.R.; Lowry, L.D. Dietary evaluation of patients with smell and/or taste disorders. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1990, 51, 233–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Axelson, M.L.; Brinberg, D. Measures of Food-Related Behavior. In A Social-Psychological Perspective on Food-Related Behavior; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 1989; pp. 5–29. [Google Scholar]
  11. Brisbois, T.D.; Hutton, J.L.; Baracos, V.E.; Wismer, W.V. Taste and smell abnormalities as an independent cause of failure of food intake in patients with advanced cancer—An argument for the application of sensory science. J. Palliat. Care 2006, 22, 111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. da Costa Marinho, E.; Custódio, I.D.D.; Ferreira, I.B.; Crispim, C.A.; Paiva, C.E.; de Paiva Maia, Y.C. Impact of chemotherapy on perceptions related to food intake in women with breast cancer: A prospective study. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0187573. [Google Scholar]
  13. Gyan, E.; Raynard, B.; Durand, J.-P.; Lacau Saint Guily, J.; Gouy, S.; Movschin, M.L.; Khemissa, F.; Flori, N.; Oziel-Taieb, S.; Bannier Braticevic, C. Malnutrition in Patients with Cancer: Comparison of Perceptions by Patients, Relatives, and Physicians—Results of the NutriCancer2012 Study. J. Parenter. Enter. Nutr. 2017, 42, 255–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Boltong, A.; Keast, R.; Aranda, S. Experiences and consequences of altered taste, flavour and food hedonics during chemotherapy treatment. Support. Care Cancer 2012, 20, 2765–2774. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Boltong, A.; Keast, R. The influence of chemotherapy on taste perception and food hedonics: A systematic review. Cancer Treat. Rev. 2012, 38, 152–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Gamper, E.-M.; Zabernigg, A.; Wintner, L.M.; Giesinger, J.M.; Oberguggenberger, A.; Kemmler, G.; Sperner-Unterweger, B.; Holzner, B. Coming to your senses: Detecting taste and smell alterations in chemotherapy patients. A systematic review. J. Pain Symptom Manag. 2012, 44, 880–895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. de Vries, Y.; Winkels, R.; van den Berg, M.; de Graaf, C.; Kelfkens, C.; de Kruif, J.T.C.; Göker, E.; Grosfeld, S.; Sommeijer, D.; Van Laarhoven, H. Altered food preferences and chemosensory perception during chemotherapy in breast cancer patients: A longitudinal comparison with healthy controls. Food Qual. Prefer. 2018, 63, 135–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. IJpma, I.; Renken, R.J.; Gietema, J.A.; Slart, R.H.J.A.; Mensink, M.G.J.; Lefrandt, J.D.; Ter Horst, G.J.; Reyners, A.K.L. Taste and smell function in testicular cancer survivors treated with cisplatin-based chemotherapy in relation to dietary intake, food preference, and body composition. Appetite 2016, 105, 392–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Lipsey, M.W.; Wilson, D.B. Practical Meta-Analysis; Sage Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  20. Carson, J.; Gormican, A. Taste acuity and food attitudes of selected patients with cancer. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 1977, 70, 361–365. [Google Scholar]
  21. Trant, A.; Serin, J.; Douglass, H. Is taste related to anorexia in cancer patients? Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1982, 36, 45–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Ovesen, L.; Sorensen, M.; Hannibal, J.; Allingstrup, L. Electrical taste detection thresholds and chemical smell detection thresholds in patients with cancer. Cancer 1991, 68, 2260–2265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Pattison, R.; Richardson, R.; Dougan, H.; Davidson, H. Impact of altered taste sensitivity on dietary intake of patients with advanced cancer. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 1997, 56, 314. [Google Scholar]
