Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 13 October 2021
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

Meta-Accuracy of Very First Impressions: A Mini Review

  • Department of Psychology, Bulgarian Academy of Sciences, Institute for Population and Human Studies, Sofia, Bulgaria

The meta-accuracy of first impressions (i.e., how accurately one understands others’ perception of oneself) can be conceptualized and measured in various ways. In order to reduce conceptual and methodological overwhelm, facilitate understanding of the topic, and stimulate future work in the field, we conducted a brief introductory literature review on the meta-accuracy of first impressions. Following a definitions-and-methodology-focused overview of the historical development of the topic, we present comparative synthesis and analysis of the key conceptualization and measurement methods used to study the meta-accuracy of first impressions. We also summarize the central research themes and types of stimuli that have been studied in relation to the meta-accuracy of first impressions. Finally, we make several suggestions for further research that could be beneficial to the future development and expansion of the field.

Introduction

Jane was shaking as she left her new mentor’s office: “She suggested so many changes… She must have thought I am totally incompetent… How am I ever going to get my degree?!”

Professor Jones, still at her desk, smiled as she went over her first meeting with Jane: “What a clever girl! Quite anxious, true, but so resourceful – such an interesting research topic… I hope my feedback helps her get the finer details right.”

First Impressions

The opening anecdote describes a situation in which a first impression occurs—that of Professor Jones about her new graduate student Jane. First impressions are the inferences we make about someone upon an initial encounter with them. These impressions are formed quickly and spontaneously (Willis and Todorov, 2006) and are long-lasting (e.g., Gunaydin et al., 2017). First impressions are also remarkably influential as they are known to affect and predict situations of high personal (e.g., employment interviews; Harris and Garris, 2008) and societal relevance (e.g., political elections; Olivola and Todorov, 2010). Given the power of first impressions, understanding their various characteristics becomes essential for ensuring successful communication and avoiding misunderstanding.

Accuracy

One important characteristic of first impressions is their accuracy. Is the first impression of Professor Jones about Jane accurate? Accuracy in interpersonal perception typically refers to the correspondence between the subjective perception of the interaction partners and some more objective (i.e., more stable with respect to time and influences) criterion (e.g., Funder and West, 1993; Brauer and Proyer, 2020). First impressions can be (e.g., Ambady et al., 1999) but are not necessarily accurate (e.g., Rule et al., 2013; see Wood, 2014 for a detailed review on first impressions accuracy). Divergent findings on interpersonal accuracy may to a large extent be explained by differences in conceptualization and measurement approaches. Funder (1995) discusses common problems with defining and measuring interpersonal accuracy and proposes the Realistic Accuracy Model whose aim is to resolve many of these problems. The model also discusses moderators of interpersonal accuracy, such as the qualities of the perceiver, target, trait of interest, and the information involved.

Meta-Accuracy

In social interactions, there is a significant amount of trying to figure out what others think. The accuracy with which one infers others’ perception of oneself is termed as meta-accuracy (Kenny and DePaulo, 1993). In our opening scenario, meta-accuracy relates to the correspondence between what Professor Jones thinks of Jane and what Jane thinks Professor Jones thinks of her.

It is important to note, however, that the matter of meta-accuracy is far more complex than the mere agreement between target and perceiver and instead is the result of the interplay between various factors and moderators (Funder, 1995; Carlson and Elsaadawy, 2021). Things are further complicated by the target’s and judge’s views having both shared and distinctive components, by targets and judges achieving different levels of accuracy for different information types (e.g., self–other knowledge asymmetry or SOKA; Vazire, 2010), and by “blindness” to how one is perceived under certain circumstances (e.g., Gallrein et al., 2013, 2016).

Understanding the meta-accuracy of first impressions is just as important as understanding their accuracy because it, too, has the potential to shape subsequent interactions. What we believe others make of us may to a large extent determine our own behavioral tendencies. Jane, for example, believes that Professor Jones sees her as incompetent and might try to work harder to prove that she is not and/or engage in self-fulfilling-prophecy behaviors and indeed present herself as incompetent.

Aim

The meta-accuracy of first impressions can be conceptualized and measured in multiple ways. The diversity in definitions and measurement techniques is advantageous as it allows addressing the phenomenon from various angles. However, it may also become overwhelming, confusing, and cause uncertainty and disagreement. We conducted a brief review to facilitate comprehension of current knowledge and planning of future work on the meta-accuracy of first impressions. We outline central conceptualization and measurement traditions, summarize key topics and stimuli studied in relation to the meta-accuracy of first impressions, and we present several further research ideas.

Materials and Methods

Literature Search

In September 2020, we conducted a Google Scholar search for articles in English whose text contained the expressions “meta accuracy” and “first impressions,” combined with the Boolean operator AND. The search returned approximately 144 results which we manually reviewed for relevance. We identified additional articles from the text and reference sections of the relevant Google Search results and from the automatic suggestions on some of their journal pages. Through the literature search and the manual filtering, we ended up with 70 articles.

Inclusion Criteria

The 70 articles underwent further detailed manual scrutiny with respect to several inclusion criteria. First, articles needed to be based on empirical data and be published peer-reviewed journals. Although, as stated earlier, first impressions generally tend to be exceptionally persistent, they can still be modified following subsequent exposure to new information under the right circumstances (e.g., Gawronski et al., 2010). As we were interested in the meta-accuracy of not yet modified “very first impressions” (term borrowed from Bar et al., 2006, p. 269), we restricted our review to articles that reported data on at least one previously unacquainted group or time point with no previous acquaintance. Finally, since the developmental and clinical perspective were not central to our, we only included articles that studied at least one non-clinical group and excluded articles that studied only adolescent samples. During the peer review process, one reviewer brought to our attention two recent articles on the “liking gap”—an underestimation of interaction partners’ liking for each other. Those papers study the meta-accuracy of first impressions but had evaded our search due to the use of slightly different keywords. Both articles met our selection criteria and were included in the review. Our final selection consisted of 20 articles.

Synthesis and Analysis

From these 20 articles, we extracted the central ways of conceptualization and measurement of the meta-accuracy of first impressions, as well as the main research topics and stimulus information types studied in relation to meta-accuracy (Table 1). Based on this synthesis, we identified several prominent research themes and stimuli types already figuring in the study of the meta-accuracy of first impressions. Our synthesis also helped spot a couple of research themes intuitively linked to but not yet studied in this context.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. First impressions meta-accuracy measures, central addressed topics, and information type used to form the impressions.

Conceptualization and Measurement of the Meta-Accuracy of First Impressions

Before summarizing our observations, we briefly review the history and logic of meta-accuracy conceptualization and measurement.

The literature points to two major ways of conceptualizing meta-accuracy, both rooted in interpersonal perception research. One way is to look at meta-accuracy as a stand-alone phenomenon either devoid of or disregarding any potential effects of the perceiver, target, and the measures. Biesanz (2010) refers to this approach as “a single global measure of accuracy” (pp. 854–855). This tradition typically estimates meta-accuracy as either the correlation (and/or regression) or difference between the target’s evaluation of how they believe the perceiver sees them and the perceiver’s actual evaluation of the target. Comparison of means is also used within this tradition. Going back to our opening example, a single global measure approach would simply match (by correlations, regressions, or mean comparisons). Jane’s belief of how her professor sees her with the professor’s actual perception. Any effects on meta-accuracy caused by Jane and the professor, as well as their attributes (e.g., personality and momentary states), would not be taken into account.

The other way of conceptualizing meta-accuracy is to consider the influence of the perceiver, target, and measures. Such conceptualization produces “componential models” which include the effects of the different components in interpersonal perception (Biesanz, 2010). The first model of this type was Lee J. Cronbach’s components of accuracy model (CCAM), published in Cronbach (1955) and focusing on the interaction between target and measure for each perceiver. Kenny and La Voie (1984) published the Social Relations Model (SRM), also based on componential logic but centering around the interaction (and considering effects of the specific relationship) between perceiver and target for each measure. In 2010, Jeremy C. Biesanz combined the CCAM and SRM into the Social Accuracy Model (SAM), which allows looking at perceiver and target effects across measures and traits. Readers will find an excellent review of componential models in Biesanz (2010). It suffices for our purposes to state that the logic of componential modeling is based on estimating the variance explained by each component (and, when applicable, by the relationship between components). In our opening example, a componential model could tell us how much of the observed effect is due to Jane’s perception, the professor’s perception, their unique characteristics, and their very specific situation (i.e., different from how other people see Jane and from how the professor sees other students). Furthermore, componential approaches, as well as studying meta-accuracy for multiple trait profiles instead of for single traits (e.g., Furr, 2008), allow the decomposition of meta-accuracy into normative (stereotypical, related to group perception) and distinctive (unique, related to the individual). Thus, although they entail some conceptual and computational differences, componential and profile approaches could tell us what aspects of Jane’s meta-accuracy are stereotypical (related to how Jane believes she is seen by a larger group of people and how they actually see her) and what are distinctive (related to how Jane believes she is seen specifically by Professor Jones and how the professor sees Jane).

The choice of measurement method for meta-accuracy is determined by its conceptualization, which in turn is influenced by researchers’ preferences and tradition, the research question, and/or the availability of resources. Componential approaches generally provide a broader view on meta-accuracy but at the same time require complicated designs and computations. Often, to meet their specific research needs and circumstances, researchers opt for an adaptation and/or combination of approaches.

Out of the 20 articles we reviewed, we classified 10 as using primarily a single global measure, seven as using a componential approach, and three as relying on combinations of both. The choice of conceptualization and measure are typically reasonably justified by theory and/or previous research and follow logically from the studied questions. There is clearly a preference for the componential approach and even specific models within the approach among collaborators with established traditions, while researchers new to the field tend to begin with single global measures and later may add componential measures or switch preferences entirely.

Addressed Themes

The reviewed articles cover a broad range of topics, which could be organized into several major themes. First, following the historical development of the field, a large portion of the research has been dedicated to uncovering proof of the existence of the meta-accuracy phenomenon and identifying its principal characteristics (DePaulo et al., 1987; Carlson et al., 2010, 2011a) and biases (Re et al., 2016; Lu et al., 2018). Second, meta-accuracy has been studied in the context of specific personality traits and ways of social functioning (Malloy and Janowski, 1992; Reno and Kenny, 1992; Carlson et al., 2011b; Carlson and DesJardins, 2015; Carlson, 2016a,b, 2017; Rom and Conway, 2018; Sasson et al., 2018; Pinkham et al., 2019; Tissera et al., 2020). The third and most recent theme in the study of the meta-accuracy of first impressions, that is still in its early days, is dedicated to studying the phenomenon in the framework of Internet communication (Stopfer et al., 2014; Wu and Zheng, 2019).

Types of Stimulus Information

The majority of reviewed articles studied first impressions formed in the context of face-to-face interactions. A couple of more recent studies also used videos, photographs, or texts. These observations led us to conclude that although traditionally the meta-accuracy of first impressions has been investigated in direct face-to-face interactions, there is a tendency to reflect societal trends by also addressing novel predominant communication means.

Discussion

Summary

We reviewed 20 articles investigating the meta-accuracy of first impressions formed without previous acquaintance between/among the interaction partners. We established that based meta-accuracy conceptualization, research could be organized into three categories—(1) work considering the effects of the perceiver, target, and measures (componential approach), (2) work looking at meta-accuracy in isolation from perceiver, target, and measures effects (single global measure approach), and (3) work combining the two approaches. The way viewing meta-accuracy is determined by the specific research question, circumstances, and tradition.

We also extracted topics studied in relation to first impression meta-accuracy and organized them into three central themes—(1) evidence of, characteristics of, and biases in the meta-accuracy of first impressions, (2) personality traits and specifics of social functioning associated with meta-accuracy, and (3) meta-accuracy of first impressions on the Internet. There is not sufficient number of studies addressing each topic to allow the conclusion that there is a preferred method of conceptualization and measurement associated with a particular topic. Our observations do show that narcissism and social anxiety have been studied together with meta-accuracy using componential approaches, but this could also be explained by the tradition and composition of the research teams.

Finally, we observed that although traditionally research of the meta-accuracy of first impressions has relied on face-to-face interactions, new stimuli reflecting the predominant modes of interpersonal communication are also being incorporated.

Ideas for Further Research

Within the reviewed articles, we found systematic, but a bit limited in scope, investigation of first impression meta-accuracy. This is fully understandable as the field is relatively small and the literature reflects the methodical work around the primary questions of interest for only a couple of research groups. Researchers new to the field do introduce some diversity in the studied topics (e.g., morality; Rom and Conway, 2018).

One intuitive direction for expanding the scope of the field would be to address a broader range of individual characteristics that have the potential to affect first impression meta-accuracy. The most obvious candidates would be perceiver personality differences, such as gender, agreeableness, neuroticism, which, as Hall et al. (2016) suggest, could be linked to the accuracy of personality detection. Further logical candidates for expanding individual characteristics range would be social intelligence and Theory of Mind. Given reports of impaired meta-accuracy in autism (Sasson et al., 2018), it is reasonable to suspect positive associations between social competencies and meta-accuracy. If perspective taking abilities are impaired, one might not be able to properly grasp how another might see them upon their very first encounter. To illustrate with our opening anecdote, Jane may indeed be simply anxious, but she may also have suboptimal or impaired social abilities, causing her to misinterpret social cues and not properly understand what Professor Jones thinks of her. As noted by Kenny and Albright (1987), the role of perceiver and target personality has long been of interest to interpersonal perception accuracy research but has not been properly addressed due to research method limitations. In a way, novel models, such as the SAM, do provide the means to study such questions, and the understanding of the meta-accuracy of first impressions would benefit from a systematic investigation on the matter.

Surprisingly, we did not come across any literature directly reporting the study of meta-accuracy of first impressions from a neurological perspective. What kind of meta-accuracy-related neural activation could we expect in Jane during and following her meeting with Professor Jones? We would predict the involvement of networks associated with thinking about what others think (e.g., Theory of Mind; Saxe and Kanwisher, 2003) and representation of the self (e.g., Molnar-Szakacs and Arzy, 2009). But what would the temporal dynamics of this activation be? Would there be a (recursive) feedback-based correction mechanism associated with meta-accuracy and what would that involve? Answering these and further neural activation questions would enhance our understanding of meta-accuracy of first impressions and might point toward previously unaddressed issues related to both its conceptualization and measurement. Recent work by Schindler et al. (2021) provides a peak into this direction by demonstrating through the study of event-related potentials following dyadic interactions that feedback incongruent with one’s self-view influences different processing stages of others’ evaluations of oneself.

Finally, we have seen through both the recently addressed themes and types of stimuli used in impression formation that meta-accuracy research has already began incorporating the specifics of Internet communication. Given the ever-increasing presence of the Internet in daily life, we would like to encourage the continuation and expansion of the Internet-related branch of research on meta-accuracy of first impressions. In particular, we believe that some highly specific characteristics of Internet communication, such as synchronicity, deserve attention as they may affect meta-accuracy of first impressions. Would meta-accuracy be affected (and if so, how) by a time lag between the exchanges in the communication process? Some Internet-specific types of information used in the formation of first impressions, such as avatars, for instance, also present research possibilities for meta-accuracy research.

In short, we propose that the understanding of meta-accuracy of first impressions could be enhanced by (1) expanding the range of individual characteristics that are studied in association with it, (2) addressing its neural correlates, and (3) strengthening its investigation in the context of Internet interactions.

Limitations

Our work followed a strict protocol but also entails a strong qualitative component. This more descriptive than statistical format is explained by the overall small number of peer-reviewed studies addressing the meta-accuracy of first impressions formed without previous acquaintance. With very few scientists studying the topic directly, in many instances, the conceptualization and measurement approaches as well as the studied theme are determined by tradition and the composition of the respective research team. As the field grows and the literature in it expands, it would become feasible to conduct more systematic reviews and possibly meta-analyses.

Conclusion

With our overview of the conceptualization and measurement approaches, our summary of the central studied themes, and our overview of potential future research directions we hope to make the domain of meta-accuracy of first impressions easier to navigate and appealing to students and interested researchers. With this review, we wish to introduce the topic in a not-too-daunting way, as well as inspire and facilitate further work in the field.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to the conception of the review and the establishment of the article inclusion criteria. ETs conducted the literature search, article classification, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. ETa provided feedback and contributed to revisions of the draft. All authors approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work, including open access publication fees, was supported by a grant with contract number КП-06-ДБ-3 from the National Scientific Program “Petar Beron. Science and Innovation with Europe” (Petar Beron and NIE) of the Bulgarian National Science Fund at the Ministry of Education of the Republic of Bulgaria.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Ambady, N., Hallahan, M., and Conner, B. (1999). Accuracy of judgments of sexual orientation from thin slices of behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 77, 538–547. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.77.3.538

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar, M., Neta, M., and Linz, H. (2006). Very first impressions. Emotion 6, 269–278. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.2.269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biesanz, J. C. (2010). The social accuracy model of interpersonal perception: assessing individual differences in perceptive and expressive accuracy. Multivar. Behav. Res. 45, 853–885. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2010.519262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boothby, E. J., Cooney, G., Sandstrom, G. M., and Clark, M. S. (2018). The liking gap in conversations: do people like us more than we think? Psychol. Sci. 29, 1742–1756. doi: 10.1177/0956797618783714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brauer, K., and Proyer, R. T. (2020). Judging dispositions toward ridicule and being laughed at from short self-descriptions at zero-acquaintance: testing self-other agreement, consensus, and accuracy. J. Res. Pers. 89:104016. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2020.104016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlson, E. N. (2016a). Do psychologically adjusted individuals know what other people really think about them? The link between psychological adjustment and meta-accuracy. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 7, 717–725. doi: 10.1177/1948550616646424

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlson, E. N., and DesJardins, N. M. L. (2015). Do mean guys always finish first or just say that they do? Narcissists’ awareness of their social status and popularity over time. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 41, 901–917. doi: 10.1177/0146167215581712

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlson, E. N., and Elsaadawy, N. (2021). “Figuring out how others see us: the formation of accurate meta-perceptions,” in Oxford Handbook of Accurate Personality Judgment. eds. T. D. Letzring and J. S. Spain (NY, US: Oxford University Press), 173–192.

Google Scholar

Carlson, E. N., Furr, R. M., and Vazire, S. (2010). Do we know the first impressions we make? Evidence for idiographic meta-accuracy and calibration of first impressions. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 1, 94–98. doi: 10.1177/1948550609356028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlson, E. N. (2016b). Meta-accuracy and relationship quality: weighing the costs and benefits of knowing what people really think about you. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 111, 250–264. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlson, E. N., Vazire, S., and Furr, R. M. (2011a). Meta-insight: do people really know how others see them? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101, 831–846. doi: 10.1037/a0024297

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlson, E. N., Vazire, S., Oltmanns, T., and F., (2011b). You probably think this paper’s about you: narcissists’ perceptions of their personality and reputation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101, 185–201. doi: 10.1037/a0023781

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlson, E. N., Wright, A. G. C., and Imam, H. (2017). Blissfully blind or painfully aware? Exploring the beliefs people with interpersonal problems have about their reputation. J. Pers. 85, 757–768. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12284

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cronbach, L. J. (1955). Processes affecting scores on “understanding of others” and “assumed similarity.” Psychol. Bull. 52, 177–193. doi: 10.1037/h0044919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DePaulo, B. M., Kenny, D. A., Hoover, C. W., Webb, W., and Oliver, P. V. (1987). Accuracy of person perception: do people know what kinds of impressions they convey? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 52, 303–315. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.52.2.303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Funder, D. C. (1995). On the accuracy of personality judgment: a realistic approach. Psychol. Rev. 102, 652–670. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.4.652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Funder, D. C., and West, S. G. (1993). Consensus, self-other agreement, and accuracy in personality judgment: an introduction. J. Pers. 61, 457–476. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1993.tb00778.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furr, R. M. (2008). A framework for profile similarity: integrating similarity, normativeness, and distinctiveness. J. Pers. 76, 1267–1316. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00521.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallrein, A.-M. B., Carlson, E. N., Holstein, M., and Leising, D. (2013). You spy with your little eye: people are “blind” to some of the ways in which they are consensually seen by others. J. Res. Pers. 47, 464–471. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2013.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallrein, A.-M. B., Weßels, N. M., Carlson, E. N., and Leising, D. (2016). I still cannot see it—A replication of blind spots in self-perception. J. Res. Pers. 60, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2015.10.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gawronski, B., Rydell, R. J., Vervliet, B., and De Houwer, J. (2010). Generalization versus contextualization in automatic evaluation. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 139, 683–701. doi: 10.1037/a0020315

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunaydin, G., Selcuk, E., and Zayas, V. (2017). Impressions based on a portrait predict, 1-month later, impressions following a live interaction. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 8, 36–44. doi: 10.1177/1948550616662123

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, M. J., and Garris, C. P. (2008). “You never get a second chance to make a first impression: behavioral consequences of first impressions,” in First Impressions. eds. N. Ambady and J. J. Skowronski (Guilford Publications), 147–168.

Google Scholar

Hall, J. A., Goh, J. X., Mast, M. S., and Hagedorn, C. (2016). Individual differences in accurately judging personality from text. J. Pers. 84, 433–445. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12170

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenny, D. A., and Albright, L. (1987). Accuracy in interpersonal perception: A social relations analysis. Psychol. Bull. 102, 390–402. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.102.3.390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenny, D. A., and DePaulo, B. M. (1993). Do people know how others view them? An empirical and theoretical account. Psychol. Bull. 114, 145–161. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenny, D. A., and La Voie, L. (1984). The social relations model. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 18, 142–182. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60144-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, J., Duan, H., and Xie, X. (2018). Eagerness and optimistically biased metaperception: the more eager to learn others’ evaluations, the higher the estimation of others’ evaluations. Front. Psychol. 9:715. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00715

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malloy, T. E., and Janowski, C. L. (1992). Perceptions and metaperceptions of leadership: components, accuracy, and dispositional correlates. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 18, 700–708. doi: 10.1177/0146167292186006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mastroianni, A. M., Cooney, G., Boothby, E. J., and Reece, A. G. (2021). The liking gap in groups and teams. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 162, 109–122. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2020.10.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molnar-Szakacs, I., and Arzy, S. (2009). Searching for an integrated self-representation. Comm. Integr. Biol. 2, 365–367. doi: 10.4161/cib.2.4.8290

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olivola, C. Y., and Todorov, A. (2010). Elected in 100 milliseconds: appearance-based trait inferences and voting. J. Nonverbal Behav. 34, 83–110. doi: 10.1007/s10919-009-0082-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinkham, A. E., Shasteen, J. R., and Ackerman, R. A. (2019). Metaperception of personality in schizophrenia. J. Exp. Psychopathol. 10, 1–17. doi: 10.1177/2043808719840915

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Re, D. E., Wang, S. A., He, J. C., and Rule, N. O. (2016). Selfie indulgence: self-favoring biases in perceptions of selfies. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 7, 588–596. doi: 10.1177/1948550616644299

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reno, R. R., and Kenny, D. A. (1992). Effects of self-consciousness and social anxiety on self-disclosure among unacquainted individuals: an application of the social relations model. J. Pers. 60, 79–94. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00266.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rom, S. C., and Conway, P. (2018). The strategic moral self: self-presentation shapes moral dilemma judgments. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 74, 24–37. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.08.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rule, N. O., Krendl, A. C., Ivcevic, Z., and Ambady, N. (2013). Accuracy and consensus in judgments of trustworthiness from faces: behavioral and neural correlates. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 104, 409–426. doi: 10.1037/a0031050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasson, N. J., Morrison, K. E., Pinkham, A. E., Faso, D. J., and Chmielewski, M. (2018). Brief report: adults with autism are less accurate at predicting how their personality traits are evaluated by unfamiliar observers. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 48, 2243–2248. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3487-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saxe, R., and Kanwisher, N. (2003). People thinking about thinking people. the role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind.” NeuroImage 19, 1835–1842. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00230-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schindler, S., Höhner, A., Moeck, R., Bruchmann, M., and Straube, T. (2021). Let’s talk about each other: neural responses to dissenting personality evaluations based on real dyadic interactions. Psychol. Sci. 32:0956797621995197. doi: 10.1177/0956797621995197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stopfer, J. M., Egloff, B., Nestler, S., and Back, M. D. (2014). Personality expression and impression formation in online social networks: an integrative approach to understanding the processes of accuracy, impression management and meta-accuracy. Eur. J. Personal. 28, 73–94. doi: 10.1002/per.1935

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tissera, H., Gazzard Kerr, L., Carlson, E. N., and Human, L. J. (2020). Social anxiety and liking: towards understanding the role of metaperceptions in first impressions. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000363 [Epub ahead of print]

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vazire, S. (2010). Who knows what about a person? The self–other knowledge asymmetry (SOKA) model. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 98, 281–300. doi: 10.1037/a0017908

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willis, J., and Todorov, A. (2006). First impressions: making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychol. Sci. 17, 592–598. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01750.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, T. J. (2014). Exploring the role of first impressions in rater-based assessments. Adv. Health Sci. Educ. 19, 409–427. doi: 10.1007/s10459-013-9453-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, T., and Zheng, Y. (2019). Is impression management through status updates successful? Meta-accuracy and judgment accuracy of big five personality traits based on status updates from social network sites in China. Front. Psychol. 10:1192. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: first impressions, unacquainted persons, meta-accuracy, review (article), introduction, single global measure, componential approach

Citation: Tsankova E and Tair E (2021) Meta-Accuracy of Very First Impressions: A Mini Review. Front. Psychol. 12:736534. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.736534

Received: 05 July 2021; Accepted: 21 September 2021;
Published: 13 October 2021.

Edited by:

Hester Van Herk, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Kay Brauer, Martin Luther University of Halle-Wittenberg, Germany
Frank J. Bernieri, Oregon State University, United States

Copyright © 2021 Tsankova and Tair. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Elena Tsankova, etsankova@gmail.com

ORCID: Elena Tsankova orcid.org/0000-0002-5008-8894

Ergyul Tair orcid.org/0000-0003-2546-463X

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.