Skip to main content

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Int J Public Health, 18 March 2022

Validation of the Personal Attributes Questionnaire-8: Gender Expression and Mental Distress in the German Population in 2006 and 2018

Ana Nanette TibubosAna Nanette TibubosDaniëlle Otten
Daniëlle Otten*Manfred E. BeutelManfred E. BeutelElmar BrhlerElmar Brähler
  • Department of Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center of the Johannes Gutenberg-University Mainz, Mainz, Germany

Objectives: Mental health differences between men and women can be attributed to sex or gender. Due to absence of brief assessments, contributions of gender expressions to the mental health gap between men and women have been understudied. The purpose of this study is to develop and validate a short screening measure of gender expression and test its associations with mental distress.

Methods: German representative survey data from 2006 (N = 2,507) and 2018 (N = 2,516) were analysed. A short form of the Personality Attributes Questionnaire with 8 items (PAQ-8) was assessed to measure femininity and masculinity. Validity of the PAQ-8 was tested and associations between femininity, masculinity and mental health were examined.

Results: PAQ-8 was a valid screening measure to assess gender expression. Compared to 2006, femininity increased in women and decreased in men in 2018. Higher levels of femininity and masculinity were associated with lower distress. Sex was no longer predictive for mental distress when femininity, masculinity, age and equivalised income were considered.

Conclusion: Our findings support the use of gender measures, which may be more predictive of mental health than sex.

Introduction

Mental disorders affect a large proportion of the population affecting quality of life and life expectancy [1]. Representative German studies have shown higher prevalence rates of mental disorders for women compared to men; one in three women and one in four or five men had a diagnosis of a mental disorder in the previous 12 months [2]. A variety of factors underlie this observation, e.g., genetic and biological vulnerabilities, environmental factors such as daily stressors, and differences in emotion regulation and stress responsiveness [3]. In general, mental health differences between women and men can be attributed to sex or gender differences. While sex refers to a biological construct [4], gender subsumes psychosocial variables that characterize women and men and their life contexts [5]. Sex and gender interact in the development of diseases (e.g. [6, 7]). Nonetheless, sex and gender have been used interchangeably or altogether neglected in studies on mental health [4, 8, 9].

Gender is nowadays considered as a multidimensional concept based on individual, interactional and macro areas of social life [10]. One aspect of gender are masculine and feminine traits. Based on the seminal work of Sandra Bem in 1974 [11], masculine traits such as being assertive or aggressive and feminine traits such as being affectionate and sympathetic, were assessed in a questionnaire. Initially described as a bipolar one-dimensional personality trait ranging from femininity to masculinity, gender roles were later measured as a two-dimensional concept with two separate and independent dimensions [1214]. The dimension femininity includes scales assessing expressivity or communion capturing similar content, the term masculinity is used interchangeable with instrumentality or agency [11]. In personality psychology research, communion and agency have been referred to as the Big Two [15, 16].

Mental disorders have become a key challenge for health care in the 21st century [17] with a 1-year prevalence of over 30%. Large cross-sectional population surveys have advanced our knowledge of how common mental disorders differentially affect and burden men and women [1719]: Compared to men, women suffer more frequently from depression, anxiety disorders, somatoform disorders, eating disorders, self-injury, suicide attempt, post-traumatic stress disorder and pill addiction, termed internalising disorders. In contrast, so-called externalising disorders are more common among men including disorders of impulse control, substance dependence, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, overweight, alcoholism, pathological gambling, suicides and violence against others [3, 20].

Developed by Spence et al. [14], the Personal Attributes Questionnaire (PAQ) has remained one of the most frequently used assessments of gender with its main dimensions of masculinity and femininity. Masculinity and femininity can be understood as traits that reflect independent dimensions of a personality [21]. Originally, the constructs measured with this questionnaire were defined as gender roles, which can be regarded as a representation of the internalized societal gender. In the light of the shift in terminology of the gender concept [22] and in line with the definition of the American Psychological Association, we understand the Personal Attributes Questionnaire as an indicator of gender expression [23]. Gender expression refers to an individual’s presentation and behavior in which a person communicates its gender within a given culture. Someone’s gender expression may or may not reflect a person’s gender identity.

In previous studies, both masculinity and femininity were positively related to indicators of adjustment (low distress, self-esteem), however, higher negative correlations were found between masculinity (vs. femininity) and distress (e.g. [13, 24]). The aim of this study was twofold. The first goal was to develop and validate via item, scale, principal component, and factor analyses a brief screener for gender expression based on the PAQ which can be used in large scale surveys. Second, in order to validate the screener taking societal influences into account, the association of the dimensions masculinity and femininity with mental distress over time was assessed. As gender is both an important part of the individual self-concept and a reflection of societal trends of gender roles, we reapplied the scale in two representative national samples at two different time points, in 2006 and 2018. The shift towards a more individualistic society [25], in which values such as autonomy, self-expression and an ethos of individual rights are emphasized, expressions of diversity are more common [26, 27]. We therefore expected stronger expressions of masculinity and femininity in 2018 compared to 2006.

