Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 04 February 2019
Sec. Organizational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Fostering Creative Organizations: Antecedents, Processes, and Consequences of Individual and Team Creativity View all 9 articles

Effects of Interpersonal Sensorimotor Synchronization on Dyadic Creativity: Gender Matters

  • 1Applied Technology for Neuro-Psychology Laboratory, Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Milan, Italy
  • 2Department of Psychology, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, Milan, Italy
  • 3Department of Mechanical Engineering, Politecnico di Milano, Milan, Italy
  • 4Department of Psychology and Milan Centre for Neuroscience, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milan, Italy
  • 5Center of Translational Neurophysiology, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia (IIT), Center for Translational Neurophysiology of Speech and Communication (CTNSC), Universitá di Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy
  • 6Sezione di Fisiologia Umana, Dipartimento di Scienze Biomediche e Chirurgico Specialistiche, Universitá di Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy

Although it is noted that interpersonal sensorimotor coordination can influence several high-level socio-cognitive processes, its impact on creative collaboration is nearly unexplored. Here, we investigated the effects of a form of sensorimotor coordination, that is, sensorimotor synchronization, on a subsequent creative collaboration task. 60 pairs (n total = 120 participants) formed by previously unacquainted individuals performed a tower-building task either jointly or alone, followed by a dyadic creativity task. Tower building time in the joint condition was recorded through a sensorized platform and creativity performance was evaluated by two independent raters based on the quantity and quality of generated ideas. We controlled for gender composition and for the disposition to cooperate and to adopt a creative, analytical style. Results showed that male-male couples were more creative after the joint-action condition, whereas female-female and mixed-gender couples were more creative after the solo condition. Regression analyses of tower building time on creativity performance revealed that building time was a significant predictor of creativity dimensions in male-male and in mixed-gender couples but did not predict creative performance in female-female couples. Overall, these findings suggest that the manipulation of sensorimotor coordination can influence performance in a subsequent creative collaboration task, with the nature, and magnitude of this effect depending on the gender composition of the dyads. These results have potential implications for the design of sensorimotor-based strategies to enhance dyadic creative performance in several contexts, especially for the organizational settings.

Introduction

Joint action can be defined as the ability to act together with others and it is key to several social action behaviors, such as group dancing, music ensemble performance, surgical operations, and team sports (Sebanz and Knoblich, 2009; Repp and Su, 2013). In these joint activities, sensorimotor coordination – here defined as a temporal synchronization of body movements between individuals involved in social interactions (Bernieri et al., 1988) – is a key component. Synchronization among people during social interactions is one of the primary forms of interpersonal coordination (Miles et al., 2017). In dynamical systems theory, behavioral synchronization is regarded as an emergent phenomenon that can be modeled as a system of two coupled oscillators (Schmidt et al., 1990). This perspective has been extended to highlight the critical role of mutual behavioral prediction and thus the continuous and flexible exchange of bodily signals, to enhance sensorimotor coordination (Pezzulo et al., 2018). That is, both bodies and minds are involved to achieve interpersonal synchronization (Sebanz et al., 2006; Repp and Su, 2013).

Crucially, research in the domain of social embodied cognition has theorized and shown that even low-level behavioral synchronization can positively modulate specific high-level social processes, such as cooperation (Semin and Cacioppo, 2008; Wiltermuth and Heath, 2009; Valdesolo et al., 2010) affiliation (Hove and Risen, 2009), altruism, and empathy (Valdesolo and DeSteno, 2011). According to this perspective, also creativity can be considered as an embodied high-order cognitive process, which is influenced also, and especially, by individuals’ body (Stanciu, 2015). Emerging theoretical models are increasingly examining the role of interpersonal coordination on creative collaboration and in particular on dyadic creativity. For example, the Networked Flow model developed by Gaggioli et al. (2013a,b,c, 2015, 2017), (Sawyer, 2003, 2007; Galimberti et al., 2015), highlights the importance of social presence (i.e., the feeling of being and acting “together”; Biocca and Harms, 2003) and mutual engagement (i.e., group flow; Sawyer, 2003, 2007) as factors facilitating interpersonal creativity. Similarly, Rouse (2018) theorized the role of a “psychological pairing” supported by a constant feedback loop (Harrison and Rouse, 2014) at the base of interpersonal intimate co-creation process in which ideas flow continuously between partners.

On the empirical level, the link between primary forms of interpersonal coordination – behavioral synchronization – and dyadic creativity has started to be investigated only recently. Won et al. (2014) used automatically detected measures of synchrony – which they defined as “the temporal linkage of the non-verbal behavior of two or more interacting individuals” to retrospectively predict performance in a creative collaborative task, in which dyads were invited to generate novel strategies to conserve resources. Results of this experiment showed a significant relationship between spontaneous coordination and creativity, indicating that higher behavioral synchronization was associated with a higher number of new and valid ideas produced by the couples. Interestingly, the authors highlighted that synchronous behavior may be linked to the concept of rapport, defined as “a state of mutual positivity and interest that arises through the convergence of non-verbal expressive behavior in an interaction” (Drolet and Morris, 2000, p. 27). In the same vein, Weinstein et al. (2010) showed that connectedness between partners (i.e., the extent to which partners feel close), their mutual engagement, the presence of responsive interaction and their level of wellbeing were all significant predictors of creative dyadic performance.

The presents study adds to the emerging literature on sensorimotor synchronization and interpersonal creativity by investigating the direct impact of induced behavioral interpersonal synchronization on a subsequent dyadic creativity task. Here, we conceived interpersonal creativity as the generation of novel, original, useful, and feasible products by means of some sort of collaborative process (Torrance, 1966; Amabile, 1996; Paulus et al., 2010) raging from the one arising from a brainstorming session in a group of businessman or designers (Rawlinson, 2017) or from a couple of individuals. Moreover, as concerns the creative outcome, we defined creativity also in terms of fluency, that is, the number of ideas produced to solve a problem, flexibility, i.e., number of different conceptual categories arising from a set of ideas, and elaboration, that is, the number of details associated to a single idea (Guilford, 1950; Torrance et al., 1989). We chose to work with couples as dyads provide the basic prototypical condition for studying interpersonal synchronization arising from a joint action task in a controlled setting. Moreover, although few studies on diversity and dyadic creativity exist (e.g., Cohen et al., 1960; Hoffman and Maier, 1961; Triandis et al., 1965; Torrance, 1970), it is still unclear how creativity takes place in dyads, especially in the organizational field (Rouse, 2018). Dyads may display different processes enabling collaborative creative work.

Based on previous theoretical models linking interpersonal coordination and creative collaboration (Gaggioli et al., 2013a,b,c; Rouse, 2018), we argued that the flow of sensorimotor information occurring between two interacting partners involved in a joint motor task (i.e., building a tower together) may facilitate co-generation of ideas, resulting in higher levels of dyadic creativity. In short, we expected that pairs who had been primed with a synchronization-conductive task would generate more and better ideas than pairs who had performed the same task alone. In testing this hypothesis, we also wanted to control for diverse set of factors that may influence dyadic cooperation, such as disposition to cooperate, cognitive style, perceived interpersonal attraction, and gender. Actually, increasing evidence shows that gender modulates patterns of synchronization in dyads both at behavioral and at neural level (e.g., Abney et al., 2015; Cheng et al., 2015, 2017; Cornejo et al., 2017; Fishburn et al., 2018). For instance, mixed pairs showed significant correlations between increased brain synchronization and high cooperative performance (Cheng et al., 2015). Furthermore, gender composition influenced dyadic performance differently (Strough and Diriwächter, 2000), although effects of gender diversity on dyadic performance have been discussed for a long time (e.g., Cohen et al., 1960; Hoffman and Maier, 1961; Triandis et al., 1965; Torrance, 1970). For instance, a study by Weinstein et al. (2010) showed that stimulating pairing in couples with an orientation task (i.e., autonomy vs. control vs. no-orientation) can enhance their subsequent creative performance in relation to gender-composition, although authors did not fully elucidate the role of gender. Accordingly, in the current study, we did not formulate specific hypotheses on the role of gender-diversity, but we adopted an explorative approach.

Materials and Methods

Participants

120 participants, 60 males (mean age = 23.32; SD = 1.91), and 60 females (mean age = 22.48; SD = 1.53) took part to the experiment. Participants were undergraduate students recruited through campus announcements at universities sited in Milan. Participants were assigned to couples randomly, after matching these criteria: (i) couple members were previously unacquainted individuals and could not talk each other before the beginning of the experiment; (ii) couple members had the same manual dominance (both right-handed or left-handed). Three types of couples were compared: male couples, female couples, and mixed-gender couples. The experimental protocol was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore prior to data collection. Each participant provided written informed consent for study participation in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration. No reward was provided to participants.

Measures

Pre-experiment Measures

Prior to experiment, to control for potentially intervenient effects of cooperation-related variables, we administered three questionnaires assessing: (i) disposition to cooperate, (ii) manual dominance, and (iii) cognitive style, as follows: Disposition to cooperate was assessed using the Cooperativeness scale of the Temperament and Character Inventory-Revised (TCI-R) (Cloninger et al., 1994; Fossati et al., 2007). Cooperativeness scale is composed by 36-items on a five-point Likert scale (1, absolutely false; 5, absolutely true). It includes five subscales (Social Acceptance; Empathy; Helpfulness; and Pure-hearted Conscience) and has a good internal consistency, with a total Cronbach Alpha of 0.85.

Handedness was assessed using a 12-items questionnaire (Briggs and Nebes, 1975). We controlled this dimension in order to match people with same handedness. The Cognitive Style Indicator (CoSI) (Cools and Van den Broeck, 2007) was used to control for the disposition to adopt a preferential cognitive style to solve problems. The CoSI distinguishes between three cognitive styles: (i) a knowing style, emphasizing logic, objectivity, and precision; (ii) a planning style, emphasizing structure, control, and routines; (iii) a creating style, emphasizing subjectivity, impulsivity, and openness to possibilities. These dimensions are measured through 18-items using five-point ratings (1 = it does not describe me at all; 5 = it describes me perfectly). The Cognitive Style Indicator has shown acceptable internal consistency, with Cronbach’s alpha coefficients ranging from 0.73 to 0.85.

Dyadic Creativity Task

Drawing on theorization of Amabile (1983) and Guilford (1950, 1959, 1967), and following the methodological guidelines provided by Hashemi Farzaneh et al. (2012), we designed an ad hoc task to measure dyadic creativity, which consisted in asking participants to generate as many ideas as possible about how to safely park a bicycle. Each dyadic brainstorming session was video-recorded for later analysis. Two independent raters were involved to assess ideas arising from pairs discussion using video-recorded materials. Raters did not already know each other and had different expertise: Rater 1 was a 25-years-old master graduated in Design & Engineering; Rater 2 was a 28-years-old medical doctor. Both raters were instructed to code the generated ideas using the following criteria:

(1) Quantity, which refers to the number of ideas generated;

(2) Appropriateness, which refers to the number of ideas considered appropriate to the task;

(3) Elaboration, which refers to the number of details associated to an idea.

(4) Feasibility, which refers to which extent an idea could be realized (1, absolutely impractical; 7, absolutely feasible).

(5) Usefulness, refers to the degree in which the solution is useful. Two independent judges were requested to state their opinion on a seven-point Likert scale (1, absolutely useless; 7, absolutely useful).

(6) Originality, refers to the extent to which every idea is rare. Two independent judges were requested to state their opinion on a seven-point Likert scale (1, absolutely common; 7, absolutely rare).

(7) Flexibility, refers to the extent to which the whole creative session was creative regarding the viewpoints adopted to approach the topic/problem assigned. Every semantic category to which ideas were attributable counted one score.

Post Experimental Measures

At the end of the experimental session, participants were required to report the extent to which they experienced interpersonal attraction by filling the Measurement of Interpersonal Attraction (McCroskey and McCain, 1974). This questionnaire includes 18-items on a five-point Likert scale (1, absolutely disagree; 5, absolutely agree) and is composed by three subscales: social attraction, physical attraction, and task attraction.

Procedure

This study employed a between-subjects design. Pairs were randomly assigned to either the experimental condition or to a control one. Pairs assigned to the experimental condition were asked to build a tower together, as quick as possible, using a total of 12 colored cubes (thus, each participant used 6 cubes) (Figure 1). Couple members had to pay attention to not pick a cube before the partner had released the other one. Each trial, the experimenter assigned couple members their leader/follower role. The leader started choosing a color and the follower had to continue with the same color. In contrast, couples assigned to the control condition were required to build the tower individually. In both conditions, participants were instructed to build a total of 10 towers. The COLLEGO platform, a custom-made device developed to allow measuring participants’ moves and time during the execution of the task (D’Ausilio et al., 2015; Coco et al., 2016; Chirico et al., 2016). The experimental condition required participants to build the tower jointly. When a cube was picked/released, timestamp (ms), and position of the selected object was recorded. Participants in the control condition executed the same task facing each other, in the same room but independently (Figure 2). Thus, within this condition, each participant used only 6 of the 12 cubes available. At the end of the tower-building task, all pairs were involved in the creativity task (5 min length).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Couple involved in the tower-building task using the COLLEGO platform in the experimental condition (joint).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Couple involved in the tower-building task using the COLLEGO platform in the control condition (not-joint).

Results

Data Analyses

Analyses were done using IBM SPSS Statistics software (Version 21, release 21.0.0.0 64-bit edition). First, we conducted a normality test, testing kurtosis, and asymmetry for dyadic creativity measures (Quantity, Appropriateness, Elaboration, Feasibility, Usefulness, Originality, and Flexibility). We found that all variables followed a normal distribution.

Creativity Measures

We estimated inter-rater reliability between the two raters for each creativity dimension, using Cronbach’s alpha. Internal consistency was from acceptable to high, with alpha coefficients ranging from 0.519 to 0.936 (Quantity: 0.936; Appropriateness: 0.913; Elaboration: 0.519; Feasibility: 0.659; Usefulness: 0.778; Originality: 0.577; Flexibility: 0.833). Thus, judges’ scores were aggregated to obtain a single value for each creativity dimension. Next, a Factorial ANOVA 2 (Condition: joint vs. non-joint) × 3 (Gender: Male-Male vs. Female-Female vs. Male-Female) was conducted on creativity dimension scores. Results showed that there was no main effect of Condition. A main effect for gender was found on three creativity dimensions: Quantity [F(2,54) = 3.183, p = 0.049; η2 = 0.083], Appropriateness [F(2,54) = 3.25, p = 0.046; η2 = 0.085], and Flexibility [F(2,54) = 8.525, p = 0.001; η2 = 0.192] in the joint condition. Specifically, post hoc comparisons using Bonferroni showed that M–M couples achieved significantly higher scores of Quantity (mean = 27.40; SD = 10.79) compared with M–F couples (mean = 21.25; SD = 7.30), and F–F couples (mean = 23.90; SD = 7.15). No significant difference was found between F–F and M–F couples in terms of Quantity. At the same time, M–M couples generated a significantly higher number of appropriate solutions (mean = 23.90; SD = 9.16) than M–F couples (mean = 18.70; SD = 6.08), and, than F–F couples (mean = 20.65; SD = 6.04). No significant difference emerged between F–F and M–F couples in terms of Appropriateness. Finally, ideas produced by M–M were characterized by significantly higher level of Flexibility (mean = 10.40; SD = 1.98) compared with those generated by F–F couples (mean = 8.10; SD = 2.25), and M–F couples (mean = 8.75; SD = 1.86) (see Table 1 for general descriptive statistics and Table 2 for post hoc analyses).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of each creativity dimensions for each gender couple in both conditions: Mean and Standard Deviation.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Post hoc analysis of gender differences within each condition (joint vs. not-joint) concerning quantity, appropriateness, flexibility, duration.

A significant interaction effect was found between Condition and Gender for each creativity dimension: Quantity [F(2,54) = 8.066; p = 0.001; η2 = 0.211] (Figure 3), Appropriateness [F(2,54) = 8.196, p = 0.001; η2 = 0.213] (Figure 4), and Flexibility [F(2,54) = 7.494, p = 0.001; η2 = 0.169] (Figure 5). Specifically, M–M couples had higher Quantity scores in the joint condition (mean = 32.90; SD = 11.49) than in the non-joint one (mean = 21.90; SD = 6.80); the opposite result was found in F–F couples, which produced significantly more ideas in the non-joint condition (F–F couples mean = 27.50; SD = 7.15) than in the joint one (F–F couples mean = 20.30; SD = 5.29). M–F did not show differences regarding quantity in the not joint-condition (M–F couples mean = 23.50; SD = 7.21) compared to the joint condition (M–F couples mean = 19.00; SD = 7.00). M–M couples had higher Appropriateness scores in the joint condition (mean = 28.60; SD = 10.29) than in the non-joint one (mean = 19.20; SD = 4.73), whereas only M–F couples produced more appropriate ideas in the not-joint control condition (M–F couples mean = 21.30; SD = 5.03) than in the joint one (M–F couples mean = 16.10; SD = 6.14). M–M couples had higher Flexibility scores in the joint condition (mean = 11.30; SD = 2.11) than in the non-joint one (mean = 9.50; SD = 1.43). Again, F–F couples and M–F couples showed the opposite trend, adopting a significantly higher number of semantic categories in the non-joint condition (F–F couples mean = 9.10; SD = 1.91; M–F couples mean = 9.80; SD = 1.81) than in the joint one (F–F couples mean = 7.10; SD = 2.18; M–F couples mean = 7.70; SD = 1.25). The other creativity dimensions (i.e., Elaboration, Feasibility, Usefulness, and Originality) did not show any significant effect. Please see Table 3 for post hoc analyses on gender and condition (see Table 3 for post hoc referring to gender differences within each condition).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Interaction effect of Gender by Condition on creativity dimension of Quantity. M–M, comparison between male pairs in the joint vs. not-joint condition; F–F, comparison between female pairs in the joint vs. not-joint condition; M–F, comparison between mixed gender pairs in the joint vs. not-joint condition. p < 0.05; ∗∗p < 0.01.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Interaction effect of Gender by Condition on the creativity dimension of Appropriateness. p < 0.05.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Interaction effect of Gender by Condition on creativity dimension of Flexibility. p < 0.05; ∗∗p < 0.01.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Post hoc analysis of gender differences within each condition (joint vs. not-joint) concerning quantity, appropriateness, flexibility, duration.

Tower-Building Time

A second set of analyses focused on determining differences in tower-building time within the joint-action condition. We computed the time each couple took to complete a single tower (trial) and calculated the average time across the 10 trials. A one-way ANOVA was conducted to determine if gender composition influenced average tower-building time. Results showed a significant effect of Gender on Time [F(2,27) = 6.320, p = 0.006]. Post hoc tests using Bonferroni correction revealed that same-gender couples (M–M: mean = 17668.78; SD = 7595.72; F–F: mean = 20529.71; SD = 5473.70) were significantly faster than mixed-gender couples (mean = 28528.73; SD = 7921.08), while no significant difference was found among M–M and F–F couples. To test the causal impact of tower-building time (“Time”) on creativity dimensions for each gender composition type (M–M; F–F; M–F), we carried out 21 Simple Linear Regression models considering “Time” as predictor and creativity dimensions as dependent variables. Results of Simple Linear Regression are reported in Table 1. The measure of Time resulted significant predictor of Appropriateness, Originality, and Flexibility in M–M couples, while it emerged as a significant predictor of Elaboration only in M–F couples. Please, see Table 4 for results of the Linear Regression models.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Results of linear regression models describing the influence of tower-building time on creativity dimensions.

Discussion

We evaluated the effects of prior interpersonal sensorimotor coordination (present or absent) on a subsequent creativity task, in dyads of varying gender composition. We hypothesized that the behavioral flow of information promoted by the joint-action task would be reflected into an enhanced flow of ideas between partners. This latter process dwells at the base of dyadic creativity. We also assumed that the creativity outcome would be shaped by pairs’ gender composition. Some authors (Miles et al., 2009; Gaggioli et al., 2013a,b, 2015; Chirico et al., 2016; Rouse, 2018) suggested that the experiential correlates of this process can be found in the emergence of a sense of rapport, or as a psychological pairing, or sense of mutual engagement and connectedness between members. Here, we focused on the direct link between behavioral and cognitive performance, in line with current perspectives of Embodied creativity (e.g., Won et al., 2014; Byrge and Tang, 2015; Noy et al., 2015; Stanciu, 2015). This research revealed the role of previous behavioral synchronization, as well as gender on dyadic creative performance. Results indicated: (i) a main effect of gender composition; (ii) no main effect of the sensorimotor coordination condition; and (iii) a significant interaction effect between prior task and gender composition. We will discuss these results in the same order. Gender composition affected key dimensions of creativity performance in the joint condition. Specifically, M–M couples generated more flexible solutions compared with those generated by F–F couples and M–F couples; moreover, M–M generated a higher number of ideas, which were also more appropriate and flexible, than female and mixed-gender couples. No differences in creativity performance were observed between F–F and mixed-gender dyads nor in the joint neither in the not-joint condition. At the sensorimotor level, same-gender couples outperformed compared to mixed-gender couples, but there was no significant difference between M–M and F–F in terms of behavioral performance.

Crucially, only half participants in the joint condition was primed with a behavioral synchronization task. Participants’ performance at the behavioral synchronization task in the joint condition was specular to participants’ performance after the creativity task. Specifically, gender-based pairs who performed the tower-building task together showed the same performance trend both during the behavioral task and after the subsequent dyadic creativity task: males outperformed compared to mixed couples in terms of tower-building time, as well as in terms of Quantity (i.e., M–M significantly outperformed compared to F–F and M–F), Appropriateness (i.e., M–M significantly outperformed compared to F–F and M–F), and Flexibility (i.e., M–M significantly outperformed compared to F–F and M–F) dimensions of creativity. Although no main effect of condition was found, a completely opposite pattern emerged for creativity performance after the solo task. Females and mixed-gender couples in the not-joint condition significantly outperformed after the creativity task compared to males, who resulted as the weakest in all creativity dimensions after the not-joint condition.

These findings would be a preliminary evidence in favor of a carry-over effect from the motor task to the subsequent creativity task, which was modulated by gender composition. Therefore, in order to test this carry-over effect, we analyzed the relationship between tower-building time, gender composition and creativity performance in predictive terms. This analysis concerned only couples in the joint condition. Regression models showed that M–M couples’ execution time during the tower-building task predicted subsequent levels of creativity in terms of appropriateness, originality, and flexibility. In contrast, tower-building time did not predict creativity performance in F–F couples, and only marginally in mixed-gender couples (i.e., only for the creativity dimension of elaboration). Thus, results of regression were generally coherent with the observation that only M–M couples’ creativity performance benefited from the interpersonal movement coordination involved in the tower-building task, whereas F–F and cross-gender dyads showed superior creativity after the solo condition.

This research can provide some theoretical contributions to the field of creative collaboration in general and in particular to dyadic creativity. Prior studies on creativity have provided several evidences for understanding the individual or team-level determinants of successful collaboration (e.g., Andriopoulos, 2001; Sosa, 2011; Parjanen, 2012), but only little experimental research has been oriented to understand dyadic creativity and its determinants (e.g., Weinstein et al., 2010; Won et al., 2014). Specifically, although being in synch resulted as a key feature of highly creative dyads (e.g., Weinstein et al., 2010), no research has investigated the role of induced behavioral synchronization on a following creative performance. Therefore, the main aim of this study was to enhance our understanding of the determinants of interpersonal creativity by focusing, first, on interpersonal sensorimotor synchronization. To this end, we could not overlook the role of gender in shaping dyadic cooperative performance at the tower-building task. First, literature has evidenced that males tend to naturally coordinate better than females at a motor level (Thomas and French, 1985; Kauranen and Vanharanta, 1996; Hamill et al., 2005; Hall, 2006; Barrett et al., 2008; Chraif and Aniţei, 2013). We did not find the same pattern in our joint-action condition in which participants were required to cooperate (synchronize) to accomplish the same goal together (build a tower of cubes). Both males and females outperformed compared to mixed-gender couples: gender matters.

In line with this, we also observed that behavioral synchronization between partners was not only associated to dyadic creativity (Weinstein et al., 2010). Specifically, interpersonal synchronization fostered dyadic creativity, but only if considering also the role of dyads’ gender composition. This finding is not new to the wider literature on gender-diversity and group creativity (Campbell et al., 2013; Coursey et al., 2018). However, usually, heterogeneous groups showed better creative performance than homogeneous ones (e.g., McCroskey and McCain, 1974; Wood, 1987; Campbell et al., 2013). For instance, Schruijer and Mostert (1997) found that mixed-gender groups involved in creativity brainstorming tasks were more creative than same-gender ones, and other studies showed that couples of heterogeneous genders produced more ideas, made more associations among different ideas and took more viewpoints than same-gender ones (McCroskey and McCain, 1974; Wood, 1987). Here, we demonstrated that gender-diversity worked differently in dyads. Heterogenous couples showed the worst creative performance on each of the significant creativity dimensions (Quantity, Appropriateness, Flexibility). This is crucial since previous research on diversity and dyadic creativity have focused only on subjective variables (e.g., Cohen et al., 1960; Hoffman and Maier, 1961; Triandis et al., 1965; Torrance, 1970), while this is the first study that elucidated the role of gender, as a cross-domain “surface-level” variable (Horwitz and Horwitz, 2007, p. 990) on shaping dyadic interactions.

By including gender in our model, we moved forward also research in the sensorimotor synchronization domain. There is a common assumption on the role of interpersonal synchronization on other social or cognitive processes such as comprehension, cooperation, affiliation (Richardson and Dale, 2005; Hove and Risen, 2009; Wiltermuth and Heath, 2009; Valdesolo et al., 2010) suggesting that more (synchrony) means better (Abney et al., 2015; Paxton and Dale, 2017; Lozza et al., 2018). However, Abney et al. (2015) already evidenced that dyads’ gender composition combined with behavioral synchronization could influence the way in which interacting participants can develop productive forms of collaboration over time. Specifically, Cheng et al. (2015) speculated that increased interpersonal synchronization and high cooperative performance correlated in mixed-gender couples. However, the combined effect of gender and induced interpersonal synchronization on dyadic creativity was still an open question. This study brought evidence in favor of including gender in an effective model on dyadic creativity since it oriented the final creative outcomes differently. Only homogeneous pairs were more creative than mixed gender pairs after the tower-building task, which was designed to promote interpersonal synchronization. On the contrary, being in synch was detrimental for the creative performance of female and mixed-gender pairs. Surprisingly, interpersonal synchronization performance negatively influenced the ability to elaborate detailed creative ideas in mixed-gender couples. Being a member of a mixed-gender couple led to less detailed creative solutions. Despite diversity has been conceived as a source of new ideas and a facilitator of unusual combinations among them, some authors have suggested that it may be detrimental to interpersonal creativity since members need to coordinate in order to process new information and translate it into a concrete output (Huckman and Staats, 2011).

Implications for Organizations

Since sensorimotor synchronization can be conceived as a basic cross-domain process, by testing its impact on dyadic creative performance, we could pave the way for designing new creativity-enhancing trainings, which can be potentially implementable into different domains, such as in the organizational field in which this topic is still emerging (e.g., Rouse, 2018). That is, if preliminary evidence exists regarding how to promote group creativity in organizational setting (Shalley et al., 2004; Anderson et al., 2014; Paulus et al., 2018), far less clear is how to promote dyadic creativity into organizations (e.g., Rouse, 2018).

Our results, albeit preliminary, could be useful to design novel strategies for improving creativity of pairs in organizational settings, i.e., by defining specific preparatory motor activities able to impact on dyadic creativity. For instance, some studies have evidenced that simple tasks such as tandem walking promoted a behavioral synchronization (Van Ulzen et al., 2008). A more engaging task to promote interpersonal synchronization could be the one developed by Fusaroli et al. (2016) using creative LEGO construction task. Also, the mirror game (in which participants are required to mimic each other alternatively) is a potential source of interpersonal synchronization (Hart et al., 2014; Cornejo et al., 2017). All these tasks could promote a sensorimotor flow of information between partners, which we postulated and demonstrated at the base of flow of ideas at the cognitive level. Crucially, this flow of information can be generated even between unacquainted partners, without prior arrangement or knowledge (Pezzulo et al., 2018). Therefore, these tasks could prime interpersonal scenarios typical in the organizational field, such as the meeting between a mentor with a new protégé. Mentoring practices are usually based on a dyadic relationship between a less experienced employee (i.e., the protégé) and a more expert individual (i.e., the mentor) aiming at integrating and including newcomers with diverse background within the same organization (Ragins and McFarlin, 1990). These practices, which are crucial for organizational advancement (Bolton and Humphreys, 1977) and involve an interplay of “diversities” (background, race, gender, culture), could be a fertile domain in which testing the effectiveness of this interpersonal synchronization task to foster dyadic creativity. With this regard, despite these mentoring practices have been recognized as crucial for general creativity retrospectively (Torrance, 1980, 1981, 1983, 1984, 2002), how to shape mentoring practices toward creativity outcomes is still an open issue (Form et al., 2017). With this regard, a future step could concern combining actual mentoring practices with an interpersonal synchronization training, maybe based on one of the above-mentioned synchronization tasks, in order to maximize the creative potential of the dyads composed by a mentor and a protégé.

Limitations and Future Research

To our best knowledge, this is the first experiment showing a carry-over effect of a sensorimotor coordination task on subsequent dyadic creativity task. Due to the novel nature of the study, there are some limitations. First, a limitation of this work concerns the nature of the sample, which was small and composed of only students. However, our small sample size allowed for a mean power of 90% for interaction effects of target variables, that concerns our main hypotheses. Moreover, our aim was to study a cross-domain process (a form of interpersonal coordination, that is, behavioral synchronization), under controlled conditions. Therefore, future studies should consider also other populations and contexts, such as organizations, to test the extent to which our results are generalizable. Next, it should be noted that we selected a relatively “neutral” creativity task (i.e., brainstorming), which is usually adopted across different domains. Therefore, it could be useful to test whether our model holds even in domains of dyadic collaboration that are characterized by different levels of structuration of activities, such as within the design, engineering or artistic domains, or research collaboration.

Despite the preliminary nature of this study, results clearly evidenced that dyadic creativity can be also a function of gender and preparatory stages of a creativity task. Although our pattern of results slightly diverge from the body of studies on group creativity and diversity, it may be a preliminary experimental evidence that mechanisms underlying dyadic creativity are different from those concerning group creativity. With this regard, Rouse (2018) suggested that dyads may display different processes enabling collaborative creative work. For instance, creativity in dyads can don the guise of an intimate co-creation process in which ideas flows between partners sustained by a psychological pairing (Rouse, 2018) and constant feedback (Harrison and Rouse, 2014). Therefore, the degree of intimacy would make the difference with groups. Future studies should test these assumptions and consolidate, and further elucidate our findings.

Another useful future step could concern the role of other variables relevant for the organizational context, such as participants’ self-definition (Jung and Lee, 2015) or coping styles in relation to gender (Watson et al., 2011). Specifically, it would be useful to study creativity dynamics in organizations starting from a leader-employee unit of analysis, which is emerging as a promising field of analysis (e.g., Mittal and Dhar, 2015). This would be far more crucial if our synchronization task was able to trigger specific collective efficacy-related process (Bandura, 1977, 2000), which resulted as key factors in the relationship between managers and employees (e.g., Chen and Bliese, 2002; George, 2007; Paulus and Kenworthy, 2017), as well as drivers of team creativity (Kim and Shin, 2015) and group’ general performance (Zaccaro et al., 1995). With this regard, in this study, males’ natural better performance at motor coordination task could have promoted higher sense of collective efficacy stemming from the behavioral performance itself and turned into higher creativity levels. Finally, collective efficacy resulted also sensitive to gender differences (Lee and Farh, 2004).

However, literature on collective efficacy has focused on general group performance (not on creativity) without considering a previous task. Therefore, we could only assume that, in this study, females could have felt more competent when they performed the task alone (in this study, we refer to the “solo condition”), and this could have acted a driver of higher collective efficacy, which was translated into better dyadic creative performance. Finally, in mixed-couples the natural males’ ability to coordinate could have been dampened by the females’ behavioral contribution to the task, thus decreasing collective efficacy. In short, it would be useful to elucidate whether collective efficacy is related to gender diversity and dyadic creativity on the base of the task in which the dyads had been previously involved.

Conclusion

Despite organizations are populated with countless dyadic relationships, how to unlock the creative potential of these links is still an open issue. The current work adds new insight into how dyadic creativity can be shaped by a combination of a previous synchronization task and gender. By introducing synchronization-conductive tasks, it would be possible to boost creative performance in a differentiated way, thus potentially impacting on organizational effectiveness.

Author Contributions

AC and AnG conceived the main idea of the article. EF collected all data. FF, PC, EF, and AC carried out statistical analyses. AC and AnG wrote the first draft, while AlG, EF, SG, AD, PC, and GR contributed to the final writing and editing of the manuscript. AC and AnG supervised the entire work. All authors read and approved the final version and contributed according to their competences and interests.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Abney, D. H., Paxton, A., Dale, R., and Kello, C. T. (2015). Movement dynamics reflect a functional role for weak coupling and role structure in dyadic problem solving. Cogn. Process. 16, 325–332. doi: 10.1007/s10339-015-0648-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amabile, T. M. (1983). The social psychology of creativity: a componential conceptualization. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 45, 357–376. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.45.2.357

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in Context: Update to the Social Psychology of Creativity. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

Google Scholar

Anderson, N., Potočnik, K., and Zhou, J. (2014). Innovation and creativity in organizations: a state-of-the-science review, prospective commentary, and guiding framework. J. Manag. 40, 1297–1333. doi: 10.1177/0149206314527128

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andriopoulos, C. (2001). Determinants of organisational creativity: a literature review. Manag. Decis. 39, 834–841. doi: 10.1108/00251740110402328

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol. Rev. 84, 191–215. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (2000). Exercise of human agency through collective efficacy. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 9, 75–78. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00064

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, R., Noordegraaf, M. V., and Morrison, S. (2008). Gender differences in the variability of lower extremity kinematics during treadmill locomotion. J. Mot. Behav. 40, 62–70. doi: 10.3200/JMBR.40.1.62-70

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernieri, F. J., Reznick, J. S., and Rosenthal, R. (1988). Synchrony, pseudosynchrony, and dissynchrony: measuring the entrainment process in mother-infant interactions. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 243–253.

Google Scholar

Biocca, F., and Harms, C. (2003). Guide to the Networked Minds Social Presence Inventory v. 1.2: Measures of Co-Presence, Social Presence, Subjective Symmetry, and Intersubjective Symmetry. East Lansing, MI: Michigan State University.

Google Scholar

Bolton, E. B., and Humphreys, L. W. (1977). A training model for women–an androgynous approach. Pers. J. 56, 230–233. doi: 10.1177/104649640003100303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Briggs, G. G., and Nebes, R. D. (1975). Patterns of hand preference in a student population. Cortex 11, 230–238. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(75)80005-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrge, C., and Tang, C. (2015). Embodied creativity training: effects on creative self-efficacy and creative production. Think. Skills Creat. 16, 51–61. doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2015.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, L. G., Mehtani, S., Dozier, M. E., and Rinehart, J. (2013). Gender-heterogeneous working groups produce higher quality science. PLoS One 8:e79147. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0079147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, G., and Bliese, P. D. (2002). The role of different levels of leadership in predicting self-and collective efficacy: evidence for discontinuity. J. Appl. Psychol. 87, 549–556. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.87.3.549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, M., Kato, M., and Tseng, C. (2017). Gender and autistic traits modulate implicit motor synchrony. PLoS One 12:e0184083. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0184083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, X., Li, X., and Hu, Y. (2015). Synchronous brain activity during cooperative exchange depends on gender of partner: a fNIRS-based hyperscanning study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 2039–2048. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chirico, A., Graziosi, S., Ferrise, F., Gallace, A., Mosconi, C., Cazzaniga, M., et al. (2016). “COLLEGO: an interactive platform for studying joint action during an ecological collaboration task,” in Pervasive Computing Paradigms for Mental Health, eds P. Cipresso, S. Serino, Y. Ostrovsky, and J. T. Baker (Cham: Springer), 67–72.

Google Scholar

Chraif, M., and Aniţei, M. (2013). Gender differences in motor coordination at young students at psychology. Int. J. Soc. Sci. Humanit. 3, 147–151. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2016.1151024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cloninger, C., Przybeck, T., Svrakic, D., and Wetzel, R. (1994). TCI—The Temperament and Character Inventory: a Guide to Its Development and Use. St Louis, MO: Washington University.

Google Scholar

Coco, M., Badino, L., Cipresso, P., Chirico, A., Ferrari, E., Riva, G., et al. (2016). Multilevel behavioral synchronisation in a joint tower-building task. IEEE Trans. Cogn. Dev. Syst. 9, 223–233. doi: 10.1109/TCDS.2016.2545739

CrossRef Full Text

Cohen, D., Whitmyre, J. W., and Funk, W. H. (1960). Effect of group cohesiveness and training upon creative thinking. J. Appl. Psychol. 44, 319–322. doi: 10.1037/h0041655

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cools, E., and Van den Broeck, H. (2007). Development and validation of the Cognitive Style Indicator. J. Psychol. 141, 359–387. doi: 10.3200/JRLP.141.4.359-388

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cornejo, C., Cuadros, Z., Morales, R., and Paredes, J. (2017). Interpersonal coordination: methods, achievements, and challenges. Front. Psychol. 8:1685. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01685

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coursey, L. E., Paulus, P. B., Williams, B. C., and Kenworthy, J. B. (2018). “The role of individual differences in group and team creativity,” in Individual Creativity in the Workplace, eds R. Reiter-Palmon, V. Kennel, and J. C. Kaufman (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press. ), 311–338.

Google Scholar

D’Ausilio, A., Badino, L., Cipresso, P., Chirico, A., Ferrari, E., Riva, G., et al. (2015). Automatic imitation of the arm kinematic profile in interacting partners. Cogn. Process. 16(Suppl. 1), 197–201. doi: 10.1007/s10339-015-0699-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drolet, A. L., and Morris, M. W. (2000). Rapport in conflict resolution: accounting for how face-to-face contact fosters mutual cooperation in mixed-motive conflicts. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 36, 26–50. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1999.1395

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fishburn, F. A., Murty, V. P., Hlukowsky, C. O., MacGillivray, C., Bemis, L. M., Murphy, M. E., et al. (2018). Putting our heads together: interpersonal neural synchronization as a biological mechanism for shared intentionality. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 13, 841–849. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsy060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Form, S., Schlichting, K., and Kaernbach, C. (2017). Mentoring functions: interpersonal tensions are associated with mentees’ creative achievement. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 11, 440–450. doi: 10.1037/aca0000103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fossati, A., Cloninger, C. R., Villa, D., Borroni, S., Grazioli, F., Giarolli, L., et al. (2007). Reliability and validity of the Italian version of the Temperament and Character Inventory-Revised in an outpatient sample. Compr. Psychiatry 48, 380–387. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2007.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fusaroli, R., Bjørndahl, J. S., Roepstorff, A., and Tylén, K. (2016). A heart for interaction: shared physiological dynamics and behavioral coordination in a collective, creative construction task. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 42, 1297–1310. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaggioli, A., Chirico, A., Mazzoni, E., Milani, L., and Riva, G. (2017). Networked Flow in musical bands. Psychol. Music 45, 283–297. doi: 10.1177/0305735616665003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaggioli, A., Mazzoni, E., Milani, L., and Riva, G. (2015). The creative link: investigating the relationship between social network indices, creative performance and flow in blended teams. Comput. Hum. Behav. 42, 157–166. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaggioli, A., Riva, G., Milani, L., and Mazzoni, E. (eds). (2013a). Analyzing the Experience of Networked Flow through Social Network Analysis. Networked Flow Towards an Understanding of Creative Networks. Berlin: Springer, 71–99.

Google Scholar

Gaggioli, A., Riva, G., Milani, L., and Mazzoni, E. (2013b). Networked Flow: Towards an Understanding of Creative Networks. Dordrecht: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-5552-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaggioli, A., Riva, G., Milani, L., and Mazzoni, E. (2013c). The Cognitive Foundations of Networked Flow: Intentions, Presence, and Social Presence Networked Flow. Berlin: Springer, 21–53. doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-5552-9_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galimberti, C., Chirico, A., Brivio, E., Mazzoni, E., Riva, G., Milani, L., et al. (2015). Bridging minds: a mixed methodology to assess networked flow. Stud. Health Technol. Inform. 219, 33–36.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

George, J. M. (2007). 9 Creativity in organizations. Acad. Manag. Ann. 1, 439–477. doi: 10.5465/078559814

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guilford, J. (1950). Ñreativity. Àm. Psychol. 5, 444–454.

Google Scholar

Guilford, J. P. (1959). Traits of creativity. Creat. Cultiv. 10, 141–161.

Google Scholar

Guilford, J. P. (1967). The Nature of Human Intelligence. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Hall, J. A. (2006). “Women’s and men’s nonverbal communication: similarities, differences, stereotypes, and origins,” in The Sage Handbook of Nonverbal Communication, eds V. Manusov and M. L. Patterson (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.), 201–218.

Google Scholar

Hamill, J., Heiderscheit, B. C., and Pollard, C. D. (2005). Gender differences in lower extremity coupling variability during an unanticipated cutting maneuver. J. Appl. Biomech. 21, 143–152. doi: 10.1123/jab.21.2.143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, S. H., and Rouse, E. D. (2014). Let’s dance! Elastic coordination in creative group work: a qualitative study of modern dancers. Acad. Manag. J. 57, 1256–1283. doi: 10.5465/amj.2012.0343

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hart, Y., Noy, L., Feniger-Schaal, R., Mayo, A. E., and Alon, U. (2014). Individuality and togetherness in joint improvised motion. PLoS One 9:e87213. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0087213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hashemi Farzaneh, H., Kaiser, M., Schröer, B., Srinivasan, V., and Lindemann, U. (2012). “Evaluation of creativity-structuring solution ideas communicated in groups performing solution search,” in Proceedings of the 12th International Design Conference, Dubrovnik, Croatia, eds D. Marjanovic, M. Storga, N. Pavkovic, and N. Bojcetic (Zagreb: University of Zagre), 1871–1870.

Google Scholar

Hoffman, L. R., and Maier, N. R. (1961). Quality and acceptance of problem solutions by members of homogeneous and heterogeneous groups. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 62, 401–407. doi: 10.1037/h0044025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horwitz, S. K., and Horwitz, I. B. (2007). The effects of team diversity on team outcomes: a meta-analytic review of team demography. J. Manag. 33, 987–1015. doi: 10.1177/0149206307308587

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hove, M. J., and Risen, J. L. (2009). It’s all in the timing: interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Soc. Cogn. 27, 949–960. doi: 10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.949

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huckman, R. S., and Staats, B. R. (2011). Fluid tasks and fluid teams: the impact of diversity in experience and team familiarity on team performance. Manuf. Serv. Oper. Manag. 13, 310–328. doi: 10.1287/msom.1100.0321

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, E. J., and Lee, S. (2015). The combined effects of relationship conflict and the relational self on creativity. Organ. Behav. Hum. Dec. Process. 130, 44–57. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2015.06.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kauranen, K., and Vanharanta, H. (1996). The influence of aging, gender and handedness on motor performance of upper and lower extremities. Percept. Mot. Skills 82, 515–525. doi: 10.2466/pms.1996.82.2.515

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, M., and Shin, Y. (2015). Collective efficacy as a mediator between cooperative group norms and group positive affect and team creativity. Asia Pac. J. Manag. 32, 693–716. doi: 10.1007/s10490-015-9413-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, C., and Farh, J. L. (2004). Joint effects of group efficacy and gender diversity on group cohesion and performance. Appl. Psychol. 53, 136–154. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2004.00164.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lozza, N., Spoerri, C., Ehlert, U., Kesselring, M., Hubmann, P., Tschacher, W., et al. (2018). Nonverbal synchrony and complementarity in unacquainted same-sex dyads: a comparison in a competitive context. J. Nonverbal Behav. 42, 179–197. doi: 10.1007/s10919-018-0273-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCroskey, J. C., and McCain, T. A. (1974). The measurement of interpersonal attraction. Speech Monogr. 41, 261–266. doi: 10.1080/03637757409375845

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miles, L. K., Lumsden, J., Flannigan, N., Allsop, J. S., and Marie, D. (2017). Coordination matters: interpersonal synchrony influences collaborative problem-solving. Psychology 8, 1857–1878. doi: 10.4236/psych.2017.811121

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miles, L. K., Nind, L. K., and Macrae, C. N. (2009). The rhythm of rapport: interpersonal synchrony and social perception. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 45, 585–589. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.02.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mittal, S., and Dhar, R. L. (2015). Transformational leadership and employee creativity: mediating role of creative self-efficacy and moderating role of knowledge sharing. Manag. Decis. 53, 894–910. doi: 10.1108/MD-07-2014-0464

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noy, L., Levit-Binun, N., and Golland, Y. (2015). Being in the zone: physiological markers of togetherness in joint improvisation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:187. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parjanen, S. (2012). Experiencing creativity in the organization: from individual creativity to collective creativity. Interdiscip. J. Inf. Knowl. Manag. 7, 109–128. doi: 10.28945/1580

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulus, P. B., Baruah, J., and Kenworthy, J. B. (2018). Enhancing collaborative ideation in organizations. Front. Psychol. 9:202. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.0202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulus, P. B., and Kenworthy, J. B. (2017). “Overview of team creativity and innovation,” in Team Creativity and Innovation, ed. R. Reiter-Palmon (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 11–38. doi: 10.1093/oso/9780190222093.003.0002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulus, P. B., Levine, D. S., Brown, V., Minai, A. A., and Doboli, S. (2010). Modeling ideational creativity in groups: connecting cognitive, neural, and computational approaches. Small Group Res. 41, 688–724. doi: 10.1177/1046496410369561

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paxton, A., and Dale, R. (2017). Interpersonal movement synchrony responds to high- and low-level conversational constraints. Front. Psychol. 8:1135. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pezzulo, G., Donnarumma, F., Dindo, H., D’Ausilio, A., Konvalinka, I., and Castelfranchi, C. (2018). The body talks: sensorimotor communication and its brain and kinematic signatures. Phys. Life Rev. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2018.06.014 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ragins, B. R., and McFarlin, D. B. (1990). Perceptions of mentor roles in cross-gender mentoring relationships. J. Vocat. Behav. 37, 321–339. doi: 10.1016/0001-8791(90)90048-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rawlinson, J. G. (2017). Creative Thinking and Brainstorming. Abingdon: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9781315259000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Repp, B. H., and Su, Y. H. (2013). Sensorimotor synchronization: a review of recent research (2006–2012). Psychon. Bull. Rev. 20, 403–452. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0371-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, D. C., and Dale, R. (2005). Looking to understand: the coupling between speakers’ and listeners’ eye movements and its relationship to discourse comprehension. Cogn. Sci. 29, 1045–1060. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_29

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rouse, E. D. (2018). Where you end and I begin: understanding intimate co-creation. Acad. Manag. Rev. doi: 10.5465/amr.2016.0388

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sawyer, R. K. (2003). Group Creativity: Music, theater, Collaboration. Hove: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Sawyer, R. K. (2007). Group Genius: The Creative Power of Collaboration. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Schmidt, R. C., Carello, C., and Turvey, M. T. (1990). Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 16, 227–247. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.16.2.227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schruijer, S. G., and Mostert, I. (1997). Creativity and sex composition: an experimental illustration. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psychol. 6, 175–182. doi: 10.1080/135943297399169

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H., and Knoblich, G. (2006). Joint action: bodies and minds moving together. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 70–76. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sebanz, N., and Knoblich, G. (2009). Prediction in joint action: what, when, and where. Top. Cogn. Sci. 1, 353–367. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01024.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Semin, G. R., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). “Grounding social cognition: synchronization, entrainment, and coordination,” in Embodied Grounding: Social, Cognitive, Affective, and Neuroscientific Approaches, eds G. R. Semin and E. R. Smith (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 119–147.

Google Scholar

Shalley, C. E., Zhou, J., and Oldham, G. R. (2004). The effects of personal and contextual characteristics on creativity: where should we go from here? J. Manag. 30, 933–995. doi: 10.1016/j.jm.2004.06.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sosa, M. E. (2011). Where do creative interactions come from? The role of tie content and social networks. Organ. Sci. 22, 1–21. doi: 10.1080/10398560902948464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanciu, M. M. (2015). Embodied creativity: a critical analysis of an underdeveloped subject. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 187, 312–317. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2015.03.058

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strough, J., and Diriwächter, R. (2000). Dyad gender differences in preadolescents’ creative stories. Sex Roles 43, 43–60. doi: 10.1023/A:1007087628278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, J. R., and French, K. E. (1985). Gender differences across age in motor performance: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 98, 260–282. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.260

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torrance, E., Sprini, G., and Tomasello, S. (1989). Test di Pensiero Creativo: (“Torrance tests of creative thinking”). Firenze: Organizzazioni Speciali.

Google Scholar

Torrance, E. P. (1966). Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking: Norms-Technical Manual. Princeton, NJ: Personnel Press.

Google Scholar

Torrance, E. P. (1970). Influence of dyadic interaction on creative functioning. Psychol. Rep. 26, 391–394. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1970.26.2.391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torrance, E. P. (1980). Growing up creatively gifted: a 22-year longitudinal study. Creat. Child Adult Q. 5, 148–158.

Google Scholar

Torrance, E. P. (1981). Predicting the creativity of elementary school children (1958–80) and the teacher who “made a difference.” Gift. Child Q. 25, 55–62. doi: 10.1177/001698628102500203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torrance, E. P. (1983). Role of mentors in creative achievement. Creat. Child Adult Q. 8, 8–15.

Google Scholar

Torrance, E. P. (1984). Mentor Relationships: How they aid Creative Achievement, Endure, Change, and Die. Buffalo, NY: Bearly, Limited.

Google Scholar

Torrance, E. P. (2002). The Manifesto: A Guide to Developing a Creative Career. Westport, CT: Greenwood Publishing Group.

Google Scholar

Triandis, H. C., Hall, E. R., and Ewen, R. B. (1965). Member heterogeneity and dyadic creativity. Hum. Relations 18, 33–55. doi: 10.1177/001872676501800104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valdesolo, P., and DeSteno, D. (2011). Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion. Emotion 11, 262–266. doi: 10.1037/a0021302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valdesolo, P., Ouyang, J., and DeSteno, D. (2010). The rhythm of joint action: synchrony promotes cooperative ability. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 46, 693–695. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Ulzen, N. R., Lamoth, C. J. C., Daffertshofer, A., Semin, G. R., and Beek, P. J. (2008). Characteristics of instructed and uninstructed interpersonal coordination while walking side-by-side. Neurosci. Lett. 432, 88–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.11.070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, S. B., Goh, Y. W., and Sawang, S. (2011). Gender influences on the work-related stress-coping process. J. Individ. Dif. 32, 39–46. doi: 10.1027/1614-0001/a000033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinstein, N., Hodgins, H. S., and Ryan, R. M. (2010). Autonomy and control in dyads: effects on interaction quality and joint creative performance. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 36, 1603–1617. doi: 10.1177/0146167210386385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiltermuth, S. S., and Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and cooperation. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02253.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Won, A. S., Bailenson, J. N., Stathatos, S. C., and Dai, W. (2014). Automatically detected nonverbal behavior predicts creativity in collaborating dyads. J. Nonverbal Behav. 38, 389–408. doi: 10.1007/s10919-014-0186-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, W. (1987). Meta-analytic review of sex differences in group performance. Psychol. Bull. 102, 53–71. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.102.1.53

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaccaro, S. J., Blair, V., Peterson, C., and Zazanis, M. (1995). “Collective efficacy,” in Self-Efficacy, Adaptation, and Adjustment, ed. J. E. Maddux (Boston, MA: Springer), 305–328. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4419-6868-5_11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dyadic creativity, interpersonal coordination, interpersonal synchronization, joint action, gender, networked flow

Citation: Gaggioli A, Falletta EM, Ferrise F, Graziosi S, Gallace A, D’Ausilio A, Cipresso P, Riva G and Chirico A (2019) Effects of Interpersonal Sensorimotor Synchronization on Dyadic Creativity: Gender Matters. Front. Psychol. 9:2604. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02604

Received: 07 June 2018; Accepted: 04 December 2018;
Published: 04 February 2019.

Edited by:

Sujin Lee, Korea Advanced Institute of Science and Technology (KAIST), South Korea

Reviewed by:

Lili Bao, Case Western Reserve University, United States
Juliane J. Honisch, University of Reading, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2019 Gaggioli, Falletta, Ferrise, Graziosi, Gallace, D’Ausilio, Cipresso, Riva and Chirico. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alice Chirico, alice.chirico@unicatt.it; a.chirico@auxologico.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.