Skip to main content
Log in

Are large portions always bad? Using the Delboeuf illusion on food packaging to nudge consumer behavior

  • Published:
Marketing Letters Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Exaggerated portion sizes are generally pictured on the front of product packaging in order to stimulate food craving and encourage consumer purchasing decisions. However, one problem with such images is that they can set inappropriate norms as far as food consumption is concerned and hence result in people serving themselves more than they otherwise might. The research reported here builds on the fact that depicting a food portion in a smaller (vs. larger) container (i.e., plate or bowl) creates the illusion of a larger (vs. smaller) portion, although the actual quantity of food remains the same (this is known as the Delboeuf illusion). Here, we demonstrate in two experiments that by presenting food in a smaller container (thus giving rise to the illusion of a relatively larger portion), participants have higher purchase intentions (study 1) and perceive the food as being more appetizing (study 2) but, crucially, decrease the size of the portion that they serve themselves (studies 1 and 2). Overall, by giving the impression of a larger portion on product packaging, the Delboeuf illusion could potentially be used to nudge consumers to find food more desirable, while at the same time leading them to reduce their serving, thus potentially benefitting both consumers and the food industry.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Note that Wansink et al. (2005), as well as other work by Brian Wansink, has recently come under scrutiny, and it appears that it contains some errors (e.g., https://medium.com/@jamesheathers/sprite-case-study-5-sunset-for-souper-man-ee898b6af9f5). However, other researchers in the field have also studied and put forward the portion size effect (Holden et al. 2016). It should, however, be noted that some meta-analyses, such as Robinson et al. (2014), did not find a consistent effect of dishware size on food intake.

  2. openfoodfacts.org/ is a free and open database of food products from around the world, generally providing a picture of the food packaging. We have listed 128 packets of corn flakes and 224 packets of cheese pizza sold in 26 countries. All of the pizza plates were small, and we only found three bowls of cereals that were not small rimmed.

References

  • Aglioti, S., DeSouza, J. F., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Current Biology, 5(6), 679–685.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Akyol, A., Ayaz, A., Inan-Eroglu, E., Cetin, C., & Samur, G. (2018). Impact of three different plate colours on short-term satiety and energy intake: a randomized controlled trial. Nutrition Journal, 17(1), 46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Basso, F., Petit, O., Le Bellu, S., Lahlou, S., Cancel, A., & Anton, J. L. (2018). Taste at first (person) sight: visual perspective modulates brain activity implicitly associated with viewing unhealthy but not healthy foods. Appetite, 128, 242–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brand, J., & Wansink, B. (2016). Depicted versus stated serving sizes: exaggerated servings on packaging encourage overeating. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior, 48(7), S91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brunstrom, J. M., & Shakeshaft, N. G. (2009). Measuring affective (liking) and non-affective (expected satiety) determinants of portion size and food reward. Appetite, 52(1), 108–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cornil, Y., & Chandon, P. (2016). Pleasure as a substitute for size: how multisensory imagery can make people happier with smaller food portions. Journal of Marketing Research, 53(5), 847–864.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delboeuf, F. J. (1865). Note sur certaines illusions d’optique: essai d’une théorie psychophysique de la manière dont l’œil apprécie les distances et les angles [Note on certain optical illusions: essay on a psychophysical theory concerning the way in which the eye evaluates distances and angles]. Bulletins de l’Académie Royale des Sciences, Lettres et Beaux-arts de Belgique, 19, 2nd ser., pp 195–216.

  • Elder, R. S., & Krishna, A. (2012). The “visual depiction effect” in advertising: facilitating embodied mental simulation through product orientation. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(6), 988–1003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franz, V. H., Gegenfurtner, K. R., Bülthoff, H. H., & Fahle, M. (2000). Grasping visual illusions: no evidence for a dissociation between perception and action. Psychological Science, 11(1), 20–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, J., Mancino, L., & Lin, C. T. J. (2015). Nudging consumers toward better food choices: policy approaches to changing food consumption behaviors. Psychology and Marketing, 32(5), 501–511.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holden, S. S., Zlatevska, N., & Dubelaar, C. (2016). Whether smaller plates reduce consumption depends on who’s serving and who’s looking: a meta-analysis. Journal of the Association for Consumer Research, 1(1), 134–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labbe, D., Rytz, A., Godinot, N., Ferrage, A., & Martin, N. (2017). Is portion size selection associated with expected satiation, perceived healthfulness or expected tastiness? A case study on pizza using a photograph-based computer task. Appetite, 108, 311–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Madzharov, A. V., & Block, L. G. (2010). Effects of product unit image on consumption of snack foods. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 20(4), 398–409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClain, A. D., van den Bos, W., Matheson, D., Desai, M., McClure, S. M., & Robinson, T. N. (2014). Visual illusions and plate design: the effects of plate rim widths and rim coloring on perceived food portion size. International Journal of Obesity, 38(5), 657–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papies, E. K. (2013). Tempting food words activate eating simulations. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peng, M., Adam, S., Hautus, M. J., Shin, M., Duizer, L. M., & Yan, H. (2017). See food diet? Cultural differences in estimating fullness and intake as a function of plate size. Appetite, 117, 197–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petit, O., Basso, F., Merunka, D., Spence, C., Cheok, A. D., & Oullier, O. (2016a). Pleasure and the control of food intake: an embodied cognition approach to consumer self-regulation. Psychology and Marketing, 33(8), 608–619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petit, O., Merunka, D., Anton, J.-L., Nazarian, B., Spence, C., Cheok, A. D., Raccah, D., & Oullier, O. (2016b). Health and pleasure in consumers’ dietary food choices: individual differences in the brain’s value system. PLoS One, 11(7), e0156333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petit, O., Spence, C., Velasco, C., Woods, A. T., & Cheok, A. D. (2017). Changing the influence of portion size on consumer behavior via imagined consumption. Journal of Business Research, 75, 240–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, E., Nolan, S., Tudur-Smith, C., Boyland, E. J., Harrold, J. A., Hardman, C. A., & Halford, J. C. (2014). Will smaller plates lead to smaller waists? A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effect that experimental manipulation of dishware size has on energy consumption. Obesity Reviews, 15(10), 812–821.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sawada, R., Sato, W., Toichi, M., & Fushiki, T. (2017). Fat content modulates rapid detection of food: a visual search study using fast food and Japanese diet. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1033.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C., Okajima, K., Cheok, A. D., Petit, O., & Michel, C. (2016). Eating with our eyes: from visual hunger to digital satiation. Brain and Cognition, 110, 53–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tal, A., Niemann, S., & Wansink, B. (2017). Depicted serving size: cereal packaging pictures exaggerate serving sizes and promote overserving. BMC Public Health, 17(1), 169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, R. H., & Sunstein, C. (2008). Nudge: improving decisions about health, wealth and happiness. London: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toepel, U., Bielser, M. L., Forde, C., Martin, N., Voirin, A., le Coutre, J., Murray, M. M., & Hudry, J. (2015). Brain dynamics of meal size selection in humans. NeuroImage, 113, 133–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Laan, L. N., De Ridder, D. T. D., Viergever, M. A., & Smeets, P. A. (2011). The first taste is always with the eyes: a meta-analysis on the neural correlates of processing visual food cues. NeuroImage, 55(1), 296–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Ittersum, K., & Wansink, B. (2012). Plate size and color suggestibility: the Delboeuf Illusion’s bias on serving and eating behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(2), 215–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Velasco, C., & Spence, C. (Eds.). (2019). Multisensory packaging: designing new product experiences. Cham: Palgrave MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Versluis, I., & Papies, E. K. (2016). The role of social norms in the portion size effect: reducing normative relevance reduces the effect of portion size on consumption decisions. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 756.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wansink, B., Painter, J. E., & North, J. (2005). Bottomless bowls: why visual cues of portion size may influence intake. Obesity Research, 13(1), 93–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xie, H., Minton, E. A., & Kahle, L. R. (2016). Cake or fruit? Influencing healthy food choice through the interaction of automatic and instructed mental simulation. Marketing Letters, 27(4), 627–644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Olivia Petit.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix 1. The Delboeuf illusion

figure a

Appendix 2. Study 1 stimuli

Delboeuf illusion shown with pizza on a plate printed on the front of the packaging. The visual presentation giving rise to the larger portion illusion (left) was rated as looking 17% larger than the visual presentation giving rise to the smaller portion illusion (right).

figure b

Appendix 3. Study 2 stimuli

Delboeuf illusion shown with cereal bowls printed on the front of the product packaging. The larger portion illusion (left) was rated as looking 27% more calorific than the smaller-looking portion illusion (right).

figure c

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Petit, O., Velasco, C. & Spence, C. Are large portions always bad? Using the Delboeuf illusion on food packaging to nudge consumer behavior. Mark Lett 29, 435–449 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-018-9473-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-018-9473-6

Keywords

Navigation