Methods

Sample

Data of two German representative studies with the same recruitment procedure conducted in the years 2006 (sample 1) and 2018 (sample 2) were analyzed. Sample 1 was split in two halves: sample 1a for short scale development, sample 1b for factorial cross-validation of the short scale. The samples were representative in terms of age, sex and educational level for the general German population, collected by an independent agency (USUMA, Berlin) in a nation-wide survey. Following a random route procedure on 258 non-overlapping sample point regions, target households were randomly selected within these areas. Subsequently, one of the household members of this address was selected by chance (Kish-Selection-Grid [28]) and contacted. The target person participated in a face-to-face interview conducted by a trained interviewer and additionally independently filled out several questionnaires. Representativeness of each sample was furthermore secured by comparisons with census data from the German Federal Statistical Office. The inclusion criteria for the study were the fluency and understanding of the German language, and a minimum age of 14 years. Of the 4,036 addresses and subjects initially selected for sample 1, 597 persons (14.8%) were not present on the three occasions when the interviewer visited the address, 899 persons (22.3%) rejected participation, and 33 people (0.8%) could not participate due to severe health issues. This led to a response rate of 62.1% with a total sample of N = 2,507 (1,357; 54.1% females) persons with a mean age of 48.0 (SD = 18.1). For sample 2 a total of 5,418 households were approached, 22.4% of these households refused to identify the person of target, and 12.7% of the target persons refused to participate. In total N = 2,516 (1,372; 54.5% females) target persons participated in the study with a mean age of 48.0 (SD = 17.6). Some respondents did not answer all items of the main construct PAQ or PAQ-8. Since there were only few missing items, mean scores for femininity and masculinity could be calculated for all respondents.

All participants received a detailed data privacy statement by the study assistant, informing about the study procedures, data collection, and anonymization of all personal data. The present study posed a low risk to the participants, as procedures such as medical treatments, invasive diagnostics or procedures causing psychological, spiritual or social harm were not included in the present study. Therefore, all participants provided verbal informed consent, which was noted by the trained interviewer before starting with the survey. In the case of minors, participants gave informed assent with informed consent being provided by their caregivers. The completed questionnaires were linked to the socio-demographic data, but did not contain any data to identify the participant. The study and procedure, including the consent procedure, were approved by the institutional ethics review board of the University of Leipzig (Az 132/18-ek). Furthermore, the study adhered to the guidelines of the ICC/ESOMAR International Code of Marketing and Social Research Practice.

Measures

All following measures were available for both measurement points, except PAQ which was only assessed in 2006.

PAQ and PAQ-8. For the present study, the German version [13, 29, 30] of the PAQ (GEPAQ) with the scales masculinity/positive instrumentality and femininity/expressivity with eight items each was used in sample 1 as starting point for scale reduction. For masculinity/positive instrumentality the items of the PAQ (items of the PAQ-8 indicated in brackets) were: not at all independent—very independent, very passive—very active, not at all competitive—very competitive, can make decisions easily—has difficulty making decisions, never gives up easily—gives up very easily (PAQ-8), not at all self-confident—very self-confident (PAQ-8), feels superior—feels very inferior (PAQ-8), and goes to pieces under pressure–stands up well under pressure (PAQ-8). For femininity/expressivity the items were: not at all emotional—very emotional (PAQ-8), not at all able to devote self completely to others—able to devote self completely to others, very rough—very gentle, not at all helpful to others—very helpful to others, not at all kind—very kind, not at all aware of feelings of others—very aware of feelings of others (PAQ-8), not at all understanding of others—very understanding of others (PAQ-8), and very warm in relations with others—very cold in relations with others (PAQ-8). Each item had a six-step response answer scale ranging from 1 to 6. These extremes indicated the characteristic applying perfectly to the respondent. The closer to the middle of the scale, the more neutral the respondent answered between both opposing characteristics.

The PAQ showed a two-factor structure with moderately correlated subscales (r = 0.48). Reliability of the PAQ was good, with Cronbach’s α: masculinity-scale: 0.77; femininity-scale: 0.84. A brief version (PAQ-8) has been developed in this study based on the German PAQ version with 8 items in total, 4 items for masculinity and femininity each. Detailed psychometric properties and item formulation are reported in the results section (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Items and descriptive statistics of the Personal Attributes Questionnaire-8 (Germany, 2006 & 2018).

PHQ-4. The Patient Health Questionnaire-4 (PHQ-4) [31] is an ultra-brief and reliable (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.80) screener for depressive symptoms and anxiety [32]. Depression items assess depressed mood and loss of interest (PHQ-2). Anxiety includes the two screening items of the Generalized Anxiety Disorder-7 [33]: “Feeling nervous, anxious or on edge” and “not being able to stop or control worrying.” The frequency of occurrence in the past 2 weeks was rated from 0 = “not at all”, 1 = “several days”, 2 = “over half the days”, and 3 = “nearly every day”. For the analyses, a single factor solution was used as a measure of distress.

Socio-demographic variables. Sex (male = 1, female = 2), age (stratified in groups in descriptive statistics 14-20/21-30/31-40/41-50/51-60/61-70/71+ years or as continuous variable), education (in years), and equivalised income defined as household income/√(people in household (stratified in tertiles in descriptive statistics or as continuous variable) were taken into account in analyses.

Statistical Analyses

Development and Factorial Validation of PAQ-8

In order to shorten the PAQ-questionnaire, content-based item analyses to identify redundant and less comprehensible items was conducted. Four items are supposed to be eliminated for each subscale. Afterwards, item and scale analyses and principal component analysis (PCA) using varimax rotation with half of sample 1 (sample 1a) were conducted. For cross-validation, a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was performed with the other half of sample 1 (sample 1b). For CFA the robust maximum likelihood method with Yuan-Bentler’s scaled χ2 was used for model estimation [34]. Goodness of fit was evaluated by applying the following model-fit indices: the minimum discrepancy divided by its degrees of freedom (CMIN/DF), the comparative-fit-index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) with confidence intervals (CI). For a good model fit, the ratio CMIN/DF should be as small as possible. A good model fit is shown by a non-significant YB- χ2 value (p > 0.01) as well as RMSEA ≤0.05 and CFI ≥0.97, an acceptable model fit is available if χ2-value/df ≤ 3, RMSEA ≤0.08 and CFI ≥0.95 [35]. The latent factors were used as scaling variable.

Construct Validation of PAQ-8

First, we compared the correlation patterns of PAQ-8 (sample 1 and 2) and PAQ (sample 1). Second, in terms of convergent and discriminant validity, correlations between PAQ-8, PAQ and distress (PHQ-4) were compared for the overall sample and stratified by sex. Third, in order to capture potential societal influences on gender expression over time, we performed multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) with femininity and masculinity as dependent variables (predictors: sex, year, and their interaction), and multiple regression analyses with mental distress as dependent variable for 2006 and 2018 each (predictors: sex, femininity, masculinity, and their interaction; covariates: education, equivalised income, and age). Effect sizes were reported based on partial η2: small <0.01, medium <. 06, large <0.14 [36], p <0 .05 was considered significant.

The software programs SPSS version 24 and Mplus version 8 [37] were used.

Results

Development and Factorial Validation of PAQ-8

According to the objective, eight items were suggested to be excluded due to content redundancy and less comprehensible item formulations. An overview of descriptive statistics of the remaining items are displayed in Table 1. Principal component analysis pointed out two components (57.7% explained variance) based on scree-plot, Kaiser-Eigenvalues, and Minimum Average Partial test (MAP-analysis). The two theoretical dimensions femininity and masculinity were represented by the two components. Component loadings for femininity ranged from 0.71–0.72, for masculinity from 0.73 to 0.79. Also CFA results supported the two-factor model: χ2(19) = 90.49, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.97, RMSEA = 0.05 [CI 0.04 -0.06], SRMR = 0.03. The two scales (r = 0.44) with 4 items each had an acceptable internal consistency, Cronbach`s α: masculinity-scale: 0.79; femininity-scale: 0.71 and McDonald`s ω: masculinity-scale: 0.76; femininity-scale: 0.71. Figure 1 displays the results of the CFA with standardized parameters.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. The Personal Attributes Questionnaire-8 two-factor model with standardized coefficients (Germany, 2006).

Femininity and Masculinity in the German General Population Over Time

Descriptive statistics of femininity and masculinity levels for 2006 and 2018 are displayed in Supplementary Table S1. Femininity scores were higher among women, while masculinity scores were higher among men for both measurement points. Femininity scores between women and men showed larger differences in 2018 compared to 2006, with higher femininity scores for women and lower femininity scores for men. For both dimensions, comparable scores were observed between age groups. Whilst significant differences for masculinity with very small effect size were observed, no significant differences were observed for femininity in 2006. In 2018, similarly negligible effect sizes were observed for significant differences for femininity and masculinity between the age groups. In general, a non-linear trend with the highest scores for the age range of 21–40 years were revealed for both measurement points for the total sample as well as in sex-stratified analyses. Regarding education in 2006, significant differences for femininity and masculinity with small effect sizes were reported for the total sample, for women, and for men. Larger effect sizes were displayed by men. Individuals with least years of education displayed the lowest femininity and masculinity scores. In 2018, however, no significant differences were found between the educational groups for femininity. With respect to masculinity in 2018, significant differences were observed similar to 2006. Finally, analyses of gender identities for different equivalised income groups revealed slight differences between both measurement points. In 2006, gender identities of women and men did not differ between different income groups. Yet, in 2018, income groups were associated with gender expression of women. Especially masculinity scores increased along with higher income of women.

Figure 2 compares the scores of men and women in the two gender expression subscales in 2006 and 2018. A MANOVA with femininity and masculinity as dependent variables and year, sex, and the interaction as predictors was performed in order to test sex- and time associations with gender expression. The overall model did not turn out to be significant: R2 = 0.03, F (2,5010) = 0.187, p = 0.830. Femininity was consistently higher in women and masculinity higher in men in both surveys [FFemininity (1,5011) = 385.76, p ≤ 0.001, ηp2 = 0.07, FMasculinity (1,5011) = 140.60, p ≤ 0.001, ηp2 = 0.03]. As the interaction year X sex shows, in 2018 femininity was higher in women and lower in men compared to 2006 [FFemininity (1,5011) = 25.37, p ≤ 0.001, ηp2 = 0.01]. Thus, sex differences increased regarding femininity over time, but not regarding masculinity [FMasculinity (1,5011) = 0.30, p = 0.863, ηp2 = 0.00]. There was also no significant association regarding year [FFemininity (1,5011) = 0.27, p = 0.601, ηp2 = 0.00, Fm (1,5011) = 0.20, p = 0.651, ηp2 = 0.00].

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Mean scores and standard deviation of gender expressions for males, females and total sample (Germany, 2006 & 2018).

Mental Distress by Sex and Gender Expressions in 2006 and 2018

In terms of convergent and discriminant validity, Tables 2, 3 present bivariate Spearman correlations between PAQ (only in 2006), PAQ-8, and PHQ-4, sex, age, education, and equivalised income. The correlation between PAQ-8 and mental distress was lower compared to the correlation between PAQ-8 and its original version. There was a stronger correlation between femininity and masculinity in the German general population in 2006 (r = 0.34, p < 0.001) compared to 2018 (r = 0.19, p < 0.001). Femininity was negatively associated to mental distress in 2006 (r = −0.12, p < 0.001) and 2018 (r = −0.14, p < 0.001). Masculinity was also negatively associated to distress (2006 r = −0.29, p < 0.001), but to a larger degree, particularly in 2018 (r = −0.39, p < 0.001). While the correlation between sex and masculinity was quite comparable for 2006 and 2018 (r = −0.17/−0.16; both p < 0.001), the association between femininity and sex increased (r = 0.22/.31, both p < 0.001). These significant results even hold when applied a conservatively estimated adjusted p-value of α/n (0.01/72) = 0.0001 for multiple testing since the largest p-value of the reported correlation coefficients was p = 0.0000000058. Comparable levels were reported referring to age and gender expression for the total sample at both measurement points. The older people were, the lesser femininity and masculinity were reported. This trend was generally higher for women compared to men. With regards to variables of socio-economic status, education showed low positive correlations with both gender identities. However, in 2018, the correlation between femininity and education approximated zero. For equivalised income at both measurement points, higher positive associations with masculinity compared to femininity were revealed in the total sample. Generally, the associations were weaker in 2018 than in 2006, with exception for women in 2006 where both dimensions of gender expression showed similar correlation coefficients with income. Subsequent sex stratified analysis revealed a counteracting pattern change over time: While the association between masculinity and equivalised income decreased nearly halfway among men, an almost twofold increase was observed for women. Furthermore, no significant association between equivalised income and femininity was given anymore for men in 2018 which also applied for the overall sample analysis.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Spearman correlations between gender expression, mental distress, and sex. Total sample (Germany, 2006 & 2018)

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Spearman correlations between gender identity, mental distress, and sex. Stratified by sex: lower diagonal for women, upper diagonal for men (Germany, 2006 & 2018).

While in univariate analyses sex was predictive of distress, which was higher in women for both measurement points, in the multivariate model with gender expression and sex as predictors, sex was not predictive of distress any more (see Table 4 and Supplementary Table S2). Femininity and masculinity explained 11% (2006) and 20% (2018) of the observed variance. Moderate negative associations of masculinity with mental distress were revealed, with slightly higher levels in 2006 compared to 2018. Also, negative associations between femininity and mental distress were observed, but with similarly low effect sizes for both measurement points. For the year 2018, there was an additional significant interaction between femininity and sex, albeit with weak effect size. The interaction pointed out a stronger negative association between femininity and mental distress among women compared to men. In sum, more observed variance was explained by the predictor set in 2018 (21%) vs. 2006 (12%). While education turned out be an irrelevant predictor in the full model, age positively and equivalised income negatively predicted mental distress with low effect sizes in 2006 and even lower for 2018.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Multiple Regression analyses on mental distress by sex and gender expression adjusted for sociodemographic features, Model 4 (Germany, 2006 & 2018).

Discussion

Following the recommendations of public health agencies such as the US funding guidelines of the National Institutes of Health [38], the Canadian Institute of Gender and Health, or the Robert Koch Institute in Germany [39], we provided new impetus for sex- and gender-sensitive health research. We developed an economical version of the PAQ with a total of eight items, with good internal consistency as measure of reliability for group comparisons. Its brevity makes it highly suitable for epidemiological research. The validity of the PAQ-8 is supported by our findings: As in the long version, men and women were discriminated by their measures of masculinity and femininity, respectively.

Both dimensions of gender expression showed the highest levels in the early to middle adulthood. Regarding time related aspects, the comparison between the two cohorts indicated that femininity scores were in sum higher than masculinity scores in the total sample at all measurement points. No time effect could be observed between 2006 and 2018 at the mean scores level for the German general population. An interaction between time and sex, though with very small effect size, was shown for femininity. Accordingly, sex differences increased over time with women endorsing more and men less feminine traits. An explanation for this could be a strong shift in gender terminology over the last years [22]. Furthermore, due to the inability to select a non-binary or diverse gender in this study, individuals concerned might have withdrawn from the study. Another explanation could be the discouragement to explore and display feminine traits, which is especially present among men [40]. In a meta-analysis among US college students, Donnelly and Twenge [41] observed contrary effects, specifically, a temporal increase of women’s masculinity and decrease of femininity for over 30 years. However, direct comparisons are limited due to different study populations in terms of culture, age and time range.

Furthermore, findings pointed out an increase of the gender gap. For instance, the positive association between masculinity and femininity in the German population decreased nearly by half over time for the total sample and in sex-stratified analysis. In general, masculinity was more positively associated with higher equivalised income than femininity, with the exception for women in 2006. While among men the association between masculinity and equivalised income decreased almost by half over time, the association between equivalised income and masculinity increased almost twofold for women. No association between femininity and equivalised income in men and in the overall sample was found in 2018. Contrary findings for masculinity in women and men regarding income may point to changing gender roles in society. While the stereotype of masculinity and money [42] more likely applied to men in 2006, this seemed to have shifted to women in 2018. While for women displaying more masculine traits is rewarded by society (e.g., strong association with equalized income), feminine traits are in general discouraged due to a devaluation of femininity in society, since masculinity is the norm and dominant over femininity [43], also known as femmephobia [44].

As in previous studies, both dimensions of gender expressions were negatively associated with distress [13, 24], e.g., higher levels of femininity and masculinity were associated with lower mental distress. Especially masculinity reflects resilient features showing two to three times higher negative associations than femininity to distress in bivariate and multivariate analyses. However, the association between masculinity and mental distress could be explained through the concept of toxic masculinity, which distinguishes “toxic” traditional masculine traits such as aggression and self-entitlement from “healthy” masculinity [45, 46]. Externalising disorders which are more frequently exhibited by men are associated with stigmatization of mental disorders [47]. Stigma on mental health problems in turn prevents people from reporting mental health problems [48]. Gender expression turned out to be a good predictor of distress, even when age and income were included. The fact that sex was no longer a significant predictor of mental distress when masculinity and femininity were included in the model indicates that associations between sex and mental distress are mediated by gender expression. In 2018, we found a significant interaction, yet with very small effect size, between femininity and sex. The interaction showed a trend of a stronger negative association between femininity and mental distress among women compared to men.

Based on the work of Bem [11], it has been surmised [49] that the range of behaviors available to strongly sex-typed individuals is limited (e.g., denial of weakness and need for help in men, excessive care for others while relinquishing self- interest in women). Thus, growing differentiation of cultural stereotypes between men and women might be detrimental to mental health. Further research is needed on the impact of gender dimensions such as gender expression (vs. sex) on mental health over time. In sum, findings underline that sex and gender may not be equated. We can only speculate, why the gender gap has widened over the period of 12 years. However, sensitivity to change of cultural conceptions of gender may also indicate the validity of the measure. Following the findings of Twenge et al. [25] the emphasis of “generation me” on being special and unique may lead to more polarized cultural perceptions of being masculine or feminine, while society has become more accepting of expression of diverse sexual identities.

Limitations of the study include the lack of non-binary categories and measuring only one aspect of gender, namely gender expression. Masculine and feminine traits are seen as gender stereotypes that support people in defining their self-concept, however many additional characteristics besides traits are linked to gender [50]. Information on diverse gender (including all persons whose gender does not conform to their society’s norms or values when it comes to their gendered physicality, gendered identity, gender expression or combination of those factors [51, 52]) was not present in the data and could therefore not be included in this study. Furthermore, individuals can exhibit strong masculine and feminine traits at the same time, which is indicated as androgynous [11]. Within the Sexual Configurations Theory it is argued that gender expression contains three categories, namely masculinity, femininity and androgynous gender [53] and that gender expressions exist besides sex and gender. Someone’s gender expression therefore does not necessarily match someone’s gender [53]. Androgynous, referring to an outward appearance of indeterminate gender, is not a requirement for a non-binary gender identity and women exhibiting strong masculine traits or men exhibiting strong feminine traits could identify as non-binary, women or men. Due to the shift in terminology of the gender concept, the number of persons identifying as non-binary increased over the last years [53]. In this study including data from 2006 to 2018, non-binary categories going beyond the categories of being a man or a woman were not assessed in the surveys. Additionally, we did not examine androgynous gender separately, since it did not appear as a separate scale in our principal component analyses. Furthermore, although the data sampling method was a random procedure and the response rate for both years was over 60%, refusal of participation could have led to a selection bias and should be considered when interpreting these results. Also, the cross-sectional nature of the analyses prevents one to infer causality. Strengths of the study refer to the large and representative samples from the German general population. While the samples were drawn according to the same criteria and selection process, the two assessments included somewhat different cohorts and cannot be interpreted as longitudinal data.

Conclusion

Given differential burdens of mental disorders, sex-specific reporting of mental health research has become a scientific requirement. However, sex has been often confused with gender. Due to a lack of brief assessments, the contribution of gender to the mental health gap between men and women has been understudied. In this study, we developed and validated a short screening measure of gender expression using representative survey data sets of Germany. Our findings support the use of gender measures, which turned out to be more strongly predictive of mental health than sex.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the institutional ethics review board of the University of Leipzig (Az 132/18-ek). Verbal informed consent, which was noted by the trained interviewer, was provided. In case of minors, informed consent was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

All authors have made substantial contributions to this work and have been involved in drafting the manuscript or revising it critically. All authors read and approved the final manuscript. Conceived and designed the studies: AT, MB, and EB. Performed the studies: EB. Analyzed the data: AT and DO. Wrote the main manuscript text: AT, MB, and DO. Prepared tables and figures: AT and DO.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank all participants and staff involved in data retrieval and management.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.ssph-journal.org/articles/10.3389/ijph.2022.1604510/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

BSRI, bem sex role inventory; CFA, confirmatory factor analysis; CFI, comparative-fit Index; CI, confidence interval; CMIN, minimum discrepancy; DF, degree of freedom; ESOMAR, european society for opinion and market research; F/F+, femininity/expressivity; GAD-7, generalized anxiety disorder 7; ICC, international chamber of commerce; MANOVA, multivariate analysis of variance; MAP-analysis, maximum a posteriori probability analysis; M/M+, masculinity/positive instrumentality; PAQ, personal attributes questionnaire; PAQ-8, brief version of PAQ with 8 items; PCA, principal component analysis; PHQ-4/PHQ-2, patient health questionnaire with 4 or 2 items, respectively; RMSEA, root mean square error of approximation; SD, standard deviation; SRMR, standardized root mean square residual; TLI, tucker-lewis index; USUMA, unabhängiger service für umfragen, methoden und analysen.

References

1. Walker, ER, McGee, RE, and Druss, BG. Mortality in Mental Disorders and Global Disease Burden Implications. JAMA Psychiatry (2015) 72(4):334–41. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Jacobi, F, Höfler, M, Strehle, J, Mack, S, Gerschler, A, Scholl, L, et al. Psychische Störungen in der Allgemeinbevölkerung. Nervenarzt (2014) 85(1):77–87. doi:10.1007/s00115-013-3961-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Kuehner, C. Why Is Depression More Common Among Women Than Among Men? The Lancet Psychiatry (2017) 4(2):146–58. doi:10.1016/s2215-0366(16)30263-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Tannenbaum, C, Greaves, L, and Graham, ID. Why Sex and Gender Matter in Implementation Research. BMC Med Res Methodol (2016) 16(1):145. doi:10.1186/s12874-016-0247-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Eagly, AH, and Wood, W. Social Role Theory of Sex Differences. In: N Naples, RC Hoogland, M Wickramasinghe, and WCA Wong, editors. The Wiley Blackwell Encyclopedia of Gender and Sexuality Studies. London: Wiley Blackwell (2016). p. 1–3. doi:10.1002/9781118663219.wbegss183

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Regitz-Zagrosek, V, Oertelt-Prigione, S, Prescott, E, Franconi, F, Gerdts, E, Foryst-Ludwig, A, et al. Gender in Cardiovascular Diseases: Impact on Clinical Manifestations, Management, and Outcomes. Eur Heart J (2016) 37(1):24–34. doi:10.1093/eurheartj/ehv598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Beutel, ME, Brähler, E, and Tibubos, AN. Gender und psychische Gesundheit. In: I Moeslein-Teising, G Schäfer, and R Martin, editors. Geschlechter-Spannungen. Gießen: Psychosozial-Verlag (2019). p. 54–65. doi:10.30820/9783837976229-54

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Pelletier, R, Ditto, B, and Pilote, L. A Composite Measure of Gender and its Association With Risk Factors in Patients With Premature Acute Coronary Syndrome. Psychosom Med (2015) 77(5):517–26. doi:10.1097/PSY.0000000000000186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Starker, A, Rommel, A, and Saß, A-C. Bericht zur gesundheitlichen Lage der Männer in Deutschland - Fazit und Herausforderungen für eine gendersensible Gesundheitsberichterstattung. Bundesgesundheitsbl (2016) 59(8):979–85. doi:10.1007/s00103-016-2383-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Scarborough, WJ. Handbook of the Sociology of Gender. In: BJ Risman, CM Froyum, and WJ Scarborough, editors. Handbook of the Sociology of Gender. Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research. Cham: Springer (2018). 3–18. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-76333-0_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Bem, SL. The Measurement of Psychological Androgyny. J Consulting Clin Psychol (1974) 42(2):155–62. doi:10.1037/h0036215

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Bem, SL. On the Utility of Alternative Procedures for Assessing Psychological Androgyny. J Consulting Clin Psychol (1977) 45(2):196–205. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.45.2.196

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Goldschmidt, S, Linde, K, Alfermann, D, and Brähler, E. Das Geschlechtsrollenselbstkonzept von Erwachsenen. Eine Überprüfung der deutschsprachigen Version des Personal Attributes Questionnaire (PAQ). [Gender role self-concept of adults. Testing the German version of the Personal Attributes Questionnaire (PAQ)]. Psychosozial (2014) 37:89–108.

Google Scholar

14. Spence, JT, Helmrich, R, and Stapp, J. The Personal Attributes Questionnaire: A Measure of Sex Role Stereotypes and Masculinity-Femininity. Washington: Journal Supplement Abstract Service (1978).

Google Scholar

15. Gebauer, JE, Sedikides, C, Lüdtke, O, and Neberich, W. Agency-communion and Interest in Prosocial Behavior: Social Motives for Assimilation and Contrast Explain Sociocultural Inconsistencies. J Pers (2014) 82(5):452–66. doi:10.1111/jopy.12076

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Wiggins, JS. Agency and Communion as Conceptual Coordinates for the Understanding and Measurement of Interpersonal Behaviour. In: D Cicchetti, and WM Grove, editors. Thinking Clearly about Psychology: Essays in Honor of Paul E Meehl. University of Minnesota Press (1991). p. 89–113.

Google Scholar

17. Wittchen, HU, Jacobi, F, Rehm, J, Gustavsson, A, Svensson, M, Jönsson, B, et al. The Size and burden of Mental Disorders and Other Disorders of the Brain in Europe 2010. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol (2011) 21(9):655–79. doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2011.07.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Piccinelli, M, and Wilkinson, G. Gender Differences in Depression. Br J Psychiatry (2000) 177(6):486–92. doi:10.1192/bjp.177.6.486

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Kolip, P, and Hurrelmann, K. Handbuch Geschlecht und Gesundheit: Männer und Frauen im Vergleich [Handbook of Gender and Health: Men and Women Compared]. Bern: Hogrefe Verlag (2016).

Google Scholar

20. Merbach, M, and Brähler, E. Prävention und Gesundheitsförderung bei Männern und Frauen". ["Prevention and health promotion for men and women"]Referenzwerk Prävention und Gesundheitsförderung. In: K Hurrelmann, M Richter, T Klotz, and S Stock, editors (2018). p. 419–28.Göttingen: Hogrefe

Google Scholar

21. Ward, CA. Models and Measurements of Psychological Androgyny: A Cross-Cultural Extension of Theory and Research. Sex Roles (2000) 43(7-8):529–52. doi:10.1023/A:1007171500798

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Thorne, N, Yip, AK-T, Bouman, WP, Marshall, E, and Arcelus, J. The Terminology of Identities between, outside and beyond the Gender Binary - A Systematic Review. Int J Transgenderism (2019) 20(2-3):138–54. doi:10.1080/15532739.2019.1640654

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23.American Psychological Association. Guidelines for Psychological Practice with Transgender and Gender Nonconforming People. Am Psychol (2015) 70(9):832–64. doi:10.1037/a0039906

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Payne, FD. ?Masculinity,? ?femininity,? and the Complex Construct of Adjustment. Sex roles (1987) 17:359–74. doi:10.1007/BF00288141

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Twenge, JM, Campbell, WK, and Gentile, B. Increases in Individualistic Words and Phrases in American Books, 1960-2008. PLoS One (2012) 7(7):e40181. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0040181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Davis, LS, and Williamson, CR. Does Individualism Promote Gender equality? World Development (2019) 123:104627. doi:10.1016/j.worlddev.2019.104627

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Pöge, K, Dennert, G, Koppe, U, Güldenring, A, Matthigack, EB, and Rommel, A. The Health of Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, Transgender and Intersex People. J Health Monit (2020) 5(S1):1–27. doi:10.25646/6449

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Kish, L. A Procedure for Objective Respondent Selection within the Household. J Am Stat Assoc (1949) 44(247):380–7. doi:10.1080/01621459.1949.10483314

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Alfermann, D. Androgynie" ["Androgyny"]. In: D Reigber, editor. Frauen-Welten. Düsseldorf: Econ-Verlag (1993). p. 147–200.

Google Scholar

30. Alfermann, D, Reigber, D, and Turan, J. Androgynie, soziale Einstellungen und psychische Gesundheit: Zwei Untersuchungen an Frauen im Zeitvergleich. In: U Bock, and D Alfermann, editors. Querelles Jahrbuch für Frauenforschung. Stuttgart: Metzler (1999). p. 142–55. doi:10.1007/978-3-476-03774-9_9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Kroenke, K, Spitzer, RL, Williams, JBW, and Löwe, B. An Ultra-brief Screening Scale for Anxiety and Depression: The PHQ-4. Psychosomatics (2009) 50(6):613–21. doi:10.1176/appi.psy.50.6.613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Tibubos, AN, and Kröger, H. A Cross-Cultural Comparison of the Ultrabrief Mental Health Screeners PHQ-4 and SF-12 in Germany. Psychol Assess (2020) 32(7):690–7. doi:10.1037/pas0000814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Kroenke, K, Spitzer, RL, Williams, JBW, Monahan, PO, and Löwe, B. Anxiety Disorders in Primary Care: Prevalence, Impairment, Comorbidity, and Detection. Ann Intern Med (2007) 146(5):317–25. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-146-5-200703060-00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Yuan, K-H, and Bentler, PM. 5. Three Likelihood-Based Methods for Mean and Covariance Structure Analysis with Nonnormal Missing Data. Sociological Methodol (2000) 30(1):165–200. doi:10.1111/0081-1750.00078

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Schermelleh-Engel, K, Moosbrugger, H, and Müller, H. Evaluating the Fit of Structural Equation Models: Tests of Significance and Descriptive Goodness-Of-Fit Measures. Methods Psychol Res Online (2003) 8(2):23–74.

Google Scholar

36. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New York, NY: Routledge Academic (1988). 567 p.

Google Scholar

37. Muthén, LK, and Muthén, BO. Mplus User's Guide. 8 ed. Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén (1998).

Google Scholar

38.National Institutes of Health. Consideration of Sex as a Biological Variable in NIH-Funded Research Not-OD-15-102 (2015). Available from: https://orwh.od.nih.gov/resources/pdf/NOT-OD-15-102_Guidance.pdf] (Accessed February 13, 2021).

Google Scholar

39. Jahn, I, and Foraita, R. Geschlechtergerechte epidemiologische Datenanalyse: Methodische Aspekte und empirische Befunde. Bundesgesundheitsbl. (2008) 51(1):13–27. doi:10.1007/s00103-008-0415-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Kierski, W, and Blazina, C. The Male Fear of the Feminine and its Effects on Counseling and Psychotherapy. J Men's Stud (2009) 17(2):155–72. doi:10.3149/jms.1702.155

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Donnelly, K, and Twenge, JM. Masculine and Feminine Traits on the Bem Sex-Role Inventory, 1993–2012: a Cross-Temporal Meta-Analysis. Sex Roles (2017) 76(9):556–565. doi:10.1007/s11199-016-0625-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Williams, MJ, Elizabeth, LP, and Spencer-Rodgers, J. The Masculinity of Money: Automatic Stereotypes Predict Gender Differences in Estimated Salaries. Psychol Women Q (2010) 34(1):7–20. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2009.01537.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Schippers, M. Recovering the Feminine Other: Masculinity, Femininity, and Gender Hegemony. Theor Soc (2007) 36:85–102. doi:10.1007/s11186-007-9022-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Ashley Hoskin, R. Femme Interventions and the Proper Feminist Subject: Critical Approaches to Decolonizing Western Feminist Pedagogies. Cogent Soc Sci (2017) 3:1276819. doi:10.1080/23311886.2016.1276819

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Harrington, C. What Is “Toxic Masculinity” and Why Does it Matter? Men and Masculinities (2020) 24(2):345–52. doi:10.1177/1097184X20943254

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. de Boise, S. Editorial: Is Masculinity Toxic? Norma (2019) 14(3):147–51. doi:10.1080/18902138.2019.1654742

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Boysen, GA. Exploring the Relation between Masculinity and Mental Illness Stigma Using the Stereotype Content Model and BIAS Map. J Soc Psychol (2017) 157(1):98–113. doi:10.1080/00224545.2016.1181600

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Stolzenburg, S, Freitag, S, Evans-Lacko, S, Muehlan, H, Schmidt, S, and Schomerus, G. The Stigma of Mental Illness as a Barrier to Self Labeling as Having a Mental Illness. J Nerv Ment Dis (2017) 205:903–9. doi:10.1097/NMD.0000000000000756

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Helgeson, VS, and Fritz, HL. The Implications of Unmitigated Agency and Unmitigated Communion for Domains of Problem Behavior. J Personal (2000) 68(6):1031–57. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.00125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Athenstaedt, U. On the Content and Structure of the Gender Role Self-Concept: Including Gender-Stereotypical Behaviors in Addition to Traits. Psychol Women Q (2003) 27(4):309–18. doi:10.1111/1471-6402.00111

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Winter, S. Are Human Rights Capable of Liberation? the Case of Sex and Gender Diversity. Aust J Hum Rights (2009) 15(1):151–73. doi:10.1080/1323238X.2009.11910865

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Hines, S, and Taylor, M. Is Gender Fluid? A Primer for the 21st century (The Big Idea). London: Thames and Hudson Ltd (2018).

Google Scholar

53. Sari, M, and va, A. Beyond Sexual Orientation: Integrating Gender/Sex and Diverse Sexualities via Sexual Configurations Theory. Arch Sex Behav (2015) 44(5):1177–213. doi:10.1007/s10508-015-0490-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sex, gender, gender expression, femininity, masculinity, mental health, epidemiology

Citation: Tibubos AN, Otten D, Beutel ME and Brähler E (2022) Validation of the Personal Attributes Questionnaire-8: Gender Expression and Mental Distress in the German Population in 2006 and 2018. Int J Public Health 67:1604510. doi: 10.3389/ijph.2022.1604510

Received: 05 October 2021; Accepted: 16 February 2022;
Published: 18 March 2022.

Edited by:

Olaf von dem Knesebeck, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Germany

Reviewed by:

Julia Ludwig, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Germany

Copyright © 2022 Tibubos, Otten, Beutel and Brähler. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Daniëlle Otten, Danielle.Otten@unimedizin-mainz.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.