Skip to main content
Log in

Cross-language positive priming disappears, negative priming does not: Evidence for two sources of selective inhibition

  • Published:
Memory & Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The authors used a unilingual and bilingual primed lexical decision task to investigate priming effects produced by attended and ignored words. In the unilingual experiment, accelerated lexical decisions to probe target words resulted when the word matched the preceding target word, whereas slowed lexical decisions to probe target words resulted when the word matched the preceding ignored nontarget word. In the bilingual (English-Spanish) experiment, between-language, rather than within-language, priming manipulations were used. Although the ignored repetition negative priming effect replicated across languages, cross-language attended repetition positive priming did not. This dissociation of priming effects in the inter- versus intralanguage priming conditions contradicts episodic retrieval accounts of negative priming that deny the existence of selective inhibitory processes. On the other hand, these results support an extension of inhibition-based accounts of negative priming, because they indicate that inhibition can operate at two levels of abstraction—local word and global—language simultaneously.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, J. R. (1983). A spreading activation theory of memory.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,22, 261–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C., &Spellman, B. A. (1995). On the status of inhibitory mechanisms in cognition: Memory retrieval as a model case.Psychological Review,102, 68–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B., Davies, P., &Richman, B. (1982).American heritage word frequency book. New York: American Heritage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, H.-C., &Ng, M.-L. (1989). Semantic facilitation and translation priming effects in Chinese-English bilinguals.Memory & Cognition,17, 454–462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A. M., &Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading activation theory of semantic processing.Psychological Review,85, 407–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costin, D. (1988). MacLab: A Macintosh system for psychology labs.Behavior Research, Methods, Instruments, & Computers,20, 197–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Groot, A. M. B., &Nas, G. L. (1991). Lexical representation of cognates and noncognates in compound bilinguals.Journal of Memory & Language,30, 90–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forbach, G. B., Stanners, R. F., &Hochhaus, L. (1974). Repetition and practice effects in a lexical decision task.Memory & Cognition,2, 337–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E. (1995). Negative priming from ignored distractors in visual search: A review.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,2, 145–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E. (1996). Cross-language priming from ignored words: Evidence for a common representational system in bilinguals.Journal of Memory & Language,35, 353–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Francis, W. N., &Kučera, H. (1982).Frequency analysis of English usage. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frenck-Mestre, C., &Prince, P. (1997). Second language autonomy.Journal of Memory & Language,37, 481–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuster, J. M. (1997).The prefrontal cortex: Anatomy, physiology, and neuropsychology of the frontal lobe (3rd ed.). Philadelphia: Lippincott-Raven.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M. A., &Faust, M. (1995). Skilled suppression. In F. Dempster & C. Brainerd (Eds.),Interference and inhibition in cognition (pp. 295–327). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Grainger, J., &Beauvillain, C. (1988). Associative priming in bilinguals: Some limitations of interlingual facilitation effects.Canadian Journal of Psychology,42, 261–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, D.W. (1986). Control, activation, and resource: A framework and a model for the control of speech in bilinguals.Brain & Language,27, 210–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, D. W. (1993). Towards a model of L2 comprehension and production. In R. Schreuder & B. Weltens (Eds.),The bilingual lexicon (pp. 249–277). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heredia, R. R. (1997). Bilingual memory and hierarchical models: A case for language dominance.Current Directions in Psychological Science,6, 34–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Houghton, G., &Tipper, S. P. (1994). A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.),Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 53–112). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M. J., May, C. P., Hasher, L., Rahhal, T., &Stolzfus, E. R. (1997). Dual mechanisms of negative priming.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,23, 632–650.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keatley, C. W., &de Gelder, B. (1992). The bilingual primed lexical decision task: Cross-language priming disappears with speeded responses.European Journal of Cognitive Psychology,4, 273–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keele, S. W., &Neill, W. T. (1978). Mechanisms of attention. In E. C. Carterette & M. P. Friedman (Eds.),Handbook of perception (Vol. 9, pp. 3–47). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirsner, K., Smith, M., Lockhart, R., King, M., &Jain, M. (1984). The bilingual lexicon: Language-specific units in an integrated network.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,23, 519–539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kroll, J. F., &Curley, J. (1988). Lexical memory in novice bilinguals: The role of concepts in retrieving second language words. In M. Grunenberg, P. Morris, & R. Sykes (Eds.),Practical aspects of memory (pp. 389–395). London: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kučera, H., &Francis, E. (1967).Computational analysis of presentday American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langley, L. K., Overmier, J. B., Knopman, D. S., &ProdHomme, M. M. (1998). Inhibition and habituation: Preserved mechanisms of attentional selection in aging and Alzheimers disease.Neuropsychology,12, 353–366.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, J. R., &Neumann, E. (1999). Testing for predicted patterns: When interest in the whole is greater than in some of its parts.Psychological Methods,4, 44–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization.Psychological Review,95, 492–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1990). Repetition priming and automaticity: Common underlying mechanisms?Cognitive Psychology,22, 1–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malley, G. B., &Strayer, D. L. (1995). Effect of stimulus repetition on positive and negative identity priming.Perception & Psychophysics,57, 657–667.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • May, C. P., Kane, M. J., &Hasher, L. (1995). Determinants of negative priming.Psychological Bulletin,118, 35–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, D. E., Schvaneveldt, R. W., &Ruddy, M. G. (1975). Loci of contextual effects on visual word recognition. In P. M. A. Rabbitt & S. Dornic (Eds.),Attention and performance V (pp. 98–118). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C. M. (1994). Negative priming depends on probetrial conflict: Where has all the inhibition gone?Perception & Psychophysics,56, 133–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Motter, B. C. (1998). Neurophysiology of visual attention. In R. Parasuraman (Ed.),The attentive brain (pp. 51–70). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In D. Besner & G. Humphreys (Eds.),Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 264–336). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neill, W. T. (1997). Episodic retrieval in negative priming and repetition priming.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,23, 1291–1305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neill, W. T., &Valdes, L. A. (1992). The persistence of negative priming: Steady-state or decay?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,18, 565–576.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neill, W. T., &Valdes, L. A. (1996). Facilitatory and inhibitory aspects of attention. In A. F. Kramer, M. G. H. Coles, & G. D. Logan (Eds.),Converging operations in the study of visual selective attention (pp. 77–106). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., Terry, K. M., &Gorfein, D. S. (1992). The persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,18, 993–1000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, E. (1993, June).Evidence for a relationship between interference and negative priming in selective attention. Poster presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Society, Chicago.

  • Neumann, E., Cherau, J. F., Hood, K. L., &Steinnagel, S. L. (1993). Does inhibition spread in a manner analogous to spreading activation?Memory,1, 81–105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, E., &DeSchepper, B. G. (1991). Costs and benefits of target activation and distractor inhibition in selective attention.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,17, 1136–1145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, E., &DeSchepper, B. G. (1992). An inhibition-based fan effect: Evidence for an active suppression mechanism in selective attention.Canadian Journal of Psychology,46, 1–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pavio, A., Clark, J. M., &Lambert, W. E. (1988). Bilingual dualcoding theory and semantic repetition effects on recall.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,14, 163–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pavio, A., Yuille, J. C., &Madigan, S. A. (1968). Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns.Journal of Experimental Psychology Monographs,76(1, Pt. 2), 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., &Rafal, R. D. (1987). Cognitive theories of attention and rehabilitation of attentional deficits. In R. J. Meier, L. Diller, & A. L. Benton (Eds.),Neuropsychological rehabilitation (pp. 182–201). London: Churchill & Livingston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruthruff, E., &Miller, J. (1995). Negative priming depends on ease of selection.Perception & Psychophysics,57, 715–723.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schadler, M., &Thissen, D. M. (1981). The development of automatic word recognition and reading skill.Memory & Cognition,9, 132–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiller, P. H. (1966). Developmental study of color-word interference.Journal of Experimental Psychology,72, 105–108.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schooler, C., Neumann, E., Caplan, L. J., &Roberts, B. R. (1997). Continued inhibitory capacity throughout adulthood: Conceptual negative priming in younger and older adults.Psychology & Aging,12, 667–674.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Segalowitz, N. (in press). Automaticity and attentional skill in fluent performance. In H. Riggenbach (Ed.),Perspectives on fluency. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

  • Stadler, M. A., &Hogan, M. E. (1996). Varieties of positive and negative priming.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,3, 87–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, S. P. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,37A, 571–590.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, S. P. (1992). Selection for action: The role of inhibitory mechanisms.Current Directions in Psychological Science,1, 105–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, S. P., &Driver, J. (1988). Negative priming between pictures and words in a selective attention task: Evidence for semantic processing of ignored stimuli.Memory & Cognition,16, 64–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, S. P., Lortie, C., &Baylis, G. C. (1992). Selective reaching: Evidence for action-centered attention.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,18, 891–905.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, S. P., &Milliken, B. (1996). Distinguishing between inhibition and episodic retrieval based accounts of negative priming. In A. F. Kramer, M. Coles, & G. D. Logan (Eds.),Converging operations in the study of visual selective attention (pp. 337–364). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, S. P., Weaver, B., Cameron, S., Brehaut, J. C., &Bastedo, J. (1991). Inhibitory mechanisms of attention in identification and localization tasks: Time course and disruption.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,17, 681–692.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tzelgov, J., &Eben-Ezra, S. (1992). Components of the between-language semantic priming effect.European Journal of Cognitive Psychology,4, 253–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tzelgov, J., Henik, A., &Leiser, D. (1990). Controlling Stroop interference: Evidence from a bilingual task.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,16, 760–771.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Studnitz, R. E., &Green, D. W. (1997). Lexical decision and language switching.International Journal of Bilingualism,1, 3–24.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ewald Neumann.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Neumann, E., McCloskey, M.S. & Felio, A.C. Cross-language positive priming disappears, negative priming does not: Evidence for two sources of selective inhibition. Mem Cogn 27, 1051–1063 (1999). https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03201234

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03201234

Keywords

Navigation