Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 25 October 2021
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Ageing and Migration Status: Intersectional Forms of Discrimination and Exclusion View all 10 articles

An Overlooked Perspective in Psychological Interventions to Reduce Anti-elderly Discriminatory Attitudes

  • Graduate School of Humanities and Sociology, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan

Introduction

The world's population is aging at a remarkable rate. The percentage of the world's population aged 65 and over was 5.1% in 1950, was 8.3% in 2015, and will increase to 17.8% by 2060 (United Nations, 2017). In this aging society, intergenerational conflicts between the elderly and the rest of the population are frequently observed in many workplaces and nursing care (Binstock, 2010). Anti-elderly discriminatory attitudes held by non-elderly people have been examined as one of the major causes of such intergenerational conflicts (North and Fiske, 2013). Specifically, the elderly is often perceived as incompetent and stubborn (McKenzie and Brown, 2014). Non-elderly people may also hold the discriminatory view that the elderly should pass down any resources, avoid excessive consumption of any shared social resources, and not behave as if they were younger (North and Fiske, 2013). These anti-elderly discriminatory attitudes lead to a decrease in the quality of life of the elderly (Levy et al., 2000), a disregard for the will of the elderly (Vitman et al., 2014), and an inhibition of the formation of a harmonious intergenerational society (Ishii and Tadooka, 2015). Based on the above, the affirmation of attitudes toward the elderly is an important issue in psychological research.

This opinion paper will begin with a broad overview of the interventions that have been implemented to reduce anti-elderly discriminatory attitudes. Then, an important perspective specific to a social group of the elderly, which have not been sufficiently paid attention to, is pointed out; all people will eventually belong to a social group of the elderly. As a theory that incorporates this perspective, Levy's (2009) stereotype embodiment theory (SET) will be introduced, and a typical factor (i.e., subjective time to become elderly) that should be focused on in future interventions to reduce anti-elderly discriminatory attitudes, will be discussed. In this paper, the fact that we all become elderly is focused on, and this mainly refers to healthy aging, not pathological aging.

Previous Interventions to Combat the Anti-Elderly Discriminatory Attitudes

To reduce anti-elderly discriminatory attitudes, a wide range of strategies have been conducted. Previous research has conducted educational interventions which aim to demystify certain commonly misunderstood aspects of the aging and elderly (Chonody, 2015; Lytle and Levy, 2019). For example, Wurtele and Maruyama (2013) found that, after participants were presented with accurate information about the elderly, their anti-elderly discriminatory attitudes significantly decreased. The advantage of such educational interventions is that they are relatively easy to implement and can be delivered to a wide range of participants simultaneously. On the contrary, interventions to encourage perspective taking by participants' experience in the impaired physical movements of the elderly (Berthold et al., 2013) and experience in the elderly's appearance using virtual reality (Oh et al., 2016), are also shown to be effective. These types of interventions will continue to increase as scientific technology have developed in recent years. In addition, various studies aimed to reduce anti-elderly discriminatory attitudes through direct contact experience with the elderly (Allan and Johnson, 2008). For instance, Meshel and McGlynn (2004) conducted a 6-week intergenerational exchange program; elementary and junior high school students' attitudes toward the elderly became more positive after the intervention. This result is consistent with the classical findings on mere exposure effects (Zajonc, 1968; Kwan et al., 2015). Extended contact experiences, in which participants imagine getting positive and favorable contact experience with the elderly, are also effective in reducing discriminatory attitudes (Drury et al., 2016; Pekçetin et al., 2021). A strategy of extended contact has the advantage of being relatively easy to implement even in environments where it is difficult to get contact experience with the elderly.

Intervention strategies described above contribute to reducing anti-elderly discriminatory attitudes, but they miss an important perspective specific to a social group of the elderly; all people will eventually belong to a social group of the elderly. This characteristic does not apply to other contexts of prejudice, such as gender or race. Based on the above, it is important to incorporate the perspective that everyone will eventually become an elderly into the context of prejudice against the elderly, but there has been insufficient research to date (Levy, 2009; Takeuchi, 2016; Shimizu et al., 2021). Therefore, in this opinion paper, Levy's (2009) SET is focused on as a theory that incorporates this perspective.

Stereotype Embodiment Theory

SET is a theory that discusses the effects of elderly stereotypes on the perceivers themselves. SET consists of four major processes: internalization, unconscious operation, salience gain from self-relevance, and utilization of multiple pathways (Levy, 2009). Internalization is the process by which people are faced with and internalize elderly stereotypes throughout their lives, and the process begins in childhood (Levy and Banaji, 2002). Unconscious operation is the process by which internalized stereotypes, as described above, automatically and unconsciously influence people's judgments and actions (Bargh et al., 1996). Salience gain from self-relevance is the process by which internalized stereotypes of the elderly are understood as highly self-relevant incidents and this is a process that we all go through as we get older (Levy, 2009). Utilization of multiple pathways is the process by which self-associated stereotypes of the elderly influence themselves psychologically, behaviorally, and physiologically (Levy et al., 2006; Wurm and Benyamini, 2014; Chasteen et al., 2015). For example, it has been shown that elderly people who have strongly internalized negative elderly stereotypes are more likely to feel stressed and lonely (McHugh, 2003).

