Skip to main content
Log in

Long-form recording of infant body position in the home using wearable inertial sensors

  • Original Manuscript
  • Published:
Behavior Research Methods Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Long-form audio recordings have had a transformational effect on the study of infant language acquisition by using mobile, unobtrusive devices to gather full-day, real-time data that can be automatically scored. How can we produce similar data in service of measuring infants’ everyday motor behaviors, such as body position? The aim of the current study was to validate long-form recordings of infant position (supine, prone, sitting, upright, held by caregiver) based on machine learning classification of data from inertial sensors worn on infants’ ankles and thighs. Using over 100 h of video recordings synchronized with inertial sensor data from infants in their homes, we demonstrate that body position classifications are sufficiently accurate to measure infant behavior. Moreover, classification remained accurate when predicting behavior later in the session when infants and caregivers were unsupervised and went about their normal activities, showing that the method can handle the challenge of measuring unconstrained, natural activity. Next, we show that the inertial sensing method has convergent validity by replicating age differences in body position found using other methods with full-day data captured from inertial sensors. We end the paper with a discussion of the novel opportunities that long-form motor recordings afford for understanding infant learning and development.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

A Databrary repository (https://nyu.databrary.org/volume/1580) includes an exemplar participant’s recording session, with the raw video data files, the Datavyu annotations of those video files, a log file with machine-readable synchronization points and nap/diaper change times, and accelerometer and gyroscope data for each of the four sensors. A GitHub repository (https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.8312007) contains the exemplar participant’s data and source code to: (1) synchronize IMU and video annotations, (2) calculate windowed motion features for their data, and (3) train and test the body position classifier using an “individual model”. Because of the overall size of the full dataset and the computational power/time required to synchronize and create windowed datasets for each session, it would not be feasible to reproduce the calculations for all 34 sessions. However, in a second GitHub repository (https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.8310338) we share the full results of those computations: The dataset of windowed motion features with corresponding body position codes used to validate the method.

References

  • Adolph, K. E., & Robinson, S. R. (2011). Sampling development. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12, 411–423. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2011.608190

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Adolph, K. E., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2014). The costs and benefits of development: The transition from crawling to walking. Child Development Perspectives, 8, 187–192. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12085

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Adolph, K. E., Vereijken, B., & Denny, M. A. (1998). Learning to crawl. Child Development, 69, 1299–1312. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06213.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adolph, K. E., Robinson, S. R., Young, J. W., & Gill-Alvarez, F. (2008). What is the shape of developmental change? Psychological Review, 115, 527–543. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.115.3.527

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Airaksinen, M., Räsänen, O., Ilén, E., Häyrinen, T., Kivi, A., Marchi, V., et al. (2020). Automatic posture and movement tracking of infants with wearable movement sensors. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Airaksinen, M., Gallen, A., Kivi, A., Vijayakrishnan, P., Häyrinen, T., Ilén, E., …, Vanhatalo, S. (2022). Intelligent wearable allows out-of-the-lab tracking of developing motor abilities in infants. Communications Medicine, 2(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/s43856-022-00131-6

  • Arif, M., & Kattan, A. (2015). Physical activities monitoring using wearable acceleration sensors attached to the body. PLoS ONE, 10, e0130851.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Aust, F., Barth, M. (2022). papaja: Prepare reproducible APA journal articles with R Markdown. Retrieved from https://github.com/crsh/papaja. Accessed 10 Feb 2023.

  • Bergelson, E., Amatuni, A., Dailey, S., Koorathota, S., & Tor, S. (2019). Day by day, hour by hour: Naturalistic language input to infants. Developmental Science, 22, e12715.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Breiman, L. (2001). Random forests. Machine Learning, 45, 5–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruijns, B. A., Truelove, S., Johnson, A. M., Gilliland, J., & Tucker, P. (2020). Infants’ and toddlers’ physical activity and sedentary time as measured by accelerometry: A systematic review and meta-analysis. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 17(1), 14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Casillas, M., Elliott, M. (2021). Cross-cultural differences in children’s object handling at home. PsyArXiv.

  • Chen, Q., Schneider, J. L., West, K. L., Iverson, J. M. (2022). Infant locomotion shapes proximity to adults during everyday play in the U.S. Infancy. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12503

  • Clerkin, E. M., Hart, E., Rehg, J. M., Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2017). Real-world visual statistics and infants’ first-learned object names. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 372, 20160055.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cliff, D. P., Reilly, J. J., & Okely, A. D. (2009). Methodological considerations in using accelerometers to assess habitual physical activity in children aged 0–5 years. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 12(5), 557–567.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cristia, A., Lavechin, M., Scaff, C., Soderstrom, M., Rowland, C., Räsänen, O., …, Bergelson, E. (2020). A thorough evaluation of the language environment analysis (LENA) system. Behavior Research Methods, 53(2), 467–486. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-020-01393-5

  • Dancho, M., & Vaughan, D. (2023). Timetk: A tool kit for working with time series in R.

  • de Barbaro, K. (2019). Automated sensing of daily activity: A new lens into development. Developmental Psychobiology, 61(3), 444–464.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • de Barbaro, Kaya, & Fausey, C. M. (2022). Ten lessons about infants’ everyday experiences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 31(1), 28–33. https://doi.org/10.1177/09637214211059536

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Franchak, J. M. (2019). Changing opportunities for learning in everyday life: Infant body position over the first year. Infancy, 24, 187–209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Franchak, J. M. (2020). The ecology of infants’ perceptual-motor exploration. Current Opinion in Psychology, 32, 110–114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Franchak, J. M., Kretch, K. S., & Adolph, K. E. (2018). See and be seen: Infant-caregiver social looking during locomotor free play. Developmental Science, 21, e12626.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Franchak, J. M., Scott, V., Luo, C. (2021). A contactless method for measuring full-day, naturalistic motor behavior using wearable inertial sensors. Frontiers in Psychology, 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.701343

  • Galland, B. C., Taylor, B. J., Elder, D. E., & Herbison, P. (2012). Normal sleep patterns in infants and children: A systematic review of observational studies. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 16(3), 213–222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2011.06.001

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenspan, B., Cunha, A. B., & Lobo, M. A. (2021). Design and validation of a smart garment to measure positioning practices of parents with young infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 62, 101530.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grolemund, G., & Wickham, H. (2011). Dates and times made easy with lubridate. Journal of Statistical Software, 40(3), 1–25.

  • Herzberg, O., Fletcher, K. K., Schatz, J. L., Adolph, K. E., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2021). Infant exuberant object play at home: Immense amounts of time-distributed, variable practice. Child Development, 93(1), 150–164.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kachergis, G., Yu, C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2017). A bootstrapping model of frequency and context effects in word learning. Cognitive Science, 41(3), 590–622.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kadooka, K., Caufield, M., Fausey, C. M., Franchak, J. M. (2021, April). Visuomotor learning opportunities are nested within everyday activities. Paper Presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development.

  • Karasik, L. B., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Adolph, K. E. (2011). Transition from crawling to walking and infants’ actions with objects and people. Child Development, 82, 1199–1209. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01595.x

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Karasik, L. B., Kuchirko, Y., Dodojonova, R. M., Elison, J. T. (2022). Comparison of U.S. And Tajik infants’ time in containment devices. Infant and Child Development, 31(4). https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2340

  • Kretch, K. S., Franchak, J. M., & Adolph, K. E. (2014). Crawling and walking infants see the world differently. Child Development, 85, 1503–1518. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12206

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kwon, S., Zavos, P., Nickele, K., Sugianto, A., & Albert, M. V. (2019). Hip and wrist-worn accelerometer data analysis for toddler activities. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 16(14), 2598.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33(1), 159. https://doi.org/10.2307/2529310

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liaw, A., Wiener, M., et al. (2002). Classification and regression by randomForest. R News, 2(3), 18–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lobo, M. A., Hall, M. L., Greenspan, B., Rohloff, P., Prosser, L. A., & Smith, B. A. (2019). Wearables for pediatric rehabilitation: How to optimally design and use products to meet the needs of users. Physical Therapy, 99(6), 647–657.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Luo, C., & Franchak, J. M. (2020). Head and body structure infants’ visual experiences during mobile, naturalistic play. PLoS ONE, 15, e0242009.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Majnemer, A., & Barr, R. G. (2005). Influence of supine sleep positioning on early motor milestone acquisition. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 47, 370–376.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malachowski, L. G., Salo, V. C., Needham, A. W., Humphreys, K. L. (2023). Infant placement and language exposure in daily life. Infant and Child Development.https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2405

