Skip to main content
Log in

How to fit transfer models to learning data: a segmentation/clustering approach

  • Published:
Behavior Research Methods Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although transfer models are limited in their ability to evolve over time and account for a wide range of processes, they have repeatedly shown to be useful for testing categorization theories and predicting participants’ generalization performance. In this study, we propose a statistical framework that allows transfer models to be applied to category learning data. Our framework uses a segmentation/clustering technique specifically tailored to suit category learning data. We applied this technique to a well-known transfer model, the Generalized Context Model, in three novel experiments that manipulated ordinal effects in category learning. The difference in performance across the three contexts, as well as the benefit of the rule-based order observed in two out of three experiments, were mostly detected by the segmentation/clustering method. Furthermore, the analysis of the segmentation/clustering outputs using the backward learning curve revealed that participants’ performance suddenly improved, suggesting the detection of an “eureka” moment. Our adjusted segmentation/clustering framework allows transfer models to fit learning data while capturing relevant patterns.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10
Fig. 11
Fig. 12
Fig. 13
Fig. 14

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, J. (1991). The adaptive nature of human categorization. Psychological Review, 98, 409–429.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, F. G., & Maddox, W. T. (1992). Complex decision rules in categorization: Contrasting novice and experienced performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(1), 50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Busemeyer J, Diederich A (2010) Cognitive Modeling. SAGE Publications, Inc

  • Carvalho, P. F., & Goldstone, R. L. (2015). What you learn is more than what you see: What can sequencing effects tell us about inductive category learning? Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 505.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Carvalho, P. F., & Goldstone, R. L. (2022). A computational model of context-dependent encodings during category learning. Cognitive Science, 46(4), e13128.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Clapper, J. P., & Bower, G. H. (1994). Category invention in unsupervised learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 443–460.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clapper, J. P., & Bower, G. H. (2002). Adaptative categorization in unsupervised learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 908–923.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cox, D. R. (1972). Regression models and life-tables. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B (Methodological), 34, 187–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davies, J., Wilson, I., & Lam, W. (2005). Array cgh technologies and their applications to cancer genomes. Chromosome Research, 13, 237–248.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elio, R., & Anderson, J. (1981). The effects of category generalizations and instance similarity on schema abstraction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 7, 397–417.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elio, R., & Anderson, J. R. (1984). The effects of information order and learning mode on schema abstraction. Memory & Cognition, 12, 20–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, J. (2000). Minimization of boolean complexity in human concept learning. Nature, 407, 630–633.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, J. (2003). A catalog of boolean concepts. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 47(1), 75–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garner, W. (1974). The processing of information and structure. Potomac, MD: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gluck M, Bower G (1988) A configural-cue network model of classification learning. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 26(6).

  • Hayes, K. J. (1953). The backward curve: a method for the study of learning. Psychological Review, 60(4), 269.

  • Hintzman, D. L. (1984). Minerva 2: A simulation model of human memory. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 16, 96–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hupé P, Stransky N, Thiery J, Radvanyi E F andBarillot (2004) Analysis of array cgh data: from signal ratio to gain and loss of dna regions. Bioinformatics 20:3413–3422.

  • James A, Reynaud-Bouret P, Mezzadri G, Sargolini F, Bethus I, Muzy A (2022) Strategy inference during learning via cognitive activity-based credit assignment models. Scientific Reports 13:9408

  • Kaplan, E. L., & Meier, P. (1958). Nonparametric estimation from incomplete observations. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 53, 457–481.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruschke, J. K. (1992). Alcove: an exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning. Psychological Review, 99, 22–44.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kruschke JK (2008) Models of categorization. The Cambridge handbook of computational psychology pp 267–301

  • Lavielle, M. (2005). Using penalized contrasts for the change-point problem. Signal Processing, 85(8), 1501–1510.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieto, A. (2021). Cognitive design for artificial minds. Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Love, B., Medin, D., & Gureckis, T. (2004). Sustain: a network model of category learning. Psychological Review, 111, 309–332.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathy, F., & Feldman, J. (2009). A rule-based presentation order facilitates category learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 1050–1057.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathy, F., & Feldman, J. (2016). The influence of presentation order on category transfer. Experimental psychology, 63, 59–69.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Medin, D. L., & Schaffer, M. M. (1978). Context theory of classification learning. Psychological Review, 85, 207–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mezzadri, G., Laloë, T., Mathy, F., & Reynaud-Bouret, P. (2022a). Hold-out strategy for selecting learning models: application to categorization subjected to presentation orders. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 109, 102691.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mezzadri, G., Reynaud-Bouret, P., Laloë, T., & Mathy, F. (2022b). Investigating interactions between types of order in categorization. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 21625.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mezzadri, G., Reynaud-Bouret, P., Laloë, T., & Mathy, F. (2022c). An order-dependent transfer model in categorization. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 107, 102634.

    Google Scholar 

  • Minda, J., & Smith, J. (2002). Comparing prototype-based and exemplar-based accounts of category learning and attentional allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 275–292.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nosofsky, R. M. (1986). Attention, similarity, and the identification-categorization relationship. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 39–57.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nosofsky, R. M., Palmeri, T. J., & McKinley, S. C. (1994). Rule-plus-exception model of classification learning. Psychological Review, 101(1), 53.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nosofsky, R. M., Sanders, C. A., & McDaniel, M. A. (2017). Tests of an exemplar-memory model of classification learning in a high-dimensional natural-science category domain. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147, 328–353.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nosofsky, R. M., Sanders, C., & Mcdaniel, M. (2018). A formal psychological model of classification applied to natural-science category learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27, 129–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olshen, A., Venkatraman, E., Lucito, R., & Wigler, M. (2004). Circular binary segmentation for the analysis of array-based dna copy number data. Biostatistics, 5, 557–572.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Picard, F., Robin, S., Lebarbier, E., & Daudin, J. J. (2007). A segmentation/clustering model for the analysis of array cgh data. Biometrics, 63, 758–66.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Polk TA, Seifert CM (2002) Cognitive modeling. Boston Review

  • Pothos, E. M., & Wills, A. J. (2011). Formal Approaches in Categorization. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reed, S. K. (1972). Pattern recognition and categorization. Cognitive psychology, 3(3), 382–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rehder, B., & Hoffman, A. (2005). Thirty-something categorization results explained: Attention, eyetracking, and models of category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 811–829.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rouder, J., & Ratcliff, R. (2004). Comparing categorization models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 63–82.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders C, Nosofsky R (2020) Training deep networks to construct a psychological feature space for a natural-object category domain. Computational Brain & Behavior pp 1–23

  • Shepard, R. N. (1964). Attention and the metric structure of the stimulus space. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 1, 54–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R. N. (1987). Toward a universal law of generalization for psychological science. Science, 237, 1317–1323.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R. N., Hovland, C. I., & Jenkins, H. M. (1961). Learning and memorization of classifications. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 75, 1–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J., & Minda, J. (2000). Thirty categorization results in search of a model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 3–27.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J., Tracy, J., & Murray, M. (1993). Depression and categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 331–346.

  • Smith, J. D., Minda, J. P., & Washburn, D. A. (2004). Category learning in rhesus monkeys: a study of the shepard, hovland, and jenkins (1961) tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(3), 398.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sun, R. (2008). The Cambridge handbook of computational psychology. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wills AJ (2013) Models of categorization, Oxford University Press, p 346–357

  • Zeaman, D., & House, B. J. (1963). The role of attention in retardate discrimination learning. Handbook of mental deficiency New York: McGraw-Hill, 1(3), 159–223.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The present work was supported by the French government, through the UCAJedi and 3IA Côte d’Azur Investissements d’Avenir managed by the National Research Agency (ANR-15-IDEX-01 and ANR-19-P3IA-0002), directed by the National Research Agency with the ANR project ChaMaNe (ANR-19-CE40-0024-02) and by the interdisciplinary Institute for Modeling in Neuroscience and Cognition (NeuroMod) of the Université Côte d’Azur.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The initial idea of applying the segmentation/clustering technique to transfer models came from P.R.-B. Experiments were designed and supervised by F.M. Data analysis and coding were performed by G.M. The article was drafted by G.M. and critical revisions were provided by P.R.-B., T.L. and F.M. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Giulia Mezzadri.

Ethics declarations

Open practices statement

The data for all experiments and the computer code (including the code for reproducing figures) are publicly available in Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/zv4jf/?view_only=8403629c320d4abfa0906c59443dd4ee. None of the experiments was preregistered.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mezzadri, G., Laloë, T., Mathy, F. et al. How to fit transfer models to learning data: a segmentation/clustering approach. Behav Res 56, 2549–2568 (2024). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-023-02166-6

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-023-02166-6

Keywords

Navigation