SET is unique in that it discusses the effects of elderly stereotypes on perceivers themselves, focusing on the temporal dimension. Everyone will eventually become elderly, and those who have a negative view of the elderly when they are young are more likely to be affected by the undesirable effects described above (Levy, 2009). It is an inherent characteristic of the elderly that perceivers of stereotypes and prejudices will one day become members of the stigmatized group. Based on the above, it is necessary to incorporate this temporal dimension into the discussion in the context of reducing anti-elderly discriminatory attitudes. However, a major problem is that there has not been enough discussion that incorporates this perspective.

Discussion

As strongly argued in the SET, the elderly is unique in which everyone eventually belongs to. In order to incorporate this perspective into the context of reducing anti-elderly discriminatory attitudes, it would be useful to focus on the subjective time to become elderly. It has been reported that there are large individual differences in people's sense of time (Jokic et al., 2018; Stam et al., 2020). In other words, even if the time is the same length, each person feels it in a different way. Specifically, some people believe that becoming elderly is not a long way off, despite their actual age being young, while others believe that becoming elderly is still a long way off, despite their actual age being relatively old.

Those who believe that becoming elderly is still a long way off will be more likely to perceive the elderly as separate from the self and unlikely to imagine themselves when they become elderly. Such cognition of separating the elderly from the self as temporally distant is noteworthy because it is likely to contribute to anti-elderly discriminatory attitudes and, in turn, to the reinforcement of intergenerational conflicts. From the perspective of social identity theory (Tajfel, 1981; Onorato and Turner, 2004), it can be said that when we perceive others as different and distant from ourselves, we perceive them as an outgroup and direct discriminatory attitudes toward them. Based on the above, interventions that make participants feel that the subjective time to become elderly is shorter, may be effective. However, in the context of reducing anti-elderly discriminatory attitudes, the impact of the subjective time to become elderly has not been sufficiently examined. Future research should explore interventions such as experimentally manipulating the subjective time to become elderly.

In this opinion paper, a broad overview of the interventions to reduce anti-elderly discriminatory attitudes is shown. An important perspective; all people will eventually belong to a social group of the elderly, has been overlooked in the literature. To incorporate such perspective, Levy's (2009) SET was introduced, and it would be effective to focus on the subjective time to become elderly. Future studies should investigate and focus on the temporal dimension to find more effective intervention methods. Therefore, the concept of subjective time to become elderly deserves a great deal of attention. This opinion paper will contribute to the direction of psychological intervention research and its future development.

Author Contributions

YS: article development, composition, draft review, and creative oversight.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Allan, L. J., and Johnson, J. A. (2008). Undergraduate attitudes toward the elderly: the role of knowledge, contact and aging anxiety. Educ. Gerontol. 35, 1–14. doi: 10.1080/03601270802299780

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., and Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 71, 230–244. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berthold, A., Leicht, C., Methner, N., and Gaum, P. (2013). Seeing the world with the eyes of the outgroup—The impact of perspective taking on the prototypicality of the ingroup relative to the outgroup. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49, 1034–1041. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.07.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binstock, R. H. (2010). From compassionate ageism to intergenerational conflict? Gerontologist 50, 574–585. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnq056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chasteen, A. L., Pichora-Fuller, M. K., Dupuis, K., Smith, S., and Singh, G. (2015). Do negative views of aging influence memory and auditory performance through self-perceived abilities? Psychol. Aging 30, 881–893. doi: 10.1037/a0039723

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chonody, J. M. (2015). Addressing ageism in students: a systematic review of the pedagogical intervention literature. Educ. Gerontol. 41, 859–887. doi: 10.1080/03601277.2015.1059139

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drury, L., Hutchison, P., and Abrams, D. (2016). Direct and extended intergenerational contact and young people's attitudes towards older adults. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 55, 522–543. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishii, K., and Tadooka, Y. (2015). The influence of pathogen threat on ageism in Japan: the role of living with older adults. Japanese J. Psychol. 86, 240–248. doi: 10.4992/jjpsy.86.14020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jokic, T., Zakay, D., and Wittmann, M. (2018). Individual differences in self-rated impulsivity modulate the estimation of time in a real waiting situation. Timing Time Percept. 6, 71–89. doi: 10.1163/22134468-00002101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwan, L. Y. Y., Yap, S., and Chiu, C. Y. (2015). Mere exposure affects perceived descriptive norms: implications for personal preferences and trust. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process 129, 48–58. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levy, B. (2009). Stereotype embodiment: a psychosocial approach to aging. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 18, 332–336. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01662.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levy, B. R., Ashman, O., and Dror, I. (2000). To be or not to be: the effects of aging stereotypes on the will to live. Omega 40, 409–420. doi: 10.2190/Y2GE-BVYQ-NF0E-83VR

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levy, B. R., and Banaji, M. R. (2002). “Implicit ageism,” in Ageism: Stereotyping and Prejudice Against Older Persons, ed. T. D. Nelson (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 49–96.

Google Scholar

Levy, B. R., Slade, M. D., May, J., and Caracciolo, E. A. (2006). Physical recovery after acute myocardial infarction: positive age self-stereotypes as a resource. Int. J. Aging. Hum. Dev. 62, 285–301. doi: 10.2190/EJK1-1Q0D-LHGE-7A35

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lytle, A., and Levy, S. R. (2019). Reducing ageism: education about aging and extended contact with older adults. Gerontologist 59, 580–588. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnx177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McHugh, K. E. (2003). Three faces of ageism: society, image, and place. Ageing Soc. 23, 165–185. doi: 10.1017/S0144686X02001113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKenzie, E. L., and Brown, P. M. (2014). Nursing students' intentions to work in dementia care: influence of age, ageism, and perceived barriers. Educ. Gerontol. 40, 618–633. doi: 10.1080/03601277.2013.863545

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meshel, D. S., and McGlynn, R. P. (2004). Intergenerational contact, attitudes, and stereotypes of adolescents and other people. Educ. Gerontol. 30, 457–479. doi: 10.1080/03601270490445078

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

North, M. S., and Fiske, S. T. (2013). Act your (old) age: prescriptive, ageist biases over succession, consumption, and identity. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 39, 720–734. doi: 10.1177/0146167213480043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oh, S. Y., Bailenson, J., Weisz, E., and Zaki, J. (2016). Virtually old: embodied perspective taking and the reduction of ageism under threat. Comput. Hum. Behav. 60, 398–410. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.02.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onorato, R. S., and Turner, J. C. (2004). Fluidity in the self-concept: the shift from personal to social identity. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 34, 257–278. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.195

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekçetin, S., Hasgül, E., Dügeroglu, R. Y., and Arabac,i, Z. (2021). The effect of extended contact with community dwelling older adults on the ageist and helping attitudes of home care students: a single blind randomised controlled trial. Int. J. Older. People. Nurs. 16:e12382. doi: 10.1111/opn.12382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shimizu, Y., Hashimoto, T., and Karasawa, K. (2021). The complementation of the stereotype embodiment theory: focusing on the social identity theory. J. Hum. Environ. Stud. 19, 9–14. doi: 10.4189/shes.19.9

CrossRef Full Text

Stam, C. H., van der Veen, F. M., and Franken, I. H. (2020). Individual differences in time estimation are associated with delay discounting and alcohol use. Curr. Psychol. doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-00899-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and Social Categories: Studies in Social Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Takeuchi, M. (2016). “The effects of attitude toward aging on social participation and physical activity in the future: 15 years longitudinal study of Japanese elders [Conference presentation],” in The 31st International Congress of Psychology, Yokohama, Japan.

United Nations (2017). World Population Prospects: The 2017 Revision. Department of Economic and Social Affairs in United Nations.

Vitman, A., Iecovich, E., and Alfasi, N. (2014). Ageism and social integration of older adults in their neighborhoods in Israel. Gerontologist 54, 177–189. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnt008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wurm, S., and Benyamini, Y. (2014). Optimism buffers the detrimental effect of negative self-perceptions of ageing on physical and mental health. Psychol. Health 29, 832–848. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2014.891737

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wurtele, S. K., and Maruyama, L. (2013). Changing students' stereotypes of older adults. Teach. Psychol. 40, 59–61. doi: 10.1177/0098628312465867

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 9, 1–27. doi: 10.1037/h0025848

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: elderly people, discriminatory attitudes, stereotype embodiment theory, interventions, subjective time, intergenerational conflicts

Citation: Shimizu Y (2021) An Overlooked Perspective in Psychological Interventions to Reduce Anti-elderly Discriminatory Attitudes. Front. Psychol. 12:765394. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.765394

Received: 27 August 2021; Accepted: 29 September 2021;
Published: 25 October 2021.

Edited by:

Elaine Dewhurst, The University of Manchester, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Alessia Rosi, University of Pavia, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Shimizu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yuho Shimizu, yuhos1120mizu@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.