  • Mendoza, J. K., Fausey, C. M. (2022). Everyday parameters for episode-to-episode dynamics in the daily music of infancy. Cognitive Science, 46(8). https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.13178

  • Nam, Y., & Park, J. W. (2013). Child activity recognition based on cooperative fusion model of a triaxial accelerometer and a barometric pressure sensor. IEEE Journal of Biomedical and Health Informatics, 17, 420–426.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Patel, P., Shi, Y., Hajiaghajani, F., Biswas, S., & Lee, M.-H. (2019). A novel two-body sensor system to study spontaneous movements in infants during caregiver physical contact. Infant Behavior and Development, 57, 101383. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2019.101383

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perry, L. K., Prince, E. B., Valtierra, A. M., Rivero-Fernandez, C., Ullery, M. A., Katz, L. F., …, Messinger, D. S. (2018). A year in words: The dynamics and consequences of language experiences in an intervention classroom. PLOS ONE, 13(7), e0199893. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199893

  • Preece, S. J., Goulermas, J. Y., Kenney, L. P. J., & Howard, D. (2009). A comparison of feature extraction methods for the classification of dynamic activities from accelerometer data. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 56, 871–879.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raz, H. K., Abney, D. H., Crandall, D., Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2019). How do infants start learning object names in a sea of clutter? Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 521–526.

  • Ren, X., Ding, W., Crouter, S. E., Mu, Y., & Xie, R. (2016). Activity recognition and intensity estimation in youth from accelerometer data aided by machine learning. Applied Intelligence, 45(2), 512–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, T., Narayanan, A., Hedayatrad, L., Neville, J., Mackay, L., & Duncan, S. (2018). A dual-accelerometer system for classifying physical activity in children and adults. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 50(12), 2595–2602.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Custode, S., Kuchirko, Y., Escobar, K., & Lo, T. (2018). Routine language: Speech directed to infants during home activities. Child Development, 90(6), 2135–2152. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13089

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thurman, S. L., & Corbetta, D. (2017). Spatial exploration and changes in infant-mother dyads around transitions in infant locomotion. Developmental Psychology, 53, 1207–1221.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warlaumont, A. S., Sobowale, K., & Fausey, C. M. (2021). Daylong mobile audio recordings reveal multitimescale dynamics in infants’ vocal productions and auditory experiences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 31(1), 12–19. https://doi.org/10.1177/09637214211058166

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wass, S., Phillips, E., Smith, C., Fatimehin, E. O., Goupil, L. (2022). Vocal communication is tied to interpersonal arousal coupling in caregiver-infant dyads. eLife, 11. https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.77399

  • Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24, 2143–2152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yao, X., Plötz, T., Johnson, M., & de Barbaro, K. (2019). Automated detection of infant holding using wearable sensing: Implications for developmental science and intervention. Proceedings of the ACM on Interactive, Mobile, Wearable and Ubiquitous Technologies, 3(2), 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author note

We are grateful to Vanessa Scott, Tasnia Haider, and Ishapreet Kaur for their help in collecting data for the present study and to the research assistants of the UCR Perception, Action, and Development Lab for annotating videos.

Funding

This work was funded by National Science Foundation Grant BCS #1941449.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John M. Franchak.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors have no competing interests to declare that are relevant to the content of this article.

Ethics approval

The study was performed in accordance with the ethical standards as laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. The study procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of California, Riverside, Protocol HS-15-050.

Consent to participate

All caregivers provided written informed consent prior to the start of the study

Consent to publish

Additional written consent was obtained by caregivers for data sharing of audio and video data.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Franchak, J.M., Tang, M., Rousey, H. et al. Long-form recording of infant body position in the home using wearable inertial sensors. Behav Res (2023). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-023-02236-9

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-023-02236-9

Keywords

Navigation