  24. Sánchez-Lara, K.; Sosa-Sánchez, R.; Green-Renner, D.; Rodríguez, C.; Laviano, A.; Motola-Kuba, D.; Arrieta, O. Influence of taste disorders on dietary behaviors in cancer patients under chemotherapy. Nutr. J. 2010, 9, 15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Boltong, A.; Aranda, S.; Keast, R.; Wynne, R.; Francis, P.A.; Chirgwin, J.; Gough, K. A prospective cohort study of the effects of adjuvant breast cancer chemotherapy on taste function, food liking, appetite and associated nutritional outcomes. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e103512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Turcott, J.G.; Juárez-Hernández, E.; De la Torre-Vallejo, M.; Sánchez-Lara, K.; Luvian-Morales, J.; Arrieta, O. Value: Changes in the detection and recognition thresholds of three basic tastes in lung cancer patients receiving Cisplatin and Paclitaxel and its association with nutritional and quality of life parameters. Nutr. Cancer 2016, 68, 241–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. IJpma, I.; Renken, R.J.; Gietema, J.A.; Slart, R.H.; Mensink, M.G.; Lefrandt, J.D.; Ter Horst, G.J.; Reyners, A.K. Changes in taste and smell function, dietary intake, food preference, and body composition in testicular cancer patients treated with cisplatin-based chemotherapy. Clin. Nutr. 2017, 36, 1642–1648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. de Vries, Y.; Boesveldt, S.; Kampman, E.; de Graaf, C.; Winkels, R.; van Laarhoven, H. Low reported taste function is associated with low preference for high protein products in advanced oesophagogastric cancer patients undergoing palliative chemotherapy. Clin. Nutr. 2017, 38, 472–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Rumeau, C.; Nguyen, D.; Jankowski, R. How to assess olfactory performance with the Sniffin’Sticks test®. Eur. Ann. Otorhinolaryngol. Head Neck Dis. 2016, 133, 203–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Epstein, J.B.; Barasch, A. Taste disorders in cancer patients: Pathogenesis, and approach to assessment and management. Oral Oncol. 2010, 46, 77–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Epstein, J.B.; Smutzer, G.; Doty, R.L. Understanding the impact of taste changes in oncology care. Support. Care Cancer 2016, 24, 1917–1931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Spotten, L.; Corish, C.; Lorton, C.; Ui Dhuibhir, P.; O’Donoghue, N.; O’Connor, B.; Walsh, T. Subjective and objective taste and smell changes in cancer. Ann. Oncol. 2017, 28, 969–984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Bartoshuk, L.M.; Duffy, V.B.; Lucchina, L.A.; Prutkin, J.; Fast, K. PROP (6-n-Propylthiouracil) Supertasters and the Saltiness of NaCl. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1998, 855, 793–796. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Bartoshuk, L.M. Comparing sensory experiences across individuals: Recent psychophysical advances illuminate genetic variation in taste perception. Chem. Senses 2000, 25, 447–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Lawless, H.T.; Heymann, H. Sensory Evaluation of Food: Principles and Practices; Springer Science & Business Media: Berlin, Germany, 2010; Volume 5999. [Google Scholar]
  36. Boltong, A.; Campbell, K. ‘Taste’changes: A problem for patients and their dietitians. Nutr. Diet. 2013, 70, 262–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Egan, J.M.; Margolskee, R.F. Taste cells of the gut and gastrointestinal chemosensation. Mol. Interv. 2008, 8, 78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Skerman, H.M.; Yates, P.M.; Battistutta, D. Cancer-related symptom clusters for symptom management in outpatients after commencing adjuvant chemotherapy, at 6 months, and 12 months. Support. Care Cancer 2012, 20, 95–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Nolden, A.; Joseph, P.V.; Kober, K.M.; Cooper, B.A.; Paul, S.M.; Hammer, M.J.; Dunn, L.B.; Conley, Y.P.; Levine, J.D.; Miaskowski, C. Co-occurring Gastrointestinal Symptoms Are Associated with Taste Changes in Oncology Patients Receiving Chemotherapy. J. Pain Symptom Manag. 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Hong, J.H.; Omur-Ozbek, P.; Stanek, B.T.; Dietrich, A.M.; Duncan, S.E.; Lee, Y.; Lesser, G. Taste and odor abnormalities in cancer patients. J. Support. Oncol. 2009, 7, 58–65. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  41. Yang, H.; Cong, W.n.; Yoon, J.S.; Egan, J.M. Vismodegib, an antagonist of hedgehog signaling, directly alters taste molecular signaling in taste buds. Cancer Med. 2015, 4, 245–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Kumari, A.; Ermilov, A.N.; Allen, B.L.; Bradley, R.M.; Dlugosz, A.A.; Mistretta, C.M. Hedgehog pathway blockade with the cancer drug LDE225 disrupts taste organs and taste sensation. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol. 2014, 113, 1034–1040. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Bartoshuk, L.M.; Catalanotto, F.; Hoffman, H.; Logan, H.; Snyder, D.J. Taste damage (otitis media, tonsillectomy and head and neck cancer), oral sensations and BMI. Physiol. Behav. 2012, 107, 516–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Romesser, P.B.; Cahlon, O.; Scher, E.; Zhou, Y.; Berry, S.L.; Rybkin, A.; Sine, K.M.; Tang, S.; Sherman, E.J.; Wong, R. Proton beam radiation therapy results in significantly reduced toxicity compared with intensity-modulated radiation therapy for head and neck tumors that require ipsilateral radiation. Radiother. Oncol. 2016, 118, 286–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Running, C.A.; Hayes, J.E. Sip and spit or sip and swallow: Choice of method differentially alters taste intensity estimates across stimuli. Physiol. Behav. 2017, 181, 95–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Feeney, E.L.; Hayes, J.E. Regional differences in suprathreshold intensity for bitter and umami stimuli. Chemosens. Percept. 2014, 7, 147–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Duffy, V.; Lucchina, L.; Fast, K.; Bartoshuk, L. Taste and cancer. In Principles and Practice of Supportive Oncology; Lippincott-Raven: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 1998; pp. 141–151. [Google Scholar]
  48. de Bruijn, S.E.; de Vries, Y.C.; de Graaf, C.; Boesveldt, S.; Jager, G. The reliability and validity of the Macronutrient and Taste Preference Ranking Task: A new method to measure food preferences. Food Qual. Prefer. 2017, 57, 32–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. de Vries, Y.C.; Helmich, E.; Karsten, M.D.; Boesveldt, S.; Winkels, R.M.; van Laarhoven, H.W. The impact of chemosensory and food-related changes in patients with advanced oesophagogastric cancer treated with capecitabine and oxaliplatin: A qualitative study. Support. Care Cancer 2016, 24, 3119–3126. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  50. Archer, E.; Lavie, C.J.; Hill, J.O. The Failure to Measure Dietary Intake Engendered a Fictional Discourse on Diet-Disease Relations. Front. Nutr. 2018, 5, 105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  51. Fox, A.L. The relationship between chemical constitution and taste. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1932, 18, 115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Anonymous. Six in ten ‘tasteblind’to bitter chemical. Sci. News Lett. 1931, 9, 249. [Google Scholar]
  53. Dinehart, M.; Hayes, J.; Bartoshuk, L.; Lanier, S.; Duffy, V. Bitter taste markers explain variability in vegetable sweetness, bitterness, and intake. Physiol. Behav. 2006, 87, 304–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Kim, U.; Jorgenson, E.; Coon, H.; Leppert, M.; Risch, N.; Drayna, D. Positional cloning of the human quantitative trait locus underlying taste sensitivity to phenylthiocarbamide. Sci. STKE 2003, 299, 1221. [Google Scholar]
  55. Kim, U.; Wooding, S.; Ricci, D.; Jorde, L.B.; Drayna, D. Worldwide haplotype diversity and coding sequence variation at human bitter taste receptor loci. Hum. Mutat. 2005, 26, 199–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Hayes, J.E.; Bartoshuk, L.M.; Kidd, J.R.; Duffy, V.B. Supertasting and PROP Bitterness Depends on More Than the TAS2R38 Gene. Chem. Senses 2008, 33, 255–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Duffy, V.B.; Davidson, A.C.; Kidd, J.R.; Kidd, K.K.; Speed, W.C.; Pakstis, A.J.; Reed, D.R.; Snyder, D.J.; Bartoshuk, L.M. Bitter Receptor Gene (TAS2R38), 6-n-Propylthiouracil (PROP) Bitterness and Alcohol Intake. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2004, 28, 1629–1637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Bufe, B.; Breslin, P.A.; Kuhn, C.; Reed, D.R.; Tharp, C.D.; Slack, J.P.; Kim, U.K.; Drayna, D.; Meyerhof, W. The molecular basis of individual differences in phenylthiocarbamide and propylthiouracil bitterness perception. Curr. Biol. 2005, 15, 322–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Allen, A.L.; McGeary, J.E.; Knopik, V.S.; Hayes, J.E. Bitterness of the Non-nutritive Sweetener Acesulfame Potassium Varies with Polymorphisms in TAS2R9 and TAS2R31. Chem. Senses 2013, 38, 379–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Hayes, J.E.; Feeney, E.L.; Nolden, A.A.; McGeary, J.E. Quinine bitterness and grapefruit liking associate with allelic variants in TAS2R31. Chem. Senses 2015, 40, 437–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Bartoshuk, L.M. The biological basis of food perception and acceptance. Food Qual. Prefer. 1993, 4, 21–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Tepper, B.J. 6-n-Propylthiouracil: A genetic marker for taste, with implications for food preference and dietary habits. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 1998, 63, 1271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Tepper, B.J. Nutritional implications of genetic taste variation: The role of PROP sensitivity and other taste phenotypes. Annu. Rev. Nutr. 2008, 28, 367–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Keller, K.L.; Adise, S. Variation in the Ability to Taste Bitter Thiourea Compounds: Implications for Food Acceptance, Dietary Intake, and Obesity Risk in Children. Annu. Rev. Nutr. 2016, 36, 157–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Hayes, J.E.; Wallace, M.R.; Knopik, V.S.; Herbstman, D.M.; Bartoshuk, L.M.; Duffy, V.B. Allelic variation in TAS2R bitter receptor genes associates with variation in sensations from and ingestive behaviors toward common bitter beverages in adults. Chem. Senses 2011, 36, 311–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Hayes, J.E.; Feeney, E.L.; Allen, A.L. Do polymorphisms in chemosensory genes matter for human ingestive behavior? Food Qual. Prefer. 2013, 30, 202–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Nolden, A.A.; Hayes, J.E. Perceptual and affective responses to sampled capsaicin differ by reported intake. Food Qual. Prefer. 2017, 55, 26–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Nolden, A.A.; Hayes, J.E. Perceptual qualities of ethanol depend on concentration, and variation in these percepts associates with drinking frequency. Chemosens. Percept. 2015, 8, 149–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Wise, P.M.; Nattress, L.; Flammer, L.J.; Beauchamp, G.K. Reduced dietary intake of simple sugars alters perceived sweet taste intensity but not perceived pleasantness. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2015, 103, 50–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Bobowski, N. Shifting human salty taste preference: Potential opportunities and challenges in reducing dietary salt intake of Americans. Chemosens. Percept. 2015, 8, 112–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Carrai, M.; Steinke, V.; Vodicka, P.; Pardini, B.; Rahner, N.; Holinski-Feder, E.; Morak, M.; Schackert, H.K.; Görgens, H.; Stemmler, S.; et al. Association between TAS2R38 gene polymorphisms and colorectal cancer risk: A case-control study in two independent populations of Caucasian origin. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e20464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Choi, J.-H.; Lee, J.; Oh, J.H.; Chang, H.J.; Sohn, D.K.; Shin, A.; Kim, J. Variations in the bitterness perception-related genes TAS2R38 and CA6 modify the risk for colorectal cancer in Koreans. Oncotarget 2017, 8, 21253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Choi, J.-H.; Lee, J.; Choi, I.J.; Kim, Y.-W.; Ryu, K.W.; Kim, J. Genetic variation in the TAS2R38 bitter taste receptor and gastric cancer risk in Koreans. Sci. Rep. 2016, 6, 26904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Lambert, J.D.; VanDusen, S.R.; Cockroft, J.E.; Smith, E.C.; Greenwood, D.C.; Cade, J.E. Bitter taste sensitivity, food intake, and risk of malignant cancer in the UK Women’s Cohort Study. Eur. J. Nutr. 2018, 58, 1–11. [Google Scholar]
  75. Running, C.A. High false positive rates in common sensory threshold tests. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2015, 77, 692–700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Bartoshuk, L.M. The psychophysics of taste. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1978, 31, 1068–1077. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Sontag, A.M. Comparison of Sensory Methods, Discrimination, Intensity and Hedonic Responses in Four Modalities; University of California, Davis: Davis, CA, USA, 1978. [Google Scholar]
  78. Hayes, J.E.; Sullivan, B.S.; Duffy, V.B. Explaining variability in sodium intake through oral sensory phenotype, salt sensation and liking. Physiol. Behav. 2010, 100, 369–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Mojet, J.; Christ-Hazelhof, E.; Heidema, J. Taste perception with age: Pleasantness and its relationships with threshold sensitivity and supra-threshold intensity of five taste qualities. Food Qual. Prefer. 2005, 16, 413–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Comeau, T.B.; Epstein, J.B.; Migas, C. Taste and smell dysfunction in patients receiving chemotherapy: A review of current knowledge. Support. Care Cancer 2001, 9, 575–580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Summary of studies using direct measures of taste and/or smell and measures of food behavior.
Table 1. Summary of studies using direct measures of taste and/or smell and measures of food behavior.
StudySubjects and Cancer Type (n)Study DesignChemosensory EvaluationFood Behavior EvaluationOutcome
StimuliEvaluationFood AspectsMethod(s)
Carson and Gormican, 1997 [20]Breast (n = 14) or colon (n = 15); controls (n = 28)Prospective, longitudinal cohort study; baseline and after 2 weeks of treatment Sucrose, sodium chloride, hydrochloric acid, urea Taste: DT and RT presented as oral dropsAppetite, amount eaten, consumption of specific menu itemsQuestionnaire (rated as increased, decreased, or no change) Prior to treatment, there was a significant correlation between increased sucrose RT (decreased sensitivity) with reduced appetite within cancer patient groups
Trant et al., 1982 [21]Upper gastrointestinal or lung (n = 62)Cohort study post-treatmentCherry drink with sucrose, tomato juice with sodium chloride, lemonade with citric acid, tonic water with urea (five conc. of each)Taste: Intensity and hedonic rating on a 10-cm labeled analog scale (0–10)Energy intake and number of servings 24-h dietary recall Higher ratings for salt intensity associated with increased energy intake
Ovessen et al., 1991 [22]Breast (n = 4), lung (n = 16), ovarian (n = 11) Prospective, longitudinal; visits at baseline and after three treatment cycles (2–3 months postbaseline)Anodal current (2.5–370 µA) using a gustometerTaste: DTDietary intake3-day food record No relationship between taste or smell DT and food intake
PyridineSmell: DT using squeeze bottles
Pattison et al., 1997 [23]Non-specified cancer type (n = 22); controls (n = 16)Cross-sectional cohort study; visit not specifiedSweet, sour, salty, and bitter (compounds not stated)Taste: DT (presentation method not stated)Dietary intakeWeighted measurement of food intake for one meal on three different daysNo relationship between taste DT measures and macronutrient intake
Sánchez-Lara et al., 2010 [24]Unspecified cancer type (n = 30); controls (n = 30)Cross-sectional cohort study; visit after second treatment cycleSucrose, urea, sodium glutamate Taste: DT and RT using whole-mouth stimuliDietary intakeSNUT food frequency questionnairePatients with higher sucrose DT consumed less protein, carbohydrate, zinc, and overall calories; patients with higher urea RT consumed less protein, carbohydrate, fat, and overall calories
Boltong et al., 2014 [25]Breast (n = 52)Prospective longitudinal; visits: baseline, beginning, middle, and late in third cycle, beginning of last cycle, and 2 months after treatmentSucrose, sodium chloride, citric acid, caffeine, and monosodium glutamate Taste: Identification *: Method of investigating sensitivity of taste using whole-mouth stimuliDietary intakeFood frequency questionnaireDeterioration in identification of all five taste qualities correlated with reduced energy intake
IJpma et al., 2016 [18]Testicular (n = 50); controls (n = 48)Cross-sectional cohort; visits 1, 3, 5, or 7 years after treatmentSucrose, citric acid, quinine HCl, sodium chloride Taste: RT using taste stripsDietary intake food preference Food frequency questionnaire; preference for pictures of snack productsNo relationship between taste/smell scores and food preference or dietary intake
n-butanol and a set of 16 common odorsSmell: DT, discrimination, identification, using Sniffin’ Sticks
Turcott et al., 2016 [26]Lung (n = 40)Prospective longitudinal; visits: baseline, after two cyclesSucrose, urea, sodium glutamate presented as whole-mouth stimuliTaste: DT and RT Dietary intake, appetiteSNUT food frequency questionnaire Increase in sweetness sensitivity (i.e., lower DT from baseline) associated with decreased intake of protein, fat, and iron
IJpma et al., 2017 [27]Testicular (n = 21); controls (n = 48)Cross-sectional and prospective longitudinal; visits: baseline, before and after cycles 1 and 2, end of treatment, 7 and 12 months from start of treatmentSucrose, citric acid, quinine HCl, sodium chlorideTaste: RT using taste stripsDietary intake food preference Food frequency questionnaire; preference for pictures of snack productsNo relationship between taste/smell scores with food preferences or dietary intake
n-butanol and a set of 16 common odorsSmell: DT, discrimination, identification, using Sniffin’ Sticks
de Vries et al., 2017 [28]Oesophago-gastric (n = 15)Prospective longitudinal; visits at baseline and before start of third cyclen-butanol and a set of 16 common odorsSmell: DT, discrimination, identification using Sniffin’ SticksFood preferencesMFPRT ratingNo relationship between taste/smell and food preferences
Sucrose, citric acid, sodium chloride, quinine HClTaste: RT using taste strips
de Vries et al., 2018 [17]Breast (n = 28); controls (n = 28)Prospective longitudinal; visits at baseline, during treatment, and 1–3 weeks and 6 months after treatmentSucrose, sodium chloride, citric acid, quinine HClTaste: RT using taste stripsFood preferencesMFPRT ratingNo relationship between taste/smell and food preferences; no significant changes in ranking of sampled spiked beverages
Kool-Aid with sucrose, tomato juice with sodium chloride (five conc. of each)Taste: Preference (rank samples in order of liking)
n-butanol and a set of 16 common odorsSmell: DT, discrimination, identification using Sniffin’ sticks
* ISO 3972:2011: International Organization for Standardization—method of investigating sensitivity of taste. DT—taste or odor detection threshold; MFPRT—macronutrient and food preference rating task (rating of food pictures); RT—taste or odor recognition threshold; SNUT—nutritional assessment system habits and nutrient intake (Spanish); conc.—concentration.
Table 2. Methods used to evaluate taste and smell function.
Table 2. Methods used to evaluate taste and smell function.
MethodTaskStimuliStudies
Taste
 Recognition threshold (RT)Lowest concentration a taste can be recognizedTaste strips, whole-mouth solutions, or oral drops[17,18,20,24,26,27,28]
 Detection threshold (DT)Lowest concentration something different from water is detectedTaste strips, whole-mouth solutions, or oral drops[20,23,24,26]
 IdentificationIdentify tastes from a single suprathreshold concentrationWhole-mouth solution[25]
 IntensityRating on a 10-cm analog scale labeled from “no (taste sensation)” to “extremely (taste sensation)”.Beverages spiked with tastant[21]
 LikingRating on a 10-cm analog scale labeled from “dislike extremely” to “like extremely”Beverages spiked with tastant[21]
 ElectrogustometryLowest detected electrical stimuli (µA) (DT)Anodal current via gustometer[22]
 PreferenceRank samples in order of likingBeverages spiked with tastants at five concentrations[17]
Smell
 DTLowest concentration detectedPyridine or n-butanol (squeeze bottles or Sniffin’ Sticks)[17,18,22,28]
 DiscriminationFrom three odors, select the odor that is different from two other identical odors (16 triplets presented)Sniffin’ Sticks[17,18,28]
 IdentificationIdentify the odor from a choice of four options (16 common odors)Sniffin’ Sticks[17,18,28]
Table 3. Effect sizes for taste – difference in food intake in cancer patients.
Table 3. Effect sizes for taste – difference in food intake in cancer patients.
TasteFood-RelatedIntake a or Change in Intake b in Patients with Low DT (Mean ± SD).Intake a or Change in Intake b in Patients with High DT (Mean ± SD).Effect
d (95% CI)
Reference
SweetZinc (mg/day)17 ± 7 a9.6 ± 4.5 a1.18 (0.41, 1.96)Sánchez-Lara, 2010 [24]
SweetFat (g/day)+27.39 b−15.1 b0.97 (0.31, 1.62)Turcott, 2016 [26]
SweetCarbohydrate (g/day)240 ± 84 a167 ± 81 a0.88 (0.13, 1.63)Sánchez-Lara, 2010 [24]
SweetAnimal protein (g/day)+18.6 b−15.1 b0.86 (0.21, 1.51)Turcott, 2016 [26]
SweetProtein (g/day)+30.8 b−7.9 b0.81 (0.16, 1.45)Turcott, 2016 [26]
SweetCalories (kcal/day)1970 ± 658 a1450 ± 833 a0.69 (−0.04, 1.43)Sánchez-Lara, 2010 [24]
SweetIron (mg/day)+8.4 b−1.11 b0.65 (0.01, 1.29)Turcott, 2016 [26]
SweetAppetite * 0.59 (0.00, 1.18)Carson, 1997 [20]
SweetProtein (g/day)74 ± 45 a53 ± 32 a0.54 (−0.19, 1.27)Sánchez-Lara, 2010 [24]
BitterCalories/day2124 ± 812 a1493 ± 452 a0.96 (0.20, 1.72)Sánchez-Lara, 2010 [24]
BitterCarbohydrate (g/day)254 ± 98 a182 ± 58 a0.89 (0.14, 1.64)Sánchez-Lara, 2010 [24]
BitterFat (g/day)87 ± 38 a62 ± 22 a0.81 (0.06, 1.55)Sánchez-Lara, 2010 [24]
BitterProtein (g/day)83 ± 53 a52 ± 17 a0.79 (0.04, 1.53)Sánchez-Lara, 2010 [24]
Effect size d represents the standardized difference of means in food intake, calculated based on mean, standard deviation (SD), p-value, and sample size provided in the literature, between patients with a low/decreased DT and patients with high/increased DT. CI—95% confidence interval. a Mean intake in patients with low DT. b Change in the mean intake in patients with a decreased DT. * The study only reported the number of patients with increased or decreased appetite after chemotherapy and the significance (p-value) of the difference. “+” denotes an increase in intake.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Nolden, A.A.; Hwang, L.-D.; Boltong, A.; Reed, D.R. Chemosensory Changes from Cancer Treatment and Their Effects on Patients’ Food Behavior: A Scoping Review. Nutrients 2019, 11, 2285. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11102285

AMA Style

Nolden AA, Hwang L-D, Boltong A, Reed DR. Chemosensory Changes from Cancer Treatment and Their Effects on Patients’ Food Behavior: A Scoping Review. Nutrients. 2019; 11(10):2285. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11102285

Chicago/Turabian Style

Nolden, Alissa A., Liang-Dar Hwang, Anna Boltong, and Danielle R. Reed. 2019. "Chemosensory Changes from Cancer Treatment and Their Effects on Patients’ Food Behavior: A Scoping Review" Nutrients 11, no. 10: 2285. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11102285

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop