Next Article in Journal
Impact of the COVID-19 Lockdown on a Long-Term Care Facility: The Role of Social Contact
Next Article in Special Issue
Regular Vigorous-Intensity Physical Activity and Walking Are Associated with Divergent but not Convergent Thinking in Japanese Young Adults
Previous Article in Journal
Neural Circuits, Microtubule Processing, Brain’s Electromagnetic Field—Components of Self-Awareness
Previous Article in Special Issue
Using Brain-Breaks® as a Technology Tool to Increase Attitude towards Physical Activity among Students in Singapore
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Higher Handgrip Strength Is Linked to Better Cognitive Performance in Chinese Adults with Hypertension

1
Hunan Academy of Education Sciences, Changsha 225002, China
2
College of Sports Science, Jishou University, Jishou 416000, China
3
Research Group Neuroprotection, German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Leipziger Str. 44, 39120 Magdeburg, Germany
4
Department of Neurology, Medical Faculty, Otto von Guericke University, Leipziger Str. 44, 39120 Magdeburg, Germany
5
Health and Exercise Science Laboratory, Institute of Sports Science, Seoul National University, Seoul 08826, Korea
6
Physical Education Unit, School of Humanities and Social Science, The Chinese University of Hong Kong-Shenzhen, Shenzhen 518172, China
7
Institute of Urban Governance, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen 518060, China
8
Center for Cognitive and Brain Health, Northeastern University, Boston, MA 02115, USA
9
Beckman Institute, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL 61801, USA
10
Institute of KEEP Collaborative Innovation, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen 518060, China
11
Exercise Psychophysiology Laboratory, School of Psychology, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen 518060, China
12
Department of Physical Education, Limestone University, Gaffney, SC 29340, USA
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2021, 11(8), 985; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11080985
Submission received: 29 June 2021 / Revised: 16 July 2021 / Accepted: 19 July 2021 / Published: 25 July 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Brain Function and Health, Sports, and Exercise)

Abstract

:
Objective: There is growing evidence that in adults, higher levels of handgrip strength (HGS) are linked to better cognitive performance. However, the relationship between HGS and cognitive performance has not been sufficiently investigated in special cohorts, such as individuals with hypertension who have an intrinsically higher risk of cognitive decline. Thus, the purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between HGS and cognitive performance in adults with hypertension using data from the Global Ageing and Adult Health Survey (SAGE). Methods: A total of 4486 Chinese adults with hypertension from the SAGE were included in this study. Absolute handgrip strength (aHGS in kilograms) was measured using a handheld electronic dynamometer, and cognitive performance was assessed in the domains of short-term memory, delayed memory, and language ability. Multiple linear regression models were fitted to examine the association between relative handgrip strength (rHGS; aHGS divided by body mass index) and measures of cognitive performance. Results: Overall, higher levels of rHGS were associated with higher scores in short-term memory (β = 0.20) and language (β = 0.63) compared with the lowest tertiles of rHGS. In male participants, higher HGS was associated with higher scores in short-term memory (β = 0.31), language (β = 0.64), and delayed memory (β = 0.22). There were no associations between rHGS and cognitive performance measures in females. Conclusion: We observed that a higher level of rHGS was associated with better cognitive performance among hypertensive male individuals. Further studies are needed to investigate the neurobiological mechanisms, including sex-specific differences driving the relationship between measures of HGS and cognitive performance in individuals with hypertension.

1. Introduction

Hypertension is a major cause of cardiovascular diseases (e.g., stroke), affecting about 40% of the aging population globally [1]. The increasing incidence of hypertension characterized by high blood pressure has become an urgent public health problem, and nearly 60% of Chinese older adults are suffering from hypertension [2]. In this context, it is worth noting that hypertension can increase the risk of cognitive decline since it affects the brain negatively [3,4,5,6], although the neurobiological mechanisms driving the relationship between hypertension and cognitive performance are controversially discussed and not fully understood. In a study by Tzourio et al., a non-significant association between hypertension and cognitive performance was observed [7], whereas other studies reported that hypertension (i) was associated with structural brain damage, (ii) was associated with a deficit in cognitive performance, and (iii) could accelerate the development of dementia [3,5,8,9]. Moreover, cognitive impairments can affect individuals’ mental health and social functioning and even cause an increase in the economic burden to take care of individual symptoms caused by the disease [10]. Notably, it has also been observed that individuals with hypertension who meet established physical activity guidelines showed superior cognitive performance [11], thus buttressing the claim that regular physical exercises are important to ensure brain health and cognition in adults with hypertension [12]. Collectively, the available evidence shows that hypertension has a detrimental effect on cognitive health, and thus identifying risk and protective factors before a manifestation of cognitive impairment occurs is essential in individuals with hypertension.
Handgrip strength (HGS), an important indicator of physical function, has been linked to various physical health outcomes (e.g., increased risk of falls, depression, and pre-mortality) among older adults [13,14,15,16]. Moreover, there is mounting evidence that measures of HGS are associated with cognitive health [17,18,19]. In particular, previous studies have established a relationship between measures of HGS and cognitive performance in younger adults [20], middle-aged adults [21], and older adults [22,23]. For example, a longitudinal study of older adults indicated that the participants in the highest quartile of rHGS had a 50% lower risk to develop cognitive impairments compared with participants in the lowest quartile of rHGS [24]. In line with this finding, a cross-sectional study of obese females reported that individuals in the lowest tertile of aHGS had 84% higher odds for cognitive impairments compared with obese females in the highest tertile of aHGS [25]. In this context, there is also emerging evidence that biological sex could be an important moderator in the relationship between physical fitness and cognitive performance [26,27,28,29]. However, sex-specific effects in the relationships of HGS and cognitive performance have not been extensively investigated, even though there is some evidence that the association between HGS and prevalence of mild cognitive impairments is more pronounced in men [28]. Collectively, these findings imply that individuals with lower measures of HGS may have a higher risk to develop cognitive impairments and that this relationship is moderated by biological sex.
Despite the fact that the above-mentioned studies have shown that measures of HGS can be important markers to identify adults at higher risk for cognitive decline, it has not been sufficiently studied whether this finding can be generalized to an adult population with hypertension. Adults with hypertension are intrinsically at a higher risk to develop cognitive impairments (e.g., due to hypertension-related brain changes). To address this gap in the literature, this study investigated whether rHGS is associated with performance in specific cognitive subdomains in both male and female adults with hypertension. Thus, the current study will add novel evidence to the literature, as it is among the first studies that (i) investigated the relationship between rHGS and performance in specific cognitive domains in hypertensive adults and (ii) examined the moderating role of biological sex in the relationship between rHGS and cognitive performance. Based on the available evidence, we hypothesized that in adults with hypertension, higher levels of rHGS are associated with better cognitive performance.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Population

This cross-sectional study used publicly available data from the Global Ageing and Adult Health Survey (SAGE: http://www.who.int/healthinfo/sage/en accessed on 29 March 2021). This survey assessed data of a nationally representative sample of Chinese individuals and was carried out between 2007 and 2010 across eight provinces. All procedures of data collection were reported in a previous study in more detail [30]. In brief, well-trained interviewers conducted face-to-face household interview surveys (questionnaire) to collect the data. The survey response rate was 93%. The study was approved by the World Health Organization Ethical Review Committee and the Chinese Ethics Research Review Board of Peking University (IRB00001052-11014 and IRB00001052-11015). Each participant signed informed written consent before she/he participated in the experiment.
Data were extracted on demographic characteristics (e.g., age and sex), handgrip strength, cognitive performance, and diagnosed hypertension from the Chinese cohort of SAGE. All individuals without hypertension were excluded and were not considered in the data analysis. As a result, a total of 4486 individuals with hypertension were included for analysis in this study.

2.2. Hypertension Diagnosis

Participants were defined as hypertensive if they selected “Yes” on “Have you ever been diagnosed with high blood pressure (hypertension)?” or if they had high blood pressure values, which were systolic blood pressure (SBP) ≥140 mmHg and/or diastolic blood pressure (DBP) ≥90 mm Hg [31].

2.3. Handgrip Strength

Absolute handgrip strength (aHGS) in kilograms (kg) was measured by trained assessors using a Smedley’s hand dynamometer (Scandidact, Oldenvej 45, and 3490 Kvistgard, Denmark) [32]. The participants were seated in a comfortable chair with their feet flat on the ground. They were asked to keep their upper arm against their body and to bend their elbow to 90 degrees with their palm facing in (as if shaking hands). The trained assessors instructed the seated participants to hold the dynamometer. The participants were asked to squeeze the hand as hard as they could, and each hand was tested twice. The overall HGS was the average of the best aHGS for each hand. If the participants had any surgery on their hand, arm, or wrist in the past 3 months or arthritis or pain in the hand/wrist/arm, aHGS was not assessed for that hand. As previously described [33], we calculated relative handgrip strength (rHGS) by dividing overall HGS (i.e., the average of the best aHGS) by body mass index (BMI).

2.4. Cognitive Performance

Cognitive performance was assessed using (i) the Digit Span Forward and Backward Test, (ii) the Verbal Fluency Test, and (iii) the Verbal Recall Test.
The Digit Span Forward (DSF) and Digit Span Backward (DSB) tests were used to quantify short-term memory and working memory performance. The participants were required to repeat a set of orally presented items (i.e., numbers) while the level of difficulty was gradually increased by one item for each trial. Both the DSF and the DSB start with a sequence of two numbers (e.g., 9 − 5), and for each span, two trials are performed. For each trial, a new sequence of items was presented. The researcher stopped the test after two consecutive incorrect responses on the same set of items [34]. Performance was scored based on the maximum length of correctly remembered items. The highest achievable scores for the DSF and DSB were 9 points and 8 points, respectively.
The semantic Verbal Fluency Test (VFT) probes language abilities (i.e., lexical retrieval and production). Participants were required to name as many animals as they could within one minute. One point was given for a correctly named animal (nonrepeating) [35].
The Verbal Recall Test (VRT) was used to measure performance in delayed memory. An interviewer read 10 words for participants three times at a standardized pace. After a delay of 10 min, the participants were required to recall as many words as possible from the initially presented 10 words. One point was given for each correctly recalled word [36].

2.5. Independent Variables

Age, sex, years of education, setting (rural or urban), alcohol consumption in the past month, smoking (never, current, or past), chronic physical conditions (stroke and diabetes), and regular level of physical activity (PA; occupational and leisure PA) [37] were used as covariates in the statistical models. Please note that the classifications of stroke and diabetes were based on self-reports of existing diagnoses from medical professionals. In line with a previous study, PA was measured using the Global Physical Activity Questionnaire Version 2 (GPAQ-V2), which consists of 16 questions [38]. The GPAQ-V2, which was developed under the auspices of the World Health Organization (WHO), is an internationally recognized, reliable, and validated questionnaire that assesses levels of regular physical activity [39,40]. According to the PA calculation from a previous study [37], levels of physical activity were classified as ≥150 min/week (meeting the recommended guidelines) and <150 min/week (low PA).

2.6. Statistical Analysis

In this study, continuous variables are expressed as means and standard deviation (SD), whereas categorical variables are shown as percentages. On the basis of the known sex difference in aHGS and cognitive performance in later life [22], we analyzed males and females separately (see Table 1, Table 2 and Table 3). Multivariable linear regression models were used to estimate the associations between rHGS and cognitive performance (Digit Span Forward and Backward Test, Verbal Fluency Test, and Verbal Recall Test). As the interaction terms of rHGS and sex in our linear regression showed a significant difference in all cognitive performance tests (p < 0.05), we estimated the associations between rHGS and cognitive performance (Digit Span Forward and Backward Test, Verbal Fluency Test, and Verbal Recall Test) and adjusted the regression models for age, sex, education years, setting, alcohol consumption, smoking, chronic physical conditions, and physical activity. All statistical analyses were conducted using the Stata 15.0 (Stata Corp LP, College Station, Texas). The level of statistical significance for all statistical tests was set at p < 0.05 (two-tailed).

3. Results

The final sample included in this study consisted of 4486 adults with hypertension. Table 1 provides an overview of the demographic characteristics, rHGS, and cognitive performance. The mean age for the sample was 59.68 ± 0.19 for male participants and 59.39 ± 0.40 for female participants. Furthermore, our sample included more male participants (80.7%) than female participants (19.3%).
As shown in Table 1, we observed significant differences between male and female participants concerning the level of education, setting, alcohol consumption, smoking, level of regular occupational PA, and performance in DSF, DSB, and VFT (all p < 0.05).
Table 2 displays the characteristics of participants according to the tertile of HGS for male and female participants. We observed that age, DSF, DSB, VFT, and VRT varied as a function of the tertile of HGS (all p < 0.05).
Table 3 displays the adjusted associations between HGS and cognitive function scores in adults with hypertension. Overall, when controlling for age, sex, level of education, setting, alcohol consumption, smoking, level of regular occupational PA, and leisure PA, individuals in the highest tertile of rHGS performed better in DSF and VFT compared with individuals in the lowest tertile of HGS (p < 0.01). Independent associations were found between rHGS and performance in DSF and VRT (p < 0.05) for male participants in the tertile of moderate and high rHGS. In the highest tertile of rHGS, rHGS was associated with performance on the VFT (p < 0.01).

4. Discussion

The current study investigated the associations of rHGS and cognitive performance in a Chinese sample of hypertensive individuals recruited from the SAGE project. Overall, our findings suggest that in adults with hypertension, higher levels of rHGS are associated with better cognitive performance in short-term memory (assessed using the Digit Span Forward and Digit Span Backward tests) and language abilities (assessed using the Verbal Fluency test) after controlling for important covariates (e.g., age, sex). Furthermore, we observed pronounced sex differences, as an association between higher levels of rHGS and better performance on the DSF, VFT, and VRT could be observed in male participants but not in female participants.
Our findings are in line with previous studies showing a relationship between the level of HGS and scores in short-term memory, language, and delayed memory (but not working memory) [18,41]. For example, a scoping review suggested that higher levels of HGS were associated with a lower risk of cognitive impairments in older adults [18]. A prospective study by Alfaro-Acha et al. found that lower aHGS was associated with a deficit in cognitive function in 2160 older Mexican Americans over 7 years [41]. In a cross-sectional study of 449 adults without dementia from Sweden, Praetorius Bjork et al. showed that there are strong relationships between aHGS and short-term memory, delayed memory, and language abilities [42].
Furthermore, the current study substantiates the available evidence regarding the link between measures of HGS and cognitive performance in adults with hypertension. A comparable study observed a link between aHGS and better visuospatial abilities, episodic memory, orientation/attention, and overall cognitive function in a cohort of middle-aged and older adults with hypertension [43]. Thus, our study adds evidence to the current literature that (i) in adults with hypertension, the link between rHGS and cognitive performance does not only comprise visuospatial abilities, episodic memory, orientation/attention, and overall cognitive function but also encompasses short-term memory, delayed memory, and language abilities and (ii) there are pronounced sex-specific differences in the association between rHGS and cognitive performance.
There is some evidence from a cross-sectional study [29] and a meta-analysis of interventional studies [44] that females profit more from physical interventions than males, although this finding is not universal [45]. Even if the mentioned studies are not fully comparable with our study, the direction of the sex-specific differences in our study is somewhat surprising. As suggested by others, this could be caused by sex-specific differences in neurobiological mechanisms driving behavioral performance.
Overall, the findings of the current study suggest that higher levels of rHGS are linked to better cognitive performance, although the neurobiological mechanisms driving this relationship are yet not fully clear and warrant future investigation [46,47,48]. In this context, several possible mechanisms could explain the positive relationship between measures of HGS and cognitive performance. In particular, it has been proposed that the following levels of analysis need to be considered to understand the effect of physical activity or physical fitness (i.e., level of HGS) on cognitive performance: (i) level 1—molecular and cellular changes, (ii) level 2—functional and structural brain changes, and (iii) level 3—socioemotional changes [47,49].
Our knowledge concerning the relationship of measures of HGS and cognition-relevant changes in level 1 (cellular and molecular changes) is meager. With respect to the association between cardiovascular fitness and/or cardiovascular exercise and cognitive performance, brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) and other neurotrophic factors have been highlighted to play a crucial role [49,50,51]. In this regard, there is some evidence that (i) cardiorespiratory fitness level is associated with basal levels of BDNF [52,53], (ii) the lower levels of serum BDNF are associated with a smaller hippocampal volume and poorer memory performance [54], and (iii) changes in BDNF levels in response to long-term aerobic training are associated with changes in executive functions [55] and hippocampal volume [56]. Collectively, these studies suggest that BDNF plays a crucial role in the relationship between cardiorespiratory fitness and cognitive performance. However, comparable studies investigating the relationship between HGS, neurotrophic factors such as BDNF, and cognitive performance are, to the best of our knowledge, currently lacking. Thus, further studies investigating the relationship between HGS and neurotrophic factors (e.g., BNDF) are needed to elucidate the extent to which changes on this level of analysis can explain some variance in the positive relationship between measures of HGS and cognitive performance.
With respect to level 2 (functional and structural brain changes), it has been proposed that HGS and higher-order cognitive functions might share the same neural substrates [7]. Indeed, there is some evidence in the literature suggesting a close relationship between HGS and brain features relevant to higher-order cognitive processes. Concerning functional brain changes, it has been observed that in younger adults, higher levels of normalized HGS are linked to favorable cerebral hemodynamics [47]. However, in this study, normalized HGS was not linked to cognitive performance, nor did this study find convincing evidence that cortical hemodynamics mediate a possible relationship between normalized HGS and cognitive performance [7]. However, as this finding cannot be readily generalized on the basis of our cohort, additional studies are needed to elucidate whether HGS might influence cortical hemodynamics in adults with hypertension. Regarding structural brain changes, there is evidence in the literature linking higher levels of aHGS to greater hippocampal volume [57]. Given that a greater hippocampal volume [58] and an increase in hippocampal volume [56] are linked to better cognitive performance (i.e., spatial memory), it seems plausible to hypothesize that alterations in brain structure at least partly influence the relationship between higher levels of HGS and cognitive performance. This idea is reinforced by the fact that (i) resistance training improves hippocampal integrity in older adults with mild cognitive impairment [59,60] and (ii) the hippocampus mediates the relationship between higher levels of physical fitness (i.e., cardiorespiratory fitness) and spatial memory performance [56,58]. In this context, it could be speculated that the neurobiological processes leading to a higher HGS might be protective against the frequently reported hypertension-related worsening of brain integrity (e.g., faster decline in hippocampal volume and more global brain matter atrophy) and cognitive performance (e.g., more pronounced decline in executive function and memory) [61,62,63,64]. However, to the best of our knowledge, there is a lack of cross-sectional and longitudinal studies that assess and analyze the relationship between measures of HGS, brain structure, and cognitive performance. Thus, future investigations that provide empirical evidence to verify or refute the above-mentioned theoretical assumptions while considering sex-related differences in structural brain changes (e.g., those reported by intervention studies [44]) are urgently needed.
Level 3 (socioemotional changes) comprises changes in mood, stress, pain, and sleep. It is beyond the scope and intention of this article to discuss the relationship between HGS and these socioemotional factors. However, sleep has been highlighted as an important mediator in the relationship between physical activity and/or physical fitness and cognitive performance [49,65]. It was observed that unhealthy sleep patterns (e.g., too short or too long sleep duration and insufficient sleep quality) can be linked to weaker HGS and faster decline of HGS in adults [66,67,68]. Furthermore, empirical evidence suggests that sleep patterns mediate the relationship between regular physical activity and cognitive performance (e.g., inhibition performance) [69,70]. It seems reasonable to hypothesize that changes in socioemotional factors (e.g., sleep) and their influence on the other levels of analysis (e.g., the cellular and molecular level and the functional and structural level) can at least partly explain the positive relationships between measures of HGS and cognitive performance. However, given the lack of research in this direction, future studies are required to test these assumptions empirically.

Limitations

The findings of the current study need to be interpreted in light of some limitations. First, this cross-sectional study does not allow the assessment of causal mechanisms driving the relationship between rHGS and cognitive performance. Second, we used only rHGS as a predictor of physical performance, whereas other measures of physical performance (e.g., gait speed and sit-to-stand performance), which have been used in previous studies investigating the relationship between physical fitness and cognitive performance in hypertensive adults [43,71], were not analyzed because of a lack of available data. Third, the data assessment for the SAGE study was carried out between 2007 to 2010, which might affect the generalizability of the findings to a certain extent. However, given the fact that hypertension is still a major health issue, as it constitutes an important risk factor for heart diseases, stroke, chronic kidney disease, and dementia [72,73], we believe that the findings of the current study are still of interest for the scientific community.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, the findings of the current study suggest that a higher level of rHGS is related to better performance in specific domains of cognition (i.e., short-term memory, delayed memory, and language abilities) in our sample of Chinese adults with hypertension (especially in male adults). Further studies are needed to investigate the neurobiological mechanisms driving this relationship, including the identification of sex-specific differences.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, Y.Z. and L.Z.; methodology, S.L. and Y.Z.; software, S.L., C.J. and Y.Z.; validation, Y.Z., F.H., and Y.L.; formal analysis, F.H. and Y.Z.; investigation, F.H., Y.Z., J.L., Z.Y., Y.L., J.K., and T.W.; resources, Y.Z., Q.Y., and L.Z.; data curation, F.H. and Y.Z.; writing—original draft preparation, S.L., F.H., and Y.Z.; writing—review and editing, F.H., Y.Z., A.F.K., C.J., Q.Y., S.D., and L.Z.; project administration, Y.Z., C.J., and L.Z.; funding acquisition, S.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Social Science Foundation of Hunan Province (19YBA286) and the Key Research Project of Hunan Educational Bureau (19A407).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The open available data used in this study was approved by the World Health Organization Ethical Review Committee and the Chinese Ethics Research Review Board of Peking University (IRB00001052-11014 and IRB00001052-11015).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The publicly archived datasets can be retrieved from http://www.who.int/healthinfo/sage/en accessed on 29 March 2021.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Leung, A.A.; Daskalopoulou, S.S.; Dasgupta, K.; McBrien, K.; Butalia, S.; Zarnke, K.B.; Nerenberg, K.; Harris, K.C.; Nakhla, M.; Cloutier, L.; et al. Hypertension Canada’s 2017 Guidelines for Diagnosis, Risk Assessment, Prevention, and Treatment of Hypertension in Adults. Can. J. Cardiol. 2017, 33, 557–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Lloyd-Sherlock, P.; Beard, J.; Minicuci, N.; Ebrahim, S.; Chatterji, S. Hypertension among older adults in lowand middle-income countries: Prevalence, awareness and control. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2014, 43, 116–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Walker, K.A.; Power, M.C.; Gottesman, R.F. Defining the Relationship Between Hypertension, Cognitive Decline, and Dementia: A Review. Curr. Hypertens. Rep. 2017, 19, 24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Forte, G.; De Pascalis, V.; Favieri, F.; Casagrande, M. Effects of Blood Pressure on Cognitive Performance: A Systematic Review. J. Clin. Med. 2019, 9, 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Novak, V.; Hajjar, I. The relationship between blood pressure and cognitive function. Nat. Rev. Cardiol. 2010, 7, 686–698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Beauchet, O.; Celle, S.; Roche, F.; Bartha, R.; Montero-Odasso, M.; Allali, G.; Annweiler, C. Blood pressure levels and brain volume reduction: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Hypertens. 2013, 31, 1502–1516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Tzourio, C.; Laurent, S.; Debette, S. Is hypertension associated with an accelerated aging of the brain? Hypertension 2014, 63, 894–903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Hughes, T.M.; Sink, K.M. Hypertension and Its Role in Cognitive Function: Current Evidence and Challenges for the Future. Am. J. Hypertens. 2016, 29, 149–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Qiu, C.; Winblad, B.; Fratiglioni, L. The age-dependent relation of blood pressure to cognitive function and dementia. Lancet Neurol. 2005, 4, 487–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Fu, C.; Li, Z.; Mao, Z. Association between social activities and cognitive function among the elderly in china: A cross-sectional study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Frith, E.; Loprinzi, P.D. Physical activity and cognitive function among older adults with hypertension. J. Hypertens. 2017, 35, 1271–1275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Rêgo, M.L.M.; Cabral, D.A.R.; Costa, E.C.; Fontes, E.B. Physical Exercise for Individuals with Hypertension: It Is Time to Emphasize its Benefits on the Brain and Cognition. Clin. Med. Insights Cardiol. 2019, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. García-Hermoso, A.; Ramírez-Vélez, R.; Peterson, M.D.; Lobelo, F.; Cavero-Redondo, I.; Correa-Bautista, J.E.; Martínez-Vizcaíno, V. Handgrip and knee extension strength as predictors of cancer mortality: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sport. 2018, 28, 1852–1858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Mcgrath, R.; Robinson-Lane, S.G.; Cook, S.; Clark, B.C.; Herrmann, S.; O’connor, M.L.; Hackney, K.J. Handgrip Strength Is Associated with Poorer Cognitive Functioning in Aging Americans. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2019, 70, 1187–1196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. McGrath, R.; Vincent, B.M.; Hackney, K.J.; Robinson-Lane, S.G.; Downer, B.; Clark, B.C. The Longitudinal Associations of Handgrip Strength and Cognitive Function in Aging Americans. J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 2020, 21, 634–639.e1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. McGrath, R.; Cawthon, P.M.; Cesari, M.; Al Snih, S.; Clark, B.C. Handgrip Strength Asymmetry and Weakness Are Associated with Lower Cognitive Function: A Panel Study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2020, 68, 2051–2058. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Carson, R.G. Get a grip: Individual variations in grip strength are a marker of brain health. Neurobiol. Aging 2018, 71, 189–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Fritz, N.E.; McCarthy, C.J.; Adamo, D.E. Handgrip strength as a means of monitoring progression of cognitive decline—A scoping review. Ageing Res. Rev. 2017, 35, 112–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Sternäng, O.; Reynolds, C.A.; Finkel, D.; Ernsth-Bravell, M.; Pedersen, N.L.; Dahl Aslan, A.K. Grip strength and cognitive abilities: Associations in old age. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2016, 71, 841–848. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Choudhary, A.K.; Jiwane, R.; Alam, T.; Kishanrao, S.S. Grip Strength and Impact on Cognitive Function in Healthy Kitchen Workers. Achiev. Life Sci. 2016, 10, 168–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Adamo, D.E.; Anderson, T.; Koochaki, M.; Fritz, N.E. Declines in grip strength may indicate early changes in cognition in healthy middle-aged adults. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0232021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Yang, L.; Koyanagi, A.; Smith, L.; Hu, L.; Colditz, G.A.; Toriola, A.T.; Felipe López Sánchez, G.; Vancampfort, D.; Hamer, M.; Stubbs, B.; et al. Hand grip strength and cognitive function among elderly cancer survivors. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0197909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Veronese, N.; Smith, L.; Barbagallo, M.; Yang, L.; Zou, L.; Haro, J.M.; Koyanagi, A. Sarcopenia and fall-related injury among older adults in five low- and middle-income countries. Exp. Gerontol. 2021, 147, 111262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Kim, G.R.; Sun, J.; Han, M.; Nam, C.M.; Park, S. Evaluation of the directional relationship between handgrip strength and cognitive function: The Korean Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Age Ageing 2019, 48, 426–432. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  25. Jeong, S.M.; Choi, S.; Kim, K.; Kim, S.M.; Kim, S.; Park, S.M. Association among handgrip strength, body mass index and decline in cognitive function among the elderly women. BMC Geriatr. 2018, 18, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Barha, C.K.; Hsu, C.L.; ten Brinke, L.; Liu-Ambrose, T. Biological Sex: A Potential Moderator of Physical Activity Efficacy on Brain Health. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2019, 11, 329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Barha, C.K.; Liu-Ambrose, T. Exercise and the Aging Brain: Considerations for Sex Differences. Brain Plast. 2018, 4, 53–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Liu, X.; Chen, J.; Geng, R.; Wei, R.; Xu, P.; Chen, B.; Liu, K.; Yang, L. Sex- and age-specific mild cognitive impairment is associated with low hand grip strength in an older Chinese cohort. J. Int. Med. Res. 2020, 48, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Barha, C.K.; Best, J.R.; Rosano, C.; Yaffe, K.; Catov, J.M.; Liu-Ambrose, T. Sex-specific relationship between long-term maintenance of physical activity and cognition in the health ABC Study: Potential role of hippocampal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex volume. J. Gerontol. Ser. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2020, 75, 764–770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Kowal, P.; Chatterji, S.; Naidoo, N.; Biritwum, R.; Fan, W.; Ridaura, R.L.; Maximova, T.; Arokiasamy, P.; Phaswana-Mafuya, N.; Williams, S.; et al. Data resource profile: The world health organization study on global ageing and adult health (SAGE). Int. J. Epidemiol. 2012, 41, 1639–1649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Unger, T.; Borghi, C.; Charchar, F.; Khan, N.A.; Poulter, N.R.; Prabhakaran, D.; Ramirez, A.; Schlaich, M.; Stergiou, G.S.; Tomaszewski, M.; et al. 2020 International Society of Hypertension Global Hypertension Practice Guidelines. Hypertension 2020, 75, 1334–1357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Ramlagan, S.; Peltzer, K.; Phaswana-Mafuya, N. Hand grip strength and associated factors in non-institutionalised men and women 50 years and older in South Africa. BMC Res. Notes 2014, 7, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Kim, Y.M.; Kim, S.; Bae, J.; Kim, S.H.; Won, Y.J. Association between relative hand-grip strength and chronic cardiometabolic and musculoskeletal diseases in Koreans: A cross-sectional study. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 2021, 92, 104181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Wilke, J.; Stricker, V.; Usedly, S. Free-weight resistance exercise is more effective in enhancing inhibitory control than machine-based training: A randomized, controlled trial. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Zhao, Q.; Guo, Q.; Hong, Z. Clustering and switching during a semantic verbal fluency test contribute to differential diagnosis of cognitive impairment. Neurosci. Bull. 2013, 29, 75–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Gildner, T.E.; Liebert, M.A.; Kowal, P.; Chatterji, S.; Snodgrass, J.J. Associations between sleep duration, sleep quality, and cognitive test performance among older adults from six middle income countries: Results from the study on global ageing and adult health (SAGE). J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2014, 10, 613–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Vancampfort, D.; Stubbs, B.; Lara, E.; Vandenbulcke, M.; Swinnen, N.; Koyanagi, A. Mild cognitive impairment and physical activity in the general population: Findings from six low- and middle-income countries. Exp. Gerontol. 2017, 100, 100–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Armstrong, T.; Bull, F. Development of the World Health Organization Global Physical Activity Questionnaire (GPAQ). J. Public Health (Bangkok) 2006, 14, 66–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Bull, F.C.; Maslin, T.S.; Armstrong, T. Global physical activity questionnaire (GPAQ): Nine country reliability and validity study. J. Phys. Act. Heal. 2009, 6, 790–804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Keating, X.D.; Zhou, K.; Liu, X.; Hodges, M.; Liu, J.; Guan, J.; Phelps, A.; Castro-Piñero, J. Reliability and concurrent validity of global physical activity questionnaire (GPAQ): A systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 4128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Alfaro-Acha, A.; Al Snih, S.; Raji, M.A.; Kuo, Y.F.; Markides, K.S.; Ottenbacher, K.J. Handgrip strength and cognitive decline in older Mexican Americans. J. Gerontol. Ser. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2006, 61, 859–865. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Praetorius Bjork, M.; Johansson, B.; Hassing, L.B. I forgot when I lost my grip-strong associations between cognition and grip strength in level of performance and change across time in relation to impending death. Neurobiol. Aging 2016, 38, 68–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Zuo, M.; Gan, C.; Liu, T.; Tang, J.; Dai, J.; Hu, X. Physical Predictors of Cognitive Function in Individuals With Hypertension: Evidence from the CHARLS Basline Survey. West. J. Nurs. Res. 2019, 41, 592–614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Barha, C.K.; Davis, J.C.; Falck, R.S.; Nagamatsu, L.S.; Liu-Ambrose, T. Sex differences in exercise efficacy to improve cognition: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials in older humans. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2017, 46, 71–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Falck, R.S.; Davis, J.C.; Best, J.R.; Crockett, R.A.; Liu-Ambrose, T. Impact of exercise training on physical and cognitive function among older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurobiol. Aging 2019, 79, 119–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Herold, F.; Törpel, A.; Schega, L.; Müller, N.G. Functional and/or structural brain changes in response to resistance exercises and resistance training lead to cognitive improvements—A systematic review. Eur. Rev. Aging Phys. Act. 2019, 16, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Herold, F.; Behrendt, T.; Törpel, A.; Hamacher, D.; Müller, N.G.; Schega, L. Cortical hemodynamics as a function of handgrip strength and cognitive performance: A cross-sectional fNIRS study in younger adults. BMC Neurosci. 2021, 22, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Herold, F.; Müller, P.; Gronwald, T.; Müller, N.G. Dose–Response Matters!—A Perspective on the Exercise Prescription in Exercise–Cognition Research. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Stillman, C.M.; Cohen, J.; Lehman, M.E.; Erickson, K.I. Mediators of physical activity on neurocognitive function: A review at multiple levels of analysis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Stimpson, N.J.; Davison, G.; Javadi, A.H. Joggin’ the Noggin: Towards a Physiological Understanding of Exercise-Induced Cognitive Benefits. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018, 88, 177–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Walsh, E.I.; Smith, L.; Northey, J.; Rattray, B.; Cherbuin, N. Towards an understanding of the physical activity-BDNF-cognition triumvirate: A review of associations and dosage. Ageing Res. Rev. 2020, 60, 101044. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Schmalhofer, M.L.; Markus, M.R.P.; Gras, J.C.; Kopp, J.; Janowitz, D.; Grabe, H.J.; Groß, S.; Ewert, R.; Gläser, S.; Albrecht, D.; et al. Sex-Specific associations of brain-derived neurotrophic factor and cardiorespiratory fitness in the general population. Biomolecules 2019, 9, 630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  53. Currie, J.; Ramsbottom, R.; Ludlow, H.; Nevill, A.; Gilder, M. Cardio-respiratory fitness, habitual physical activity and serum brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in men and women. Neurosci. Lett. 2009, 451, 152–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Erickson, K.I.; Prakash, R.S.; Voss, M.W.; Chaddock, L.; Heo, S.; McLaren, M.; Pence, B.D.; Martin, S.A.; Vieira, V.J.; Woods, J.A.; et al. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor is associated with age-related decline in hippocampal volume. J. Neurosci. 2010, 30, 5368–5375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  55. Leckie, R.L.; Oberlin, L.E.; Voss, M.W.; Prakash, R.S.; Szabo-Reed, A.; Chaddock-Heyman, L.; Phillips, S.M.; Gothe, N.P.; Mailey, E.; Vieira-Potter, V.J.; et al. BDNF mediates improvements in executive function following a 1-year exercise intervention. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 101044. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Erickson, K.I.; Voss, M.W.; Prakash, R.S.; Basak, C.; Szabo, A.; Chaddock, L.; Kim, J.S.; Heo, S.; Alves, H.; White, S.M.; et al. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2011, 108, 3017–3022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Firth, J.A.; Smith, L.; Sarris, J.; Vancampfort, D.; Schuch, F.; Carvalho, A.F.; Solmi, M.; Yung, A.R.; Stubbs, B.; Firth, J. Handgrip strength is associated with hippocampal volume and white matter hyperintensities in major depression and healthy controls: A UK biobank study. Psychosom. Med. 2020, 82, 39–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Erickson, K.I.; Prakash, R.S.; Voss, M.W.; Chaddock, L.; Hu, L.; Morris, K.S.; White, S.M.; Wójcicki, T.R.; McAuley, E.; Kramer, A.F. Aerobic fitness is associated with hippocampal volume in elderly humans. Hippocampus 2009, 19, 1030–1039. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Suo, C.; Singh, M.F.; Gates, N.; Wen, W.; Sachdev, P.; Brodaty, H.; Saigal, N.; Wilson, G.C.; Meiklejohn, J.; Singh, N.; et al. Therapeutically relevant structural and functional mechanisms triggered by physical and cognitive exercise. Mol. Psychiatry 2016, 21, 1633–1642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Broadhouse, K.M.; Singh, M.F.; Suo, C.; Gates, N.; Wen, W.; Brodaty, H.; Jain, N.; Wilson, G.C.; Meiklejohn, J.; Singh, N.; et al. Hippocampal plasticity underpins long-term cognitive gains from resistance exercise in MCI. NeuroImage Clin. 2020, 25, 102182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Debette, S.; Seshadri, S.; Beiser, A.; Au, R.; Himali, J.J.; Palumbo, C.; Wolf, P.A.; DeCarli, C. Midlife vascular risk factor exposure accelerates structural brain aging and cognitive decline. Neurology 2011, 77, 461–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  62. Shah, C.; Srinivasan, D.; Erus, G.; Schmitt, J.E.; Agarwal, A.; Cho, M.E.; Lerner, A.J.; Haley, W.E.; Kurella Tamura, M.; Davatzikos, C.; et al. Changes in brain functional connectivity and cognition related to white matter lesion burden in hypertensive patients from SPRINT. Neuroradiology 2021, 63, 913–924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Muller, M.; Sigurdsson, S.; Kjartansson, O.; Aspelund, T.; Lopez, O.L.; Jonnson, P.V.; Harris, T.B.; Van Buchem, M.; Gudnason, V.; Launer, L.J. Joint effect of mid- and late-life blood pressure on the brain: The AGES-Reykjavik Study. Neurology 2014, 82, 2187–2195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  64. Firbank, M.J.; Wiseman, R.M.; Burton, E.J.; Saxby, B.K.; O’Brien, J.T.; Ford, G.A. Brain atrophy and white matter hyperintensity change in older adults and relationship to blood pressure: Brain atrophy, WMH change and blood pressure. J. Neurol. 2007, 254, 713–721. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Stillman, C.M.; Esteban-Cornejo, I.; Brown, B.; Bender, C.M.; Erickson, K.I. Effects of Exercise on Brain and Cognition Across Age Groups and Health States. Trends Neurosci. 2020, 43, 533–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Selvamani, Y.; Arokiasamy, P.; Chaudhary, M. Himanshu Association of sleep problems and sleep duration with self-rated health and grip strength among older adults in India and China: Results from the study on global aging and adult health (SAGE). J. Public Health 2018, 26, 697–707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Wang, T.Y.; Wu, Y.; Wang, T.; Li, Y.; Zhang, D. A prospective study on the association of sleep duration with grip strength among middle-aged and older Chinese. Exp. Gerontol. 2018, 103, 88–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Auyeung, T.W.; Kwok, T.; Leung, J.; Lee, J.S.W.; Ohlsson, C.; Vandenput, L.; Wing, Y.K.; Woo, J. Sleep Duration and Disturbances Were Associated With Testosterone Level, Muscle Mass, and Muscle Strength-A Cross-Sectional Study in 1274 Older Men. J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 2015, 16, 630.e1–630.e6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Wilckens, K.A.; Erickson, K.I.; Wheeler, M.E. Physical Activity and Cognition: A Mediating Role of Efficient Sleep. Behav. Sleep Med. 2018, 16, 569–586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Li, L.; Yu, Q.; Zhao, W.; Herold, F.; Cheval, B.; Kong, Z.; Li, J.; Mueller, N.; Kramer, A.F.; Cui, J.; et al. Physical Activity and Inhibitory Control: The Mediating Role of Sleep Quality and Sleep Efficiency. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Basile, G.; Catalano, A.; Mandraffino, G.; Crucitti, A.; Ciancio, G.; Morabito, N.; Lasco, A. Cognitive impairment and slow gait speed in elderly outpatients with arterial hypertension: The effect of blood pressure values. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2015, 63, 1260–1261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Zhou, B.; Perel, P.; Mensah, G.A.; Ezzati, M. Global epidemiology, health burden and effective interventions for elevated blood pressure and hypertension. Nat. Rev. Cardiol. 2021, 1–18. [Google Scholar]
  73. Ungvari, Z.; Toth, P.; Tarantini, S.; Prodan, C.I.; Sorond, F.; Merkely, B.; Csiszar, A. Hypertension-induced cognitive impairment: From pathophysiology to public health. Nat. Rev. Nephrol. 2021, 1–16. [Google Scholar]
Table 1. Overview of demographic characteristics and measures of handgrip strength and cognitive performance.
Table 1. Overview of demographic characteristics and measures of handgrip strength and cognitive performance.
Variables Male (3620)Female (866)
Age (in years) 59.68 ± 0.1959.39 ± 0.40
Education (in years) 7.54 ± 0.076.59 ± 0.18
Setting (in %)Rural59.0152.89
Urban40.9947.11
Alcohol consumption (in %)Yes70.8053.00
No29.2047.00
Smoking (in %)Never20.7586.14
Current64.7110.00
Past14.543.86
Work physical activity (in %)≤150 min/week76.2483.72
>150 min/week23.7616.28
Leisure physical activity (in %)≤150 min/week97.2997.69
>150 min/week2.712.31
Stroke (in %)Yes3.152.08
No96.8597.92
Diabetes (in %)Yes4.634.52
No95.3795.48
Handgrip strength (kg) 33.74 ± 0.1822.95 ± 0.28
Digit span forward (score) 7.33 ± 0.037.21 ± 0.05
Digit span backward (score) 3.67 ± 0.023.06 ± 0.05
Verbal fluency (score) 13.82 ± 0.0812.77 ± 0.15
Delay recall (score) 5.19 ± 0.045.13 ± 0.08
Table 2. Participants’ characteristics based on the tertiles of handgrip strength in male and female participants.
Table 2. Participants’ characteristics based on the tertiles of handgrip strength in male and female participants.
VariablesQ3 (1.579–6.347)Q2 (1.147–1.578)Q1 (0.224–1.146)p Value
Male
Age (n = 3421) (in years)55.46 ± 0.3060.49 ± 0.2965.21 ± 0.37p < 0.001
Handgrip strength (kg)42.46 ± 0.2332.68 ± 0.1421.30 ± 0.22p < 0.001
Digit span forward (n = 3414) (score)7.61 ± 0.047.37 ± 0.046.90 ± 0.05p < 0.001
Digit span backward (n = 3406) (score)3.81 ± 0.043.60 ± 0.043.50 ± 0.05p < 0.001
Verbal fluency (n = 3414) (score)14.53 ± 0.1413.80 ± 0.1412.84 ± 0.17p < 0.001
Delay recall (n = 3409) (score)5.58 ± 0.065.22 ± 0.064.67 ± 0.08p < 0.001
Female
Age (n = 829) (in years)53.98 ± 1.5355.22 ± 0.7661.20 ± 0.47p < 0.001
Handgrip strength (kg)37.23 ± 1.0029.23 ± 0.2819.29 ± 0.25p < 0.001
Digit span forward (n = 829) (score)7.41 ± 0.197.51 ± 0.097.13 ± 0.06p = 0.02
Digit span backward (n = 827) (score)3.57 ± 0.153.25 ± 0.092.96 ± 0.06p < 0.001
Verbal fluency (n = 829) (score)14.05 ± 0.5513.19 ± 0.3212.52 ± 0.19p = 0.014
Delay recall (n = 825) (score)6.07 ± 0.195.84 ± 0.154.86 ± 0.10p < 0.001
Note: Q1, Low tertile of relative handgrip strength (rHGS); Q2, Moderate tertile of relative handgrip strength (rHGS); Q3, High tertile of relative handgrip strength (rHGS).
Table 3. Regression models of the association between handgrip strength or covariates and the indices of cognitive performance in adults with hypertension.
Table 3. Regression models of the association between handgrip strength or covariates and the indices of cognitive performance in adults with hypertension.
Variables Q1Q2
B (95% Confidence Interval)
Q3
B (95% Confidence Interval)
R2
Digit span forward (n = 3534)TotalReference0.18 (0.07, 0.30) ***0.20 (0.09, 0.32) ***0.11
MaleReference0.27 (0.14, 0.40) ***0.31 (0.18, 0.44) ***0.12
FemaleReference0.11 (−0.14,0.37)−0.12 (−0.51, 0.28)0.12
Digit span backward (n = 3530)TotalReference0.01 (−0.09, 0.12)−0.04 (−0.07, 0.15)0.16
MaleReference−0.05 (−0.17, 0.08)−0.05 (−0.18, 0.07)0.15
FemaleReference−0.12 (−0.35, 0.12)−0.07 (−0.43, 0.30)0.25
Verbal fluency (n = 3534)TotalReference0.39 (−0.11, 0.79)0.63 (0.22, 1.05) **0.09
MaleReference0.46 (−0.01, 0.93)0.64 (0.16, 1.11) **0.09
FemaleReference0.14 (−0.94, 0.67)0.55 (−0.71, 1.84)0.13
Delay recall (n = 3532)TotalReference0.19 (0.01, 0.37) *0.15 (−0.03, 0.33)0.13
MaleReference0.22 (0.01, 0.42) *0.22 (0.01,0.42) *0.13
FemaleReference0.27 (−0.14, 0.70)0.31 (−0.32,0.95)0.18
Note: Q1, Low tertile of relative handgrip strength (rHGS); Q2, Moderate tertile of relative handgrip strength (rHGS); Q3, High tertile of relative handgrip strength (rHGS). The adjusted model included age, sex, education years, setting, alcohol consumption, smoking, health condition, work physical activity, and leisure physical activity; *, p < 0.05, **, p < 0.01; ***, p < 0.001.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Lu, S.; Herold, F.; Zhang, Y.; Lei, Y.; Kramer, A.F.; Jiao, C.; Yu, Q.; Doig, S.; Li, J.; Yan, Z.; et al. Higher Handgrip Strength Is Linked to Better Cognitive Performance in Chinese Adults with Hypertension. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 985. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11080985

AMA Style

Lu S, Herold F, Zhang Y, Lei Y, Kramer AF, Jiao C, Yu Q, Doig S, Li J, Yan Z, et al. Higher Handgrip Strength Is Linked to Better Cognitive Performance in Chinese Adults with Hypertension. Brain Sciences. 2021; 11(8):985. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11080985

Chicago/Turabian Style

Lu, Shenghua, Fabian Herold, Yanjie Zhang, Yuruo Lei, Arthur F. Kramer, Can Jiao, Qian Yu, Scott Doig, Jinming Li, Zhe Yan, and et al. 2021. "Higher Handgrip Strength Is Linked to Better Cognitive Performance in Chinese Adults with Hypertension" Brain Sciences 11, no. 8: 985. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11080985

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop