Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 24 May 2017
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

The Infant Version of the Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery (Lab-TAB): Measurement Properties and Implications for Concepts of Temperament

  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, WI, United States
  • 2Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, WI, United States
  • 3Department of Psychology, University of Texas at Arlington, Arlington, TX, United States

We describe large-sample research using the Infant Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery (Lab-TAB; Goldsmith and Rothbart, 1996) in 1,076 infants at 6 and 12 months of age. The Lab-TAB was designed to assess temperament dimensions through a series of episodes that mimic everyday situations. Our goal is to provide guidelines for scoring Lab-TAB episodes to derive temperament composites. We also present a set of analyses examining mean differences and stability of temperament in early infancy, gender differences in infant temperament, as well as a validation of Lab-TAB episodes and composites with parent reported Infant Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ; Rothbart, 1981) scales. In general, laboratory observed temperament was only modestly related to parent reported temperament. However, temperament measures were significantly stable across time and several gender differences that align with previous research emerged. In sum, the Lab-TAB usefully assesses individual differences in infant emotionality.

Introduction

Temperament is manifest in individual differences in emotional reactivity and regulation (Goldsmith et al., 1987; Goldsmith, 1993). As an early emerging set of behavioral tendencies, temperament is relatively stable through infancy and childhood, and forms a basis for later personality (Rothbart and Bates, 1998). Individual differences in infant temperament are conveniently assessed by parent report but more convincingly assessed objectively via elicited behavior in the laboratory or at home (Gagne et al., 2011). In this paper, we describe the use of the Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery (Lab-TAB), an internationally used method to assess infant and child temperamental dimensions. We begin with a discussion of the definitional issues differentiating discrete emotion from temperament. Second, we highlight current and historical use of the Lab-TAB. Next, we describe each temperament dimension's observable components and inter-relations among them. Lastly, we briefly discuss existing research on gender differences and continuity in temperament dimensions across development.

The Distinction between Emotional Reactions and Emotional Traits (Temperament)

Despite a long history of conceiving emotional differences as temperamental (Allport, 1937; Campos et al., 1983; Goldsmith, 1993, 1994), the distinction can be confusing. Here, we describe definitional differences between emotion and temperament from the perspective of Lab-TAB. Let us suppose that a single infant is tested in the Stranger Approach episode of the Lab-TAB and shows a fearful facial expression when the stranger first begins to approach. The infant's fearful facial expression should not necessarily be conceived as temperamental, but instead is an emotional reaction. What if the infant continues to show facial fear reactions throughout the Stranger Approach, is this pattern of facial expressions temperamental? Again, no. We have simply observed a series of facial expressions. Now, if 100 infants are tested using a Stranger Approach and we quantify their individual differences in patterns of facial expression, can we call these differences temperamental? Yes; temperament requires an individual differences perspective. However, our designation of these differences as temperament is simply definitional; it is open to dispute. If we now quantify individual differences in the sample to other responses that prove to be correlated with facial fear (such as withdrawal, freezing, looking away, and whimpering or crying) and infer a trait that accounts for correlated responses, we are on firmer ground for calling the pattern of reactions temperamental. Still, competing explanations for the correlated responses exist (e.g., individual differences in the effects of infant state, such as tiredness or hunger). Measured emotional reactions to other novel or threatening stimuli besides the stranger that converge across situation and extend over time (days, weeks, and longer) and demonstrate cross-situational consistency as well as longitudinal stability adds to our confidence that we are observing the manifestation of a temperamental trait, using Allport's (1937) definition. Individual instances of emotional behaviors are never properly viewed as temperamental; patterns of consistent and stable individual differences in emotion manifestation are temperamental, or at least they are consistent with temperament theory.

Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery

At the most recent counting, we have received over 600 requests for the Pre-locomotor, Locomotor and Preschool Lab-TAB manuals from researchers in 36 different countries. We do not intend for Lab-TAB administration, coding or scoring to be prescriptive; therefore, researchers may use different coding schemes and modify administration of the episodes depending on their needs. Developmental scientists who have used the manuals over the last 25 years have addressed important questions about the early development of temperament and emotion from a multi-method perspective. For example, one group of researchers has used infant Lab-TAB data to examine the contextual basis of maternal perceptions of temperament (Hane et al., 2006), and another has examined interactions between negative temperament and parenting behaviors as risk factors for child obesity and obesity interventions (Anzman-Frasca et al., 2013, 2014). Others have examined early social and nonsocial fear and other temperament dimensions assessed in Lab-TAB episodes to predict the development of anxiety in early school years (Buss, 2011; Kiel and Buss, 2011; Brooker et al., 2016). Another group has noted remarkable stability in five temperament dimensions (Dyson et al., 2015) as well as an interaction between elevated cortisol levels and a high quality relationship with the primary parent predicting lower negative emotionality and higher positive emotionality in children from age three to six (Kopala-Sibley et al., 2017), using Lab-TAB assessments. However, psychometric properties of the infant Lab-TAB as a whole have not been examined in great depth or detail although we have examined properties of a home-administered version of Lab-TAB for older children, at age 4.5 years (Gagne et al., 2011). Although many investigators use one or a few Lab-TAB episodes as suitable for their research (e.g., Braungart-Rieker et al., 2010; Degnan et al., 2011; He et al., 2013), a need remains for a comprehensive examination of multiple dimensions of infant temperament to garner a more holistic, person-centered understanding of infant emotional development.

Temperament Dimensions

Following an intellectual tradition dating back to Darwin (1872), discrete emotion theorists such as Tomkins (1982), Ekman (1982), Izard (1972), Izard and Malatesta (1987), and Plutchik (1980) identify fear, anger, sadness, joy, interest, disgust, and surprise as fundamental emotions, along with other feeling states (Ekman, 1982, 1989). Temperament theorists traditionally include activity level as aspects of temperament (Rothbart, 1981; Goldsmith and Campos, 1986) and sometimes point out affective features of this overtly non-emotional temperament construct (Goldsmith and Campos, 1986; Goldsmith et al., 1987). For the purposes of this paper, we examine individual differences in fear, anger, sadness, positive affect, interest/persistence, and activity level.

Although researchers often use a more general measure of negative affect in early infancy, we believe that a broad dimension of proneness to distress is less useful than more focal dimensions of fearfulness, anger proneness, and sadness, which show differences in function, expression, and development (Campos et al., 1983; Clifford et al., 2015). Previous research (Braungart-Rieker et al., 2010; Clifford et al., 2015) has indicated etiological as well as behavioral differences in fear, anger and sadness. Though all conceptually negative, anger is related to approach behaviors whereas fear and sadness are related to withdrawal behaviors. Fear is typically expressed during novel situations whereas sadness is expressed upon loss of an object or need. Thus, evidence from outside Lab-TAB dictates that these three dimensions represent different aspects of hedonic negativity and thus should be examined separately.

Fearfulness

Fear is sometimes exhibited when the infant responds to stimuli that convey the threat of harm or uncertainty, for example, a novel or unfamiliar person. Infant fear is expressed with behaviors such as crying, freezing, or escape. The associated action tendency is withdrawal or avoidance. Fear can be an evolutionarily adaptive mechanism used to protect oneself, and behavioral expressions of fear during infancy can elicit aid from caregivers (Campos et al., 1983; Buss and Goldsmith, 1998). Fear is often grouped with anger and sadness as components of an overall negative affectivity factor (Goldsmith, 1996; Putnam et al., 2001). In addition, fear in infancy predicts behavioral inhibition, the tendency to express distress and withdraw in the face of novelty or uncertainty, in early childhood (Kagan, 1997; Brooker et al., 2013). The individual differences facet of fear is regarded as temperamental.

Anger

As a temperament dimension, anger/frustration is the child's propensity toward expressing a negative affective response within a challenging situation, such as contexts where attack or goal blockage is perceived (Stenberg and Campos, 1990; Gagne et al., 2009). The action tendency for anger is approach (as opposed to fear or sadness), and relevant behaviors are crying, protesting, hitting or pushing, and pouting (Goldsmith and Rothbart, 1991; Buss and Goldsmith, 1998). Anger and frustration are evident early in infancy and considered normative markers of development (Stenberg et al., 1983).

Sadness

Sadness is considered a primary emotion (Izard and Malatesta, 1987), yet it is often included in an overall negativity factor rather than examined independently as a dimension of temperament (Putnam et al., 2001). Like anger, sadness reflects an infant's reaction to goal blockage or loss, but without an approach orientation. Sadness is expressed with crying, pouting, and at times a slumped head and/or shoulders. Sadness also shares a withdrawal action tendency with fearfulness, without the novel or threatening incentive features associated with fearfulness.

Positive Affect

Positive affect is relatively neglected in the literature compared with hedonically negative aspects of infant temperament (Gartstein and Marmion, 2008). Positive affect is exhibited with expressions of laughter, smiling, and certain motor behaviors and vocalizations (Campos et al., 1983). Signs of positive affect appear as early as 2 months of age, when infants start to indicate joy with smiling and positive vocalizations (Sroufe and Waters, 1976). From an evolutionary perspective, the social smile becomes an integral component of socioemotional development, with infants and caregivers utilizing smiling to enhance attachment bonds (Sroufe and Waters, 1976). Similar to anger, positive affect is an approach-related behavior, relating to activity level and impulsivity in childhood (Rothbart et al., 2000; He et al., 2013).

Interest/Persistence

Infant interest or persistence, operationalized as duration of orienting by Rothbart (1981), refers to an infant's tendency to look at or play with an object for an extended period of time in the absence of obstacles or interruptions. Interest/persistence reflects the motivational system underlying many attentional systems and the notion of individual differences in the deployment of interest/persistence is often included as part of a broader dimension such as effortful control (Rothbart and Goldsmith, 1985) or task orientation (Matheny, 1980). These measures show stability across infancy (Rothbart et al., 2000; Auerbach et al., 2004) but should not be confused with the cognitive construct of visual attention used to mark the development of executive function (Colombo, 2002). Indeed, the development of an executive attention system may underlie effortful control (Posner et al., 2016). Gartstein and Rothbart (2003) found that interest and attentional orienting measures cluster with positive affect, but other studies find no relationship between interest, attentional orienting and other temperament dimensions (Matheny, 1980; Rothbart, 1981). Despite its potential role in predicting key developmental milestones and its status as complex set of systems that form a cornerstone of cognitive development, infant interest/persistence is omitted from studies of infant temperament.

Activity Level

Activity level, although not overtly emotion-related, is a traditional temperament dimension (Escalona, 1968). Activity level refers to the typical level of energy expenditure through gross motor movements (Saudino, 2012); this energy expenditure often occurs in the service of attaining goals, much as more emotion-related features of temperament motivate individuals to attain goals. In infancy, activity level can be operationally defined as individual differences in proneness to higher or lower degrees of intensity and frequency of movement of the arms, legs, head, or trunk (Gagne et al., 2009). In general, infants who are more active are also higher on approach based temperamental tendencies such as anger and exuberance (He et al., 2013) and show less sustained attention in toddlerhood (Goldsmith, 1996).

Assessment of Infant Temperament

The idea that temperament dimensions manifest in a diverse set of behaviors is well-established. More discrete and quantitative measurement of these dimensions in infancy becomes difficult because we must depend entirely on behavioral observations without the benefit of knowing with certainty whether the infant interprets the context of assessment, including the affective incentives, in the way that investigators intend. Temperament measures can take many forms: parent reports of infant's responses to various scenarios or laboratory assessment of infant reactions to specific stimuli are often utilized to assess infant behaviors (Rothbart and Goldsmith, 1985). Despite some convergence across parent reported and laboratory assessed infant behaviors, the differences between these two modes of assessment have been explicated extensively (Goldsmith and Rieser-Danner, 1990). Among the key differences are that parents generalize across contexts and time and bring their own biases to bear in interpreting behavior whereas laboratory measures reflect objective, minimally interpreted responses to a specific context over period as short as a minute. Thus, the lack of strong convergence is unsurprising.

Gender Differences in Temperament

The magnitude of gender differences in toddler and child temperament appears to be modest and variable depending on which dimension of temperament is under investigation (Chaplin and Aldao, 2013). A recent meta-analysis of gender differences in temperament indicated that boys tend to be higher in approach based tendencies such as anger, exuberance, and activity level whereas girls are higher in shyness, some fear behaviors, and inhibitory control (Else-Quest et al., 2006). Our results in this paper further examines this issue in the pursuit of describing and validating a laboratory based assessment of infant temperament.

Continuity of Temperament in Infancy

Emotional behaviors vary throughout development; early in infancy, we generally conceive of emotional behaviors as solely reactive in nature. The variety of these reactive behavior increases during infancy, and their solely reactive nature also changes. From 6 to 12 months of age (the age period examined empirically in this paper), infants grow rapidly. During this period, infants develop more muscle tone and the ability to stabilize themselves and sit more easily; they begin to crawl and then to pull themselves to standing, and finally begin to walk. These advances in motor control align with cognitive and motivational changes and support an expanding repertoire of emotional expressions (Izard and Malatesta, 1987). Moreover, increased motor and cognitive control allows infants to better regulate emotions as they approach 12 months of age (Thompson, 1991). In other words, infants develop more ways to express emotion concurrently with developing ways to downregulate emotions—especially hedonically negative emotions—more effectively. Temperament theorists generally expect that individual differences in the tendency to respond to emotionally laden stimuli remain relatively stable across infancy (Goldsmith et al., 1987; Rothbart and Bates, 1998).

Research Questions

In a sample of 6 and 12 month infants, we discuss scale development and scoring of tasks that elicit the primary emotions and describe gender differences and stability in infant temperament. Further, we use multiple methods (parent report and laboratory-based, elicited behaviors) to examine relations across reporter. We address the following questions: (1) How do we construct scores for laboratory assessments of infant temperament? (2) What degree of convergence exists between laboratory and parent report infant temperament? (3) To what extent does gender relate to infant temperament, and are temperament dimensions stable from 6 to 12 months?

Methods

Data were drawn from a longitudinal study of twins examining the development of infant temperament/emotionality. In accordance with the university's Institutional Review Board, families were recruited using multiple methods, including state birth records, newspaper birth announcements, television advertising, and flyers in doctors' offices. See Lemery-Chalfant et al. (2006) for sampling and details regarding recruitment methods.

Participants

Parents and their infant twins (N = 1,076 infants) were assessed in both the laboratory and via questionnaire methods. Some 92.8% of infants were Caucasian (3.2% Hispanic), 3.5% of infants were African-American, and 1.7% were Asian-American. Approximately half the infants (n = 550, 51.5%) were female. Mothers were on average 32 years old (M = 31.86, SD = 4.79) and fathers were 34 years old (M = 33.67, SD = 5.68), with median family income above $50,000. Seventy-five percent (75.4%) of mothers and 77.1% of fathers had completed college and 14.9 and 20.6% of mothers and fathers, respectively, had a high school degree.

Procedures

Infants participated in videotaped laboratory assessments of temperament at 6 and 12 months of age. Mothers brought their infants into the lab and completed the Lab-TAB with each infant tested in a tandem fashion (i.e., separate testing of each twin, conducted in a multi-room laboratory with a large team, such that co-twins could be tested at the same time). Following the laboratory visit, mothers and fathers completed questionnaires measuring infant temperament. This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the University of Wisconsin-Madison's Institutional Review Board with written informed consent from all subjects.

Measures

Infant Behavior Questionnaire

Parent reported infant temperament was measured at 6 and 12 months using the Infant Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ; Rothbart, 1981). The IBQ is one of the most widely used measures of infant temperament. Composed of six scales, it asks parents to rate the frequency of their infants' behaviors (crying, smiling, looking, etc.) across a variety of situations, for example while feeding, bathing, or changing. By asking parents to rate their infant's behavior during specific situations, the IBQ seeks to minimize global perceptions and interpretations of infant temperament (Rothbart and Goldsmith, 1985). Instead, parents remember discrete instances in which their infants acted in a specific way and respond to the IBQ items accordingly. Further, in our study each parent reported on each twin's behavior. The granular nature of IBQ items prevents parents from inflating behavioral differences in their offspring (Goldsmith et al., 1997). Therefore, scales derived from the IBQ items are intended to represent each individual infant's pattern of responding across specific situations.

The IBQ measures temperament using 6 scales: Fear, Distress to Limitations, Smiling, and Laughter, Soothability, Duration of Orienting, and Activity Level. Mothers and fathers rated infant behavior on a seven-point Likert scale, ranging from “never” to “always,” for 94 questions. Internal consistency was calculated for mother and father reports on the IBQ scale at 6 and 12 months and are presented in Table 2. Additionally, mother and father reports were significantly correlated for each scale (see Table 2); thus, we created a composite score for infant temperament using an average of mother and father reports. The IBQ Distress to Limitations and Duration of Orienting scales are comparable to Lab-TAB measures of Anger and Interest/Persistence.

Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery

The Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery (Lab-TAB; Goldsmith and Rothbart, 1991) assesses infant responses to stimuli in a controlled setting and is a standardized way to elicit and score behaviors. Many adult emotion coding systems rely on facial expressions (i.e., AFFEX; Izard and Malatesta, 1987), but infant emotion is also expressed through bodily movements (Weinberg and Tronick, 1994) and vocalizations, which are incorporated in the Lab-TAB coding system. Although numerous studies examine infant emotion using behavioral coding (e.g., Morris et al., 2011), no study focuses on validating the psychometric properties of the Lab-TAB in infants.

Lab-TAB episodes are standardized and designed to measure infant's reactions to stimuli which elicit emotional or behavioral reactivity across five broad dimensions of infant temperament: fearfulness, anger proneness, joy/pleasure, interest/persistence, and activity level. Although the Lab-TAB manual describes 20 episodes, researchers may choose and implement those which best support their study design. At each age, we used two episodes measuring fear, anger, sadness, positive affect, and one episode each for activity level and interest taken from the Pre-Locomotor and Locomotor versions (Goldsmith and Rothbart, 1996). A complete description of procedures and coding is in the Lab-TAB manuals. Visits were videotaped for later scoring. Coders were trained by a highly experienced coder and kappas for scorer agreement on each episode are described below. We give brief episode descriptions and dimensions measured in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. IBQ scales and Lab-TAB episode descriptions and scoring components.

Fear: stranger approach

During Stranger Approach the child is placed in a high chair across the room from a door where a stranger (i.e., male tester unknown to infant) enters. The mother is also in the room but instructed not to interact with the infant. The Stranger Approach occurs in several stages. First, the stranger enters the room and waits for 10 s. Then the stranger slowly moves toward the infant stops approximately halfway into the room (10 s), saying “Hello [infant's name]. I'm going to come a little closer to you now.” The stranger slowly walks close to the infant (10 s) and once the stranger arrives at the infant's location, he kneels down and stares at the infant for 30 s. Behaviors are scored by trained coders in three stages: the stranger's entry (10 s), the stranger's slow approach (20 s), and the stranger staring at the infant 30 s. This 30 s is broken into 6 5-s scoring epochs. Scores for each stage include facial and bodily fear (0–3 scale), facial sadness (0–3 scale) and bodily sadness (0/1), distress vocalizations (0–5 scale), and escape behaviors (0–3 scale). Kappas assessing the reliability of coding ranged from 0.71 to 0.90, with an average κ = 0.80.

Fear: masks

For the Masks episode, the infant is presented with four increasingly scary masks for 10 s each. Each infant is presented the same masks in the same order; first, an evil cartoon queen, second, an old man, third, a green vampire mask, and lastly a gas mask. Each “mask” trial is then divided into two scoring epochs of 5 s. Several behaviors are scored during each trial: facial and bodily fear (0–3 scale), facial (0–3 scale) and bodily (0/1) sadness, distress vocalizations (0–5 scale), and escape behaviors (0–3 scale). Reliability estimates for Masks ranged from 0.68 to 0.87, with an average κ = 0.76.

Anger and sadness: overview

Infant response to goal blockage is often assessed by using laboratory measures of restraint, where the infant is unable to access something he/she wants, namely an attractive toy or free movement. From these tasks, we code an infant's facial, vocalic, and behavioral reactions reflecting both anger and sadness.

Anger and sadness: arm restraint. In the Arm Restraint episode, infants are shown an enticing perpetual motion toy and allowed to play with it until the mother restrains the infant arms so the infant cannot touch or play with the toy. This is repeated twice resulting in two 30 s trials. Each trial is then divided into 6 5-s epochs from which we score bodily struggle (0–4 Likert scale), facial anger and facial sadness (0–3 scale), distress vocalizations (0–5 scale), and bodily sadness (0/1). Average reliability for Arm Restraint was κ = 0.79 (range from κ = 0.69 to κ = 0.83).

Anger and sadness: car seat. In the Car Seat episode, the infant is placed in a car seat by the parent and physically restrained for a total of 30 s. Like Arm Restraint, we code the presence or intensity of facial anger (0–3 scale), bodily anger or struggle (0–4 scale), and distress vocalizations (0–5 scale) every 5 s. Facial sadness (0–3 scale) and bodily sadness (0/1) are also scored during the Car Seat episode. Scoring reliability estimates for Car Seat ranged from 0.63 to 0.93, with an average κ = 0.77.

Positive affect: puppets

For the Puppets episode, the experimenter wears two fun hand puppets and uses them to engage the infant in game playing. Using voices for each of the puppets the experimenter talks to the infant, tickles the infant, and then allows the infant to play with the puppets over the course of 2 min. The first 90 s are divided into 4 scoring epochs. The first starts upon the emergence of the puppets. The second, third, and fourth epochs begin each time the experimenter tickles the infant with the puppets. Finally, the fifth 30 s epoch starts when the experimenter lays the puppets on the table in front of the infant to see if the infant wants to play with them. Scoring is completed for each epoch and several variables are scored based on infant behaviors with or toward the puppets. Smiling, laughter, positive vocalizations and positive motor acts are scored as 0 “not present and 1 “present.” Smiling intensity is scored on a Likert scale ranging from 0 “no smiling” to 3 “large smile.” Scoring reliability estimates for Puppets ranged from 0.80 to 0.89, with an average κ = 0.85.

Positive affect: peekaboo

In the Peekaboo episode, mothers engage the child in a game of peekaboo by standing behind a screen. Over the course of six trials, the experimenter prompts the child with “Where's Mommy? Where did she go?” and then opens one of three doors in a wooden screen. For the first three trials as well as the last trial the mother is behind the screen and says “Peek-A-Boo!” For trials four and five, the mother is not behind the screen and the experimenter says “Oops, not there.” The entire episode lasts approximately 1 min. As in the Puppets episode, several items are scored for each of the six trials: smiling, laughter positive vocalizations and positive motor acts (0/1) and intensity of smiling (0–3 scale). Scoring reliability for Peekaboo had an average κ = 0.86, with a range from κ = 0.84 to 0.92.

Interest/persistence: slides

Laboratory assessments of interest typically measure how long an infant gazes at or manipulates an object in an unstructured task. The infant is oriented toward a screen where he/she views a series of slides depicting children, mothers and their children, and various nature scenes. The infant views 5 slides in three 30-s trials for a total of 15 slides. The first slide is presented for 2 s (Epoch 1), the second slide is presented for 4 s (Epoch 2), the third slide is presented for 6 s (Epoch 3), the fourth slide is presented for 8 s (Epoch 4) and finally the last slide of the trial is presented for 10 s (Epoch 5). This is repeated three times to total 1.5 min. From this, we scored facial interest (0–2 scale), duration of looking (0–3 scale), and the presence of gestures (0/1) and vocalizations (0/1). Scoring reliability estimates for Slides ranged from 0.79 to 0.88, with an average κ = 0.85.

Activity level: basket of toys

During Basket of Toys (6 months only), the infant is placed beside a basket filled with enticing toys, such as a rattle or stuffed animal. The infant's seating is supported and coders observe the patterns of toy play for 3 min. This is divided into 9 20-s scoring epochs. Coders score the intensity of the infant's manipulation of the toys (0–4 scale) and the number of different toys the infant plays with during each epoch.

Activity level: free play

During the Free Play episode (12 months only), children are allowed to move around a room filled with toys, such as a truck, balls, inflatable rings, and a drum. The toys are set up in a circle and the child is left to play with the toys for 6 min. The first minute is a warm-up and is not coded. The remaining 5 min are divided into 15 20-s scoring epochs. By 12 months old, infants have more dexterity and are more mobile, so in addition to the intensity of manipulation of toys (0–4 scale), and number of toys the infant plays with, we also score intensity of movement (0–4 scale) and number changes in body position. Scoring Kappas for the activity level composites ranged from 0.60 to 0.80, with an average κ = 0.71.

Missing Data

Recruitment was ongoing throughout the course of the study; thus, more families participated in the 12-month than 6-month assessment (see Table 2 for Ns, which reflect both the continuing recruitment and missing data). Analyses used full information maximum likelihood estimation (FIML; Enders, 2010), a robust method for approximating unbiased parameter estimates by utilizing all available data.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Internal consistency and cross-parent (mother-father) for IBQ scales and Lab-TAB infant temperament episodes.

Results

First, we describe data reduction methods for each of the Lab-TAB episodes. Second, we examine measures of reliability of each temperament dimension tapped by the IBQ and Lab-TAB at both 6 and 12 months of age. Then, we examine stability from 6 to 12 months. Fourth, we analyze the degree of convergence of each temperament dimension across parent and observed assessments. Lastly, we examine gender differences.

Data Reduction for the LAB-Tab Episode and Dimension Composites

The Lab-TAB yields several scores (e.g., facial affect ratings) for each epoch (intervals of a few seconds) across each trial within each episode of the protocol. The resulting amount of raw data from as few as 10 episodes can be overwhelming. Here, we describe a standardized procedure for scoring the Lab-TAB and reducing the data to more useable episode-level variables for each desired construct. Figure 1 depicts our data reduction strategy to derive temperament composites from Lab-TAB raw data.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Data reduction to derive temperament composites from Lab-TAB raw data.

The first step in data reduction is to create response parameters for behaviors across the episode. For example, in the Masks episode the infant receives a raw score for facial fear on each epoch (n = 2) for each mask (n = 4), resulting in 8 scores. Thus, we average facial fear across the 8 epochs and use this as a mean facial fear score for Masks. By examining the mean level of each behavior that the infant exhibits, we quantify the infant's average reactivity for each episode. Consequently, shorter latencies to show facial fear, longer durations of facial fear, and higher levels of facial fear will all be reflected in higher means. Next, we determine the peak level of facial fear for the episode, whenever this peak might have occurred during the 8 epochs. Peaks and means tend to be correlated, but peaks better capture an infant's tendency to respond to stimuli using extreme behavioral responses. Two infants may have the same mean facial fear scores, but very different peak scores.

Finally, we calculate the latency, or the time before, the occurrence of a behavior. We do so indirectly by counting the number of epochs before the first occurrence of facial fear, for example. If facial fear is apparent right away, then the latency it takes to observe it is 0 because it occurs in the first epoch. If facial fear is not present until the third mask, the latency is 4 because it occurs in the fifth epoch. We then reverse score these latencies to reflect the speed with which the infant exhibited the behavior. This procedure results in mean, peak, and speed scores that are all in the same direction (i.e., higher scores imply more intense or frequent occurrence of a behavior) and tend to be correlated within an episode. We suggest that researchers follow this approach initially then use the mean, peak and speed scores as best fits their research question.

For this paper, we combined mean and peak scores across trials within each episode to create an overall episode score. Factor analyses to determine which scoring components to include in each episodic score and then reliability statistics (Cronbach's alphas) were calculated for each episode at each age. The speed scores did not add significant variation to our episode-level scoring, and these speeds are therefore not included in further analyses. Researchers who are interested in examining how quickly an infant may react to a stimulus, or more generally in examining emotion expression using a chronometric approach (Davidson, 1998) may opt to include the speed scores as well. The resulting episode-level scores include each of the scoring components listed in Table 1. For the fear episodes, escape behaviors and bodily fear (freezing) are mutually exclusive behaviors, so only bodily fear is included in the Stranger Approach and Masks episode-level scoring. We also combined scores across episodes to create an overall score for each temperament dimension of interest. For example, Masks and Stranger Approach both assess infant fear so we combined these for an overall Fear composite (see Figure 1).

Table 2 provides the reliability statistics for the episode-level and composite scores derived from each Lab-TAB episode. For the IBQ scales, alphas were satisfactory and generally consistent with the literature. The IBQ Soothability scale tended to somewhat less internally consistency than the other scales, possibly because the nature of the items on this scale ask about the effectiveness of different soothing techniques. High internal consistency would require most techniques to be equally effective, which may not be the case. Mother-father correlational agreement on all IBQ scales was significant at both ages in this large sample. The magnitude of the agreement might be characterized as modest-to-moderate (correlations in the 0.30–0.50 s), except for Soothability.

Turning to the Lab-TAB (lower portion of Table 2), internal consistency estimates averaged 0.794 at 6 months and 0.786 at 12 months. Variation in internal consistency across composites was limited, with Anger measures tending to be highly consistent and Sadness measures somewhat less so. Although internal consistency was enhanced by the composite construction process and would be lower in a replication sample, we judged these levels of internal consistency as satisfactory.

We created an SES variable measuring mother and father education as well as family income. SES was inversely related to parent reported IBQ Fear at 6 and 12 months of age as well as IBQ Anger and at 12 months of age. It is possible that lower SES parents encounter more external stressors which affect their reporting of infant behaviors. Only one significant relation emerged between SES and observed Lab-TAB temperament (during Basket of Toys at 6 months of age, r = 0.13, p < 0.01); thus, we did not include SES in further analyses.

Parent Reported Infant Temperament

Inter-correlations of the IBQ scales are presented in the upper left portions of Table 3 (6 months) and 4 (12 months). These correlational patterns align with previous literature examining the IBQ (Putnam et al., 2001). Scales assessing infant negativity, namely Fear and Anger, were significantly correlated with each other at 6 and 12 months. Scales assessing infant regulatory behaviors (Soothability and Duration of Orienting) were also significantly related at each age. Smiling and Laughter was positively correlated with Soothability and Duration of Orienting at both ages. Only at age 12 months was the most hedonically positive scale (Smiling and Laughter) negatively correlated with the hedonically negative Fear and Anger scales. Finally, infants who were higher in Activity Level also expressed more emotionally driven behaviors, as reflected in the Fear, Anger and Smiling and Laughter scales at each age.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlations between 6-month temperament measures.

Observed Infant Temperament

Correlations within the Lab-TAB episodes and dimensions are shown in the lower right portions of Table 3 (6 months) and 4 (12 months). The first result is that our strategy for creating cross-episode composites was supported although the magnitude of cross-episode convergence was usually modest. Episodes were significantly correlated (all ps < 0.01) with the other episode within each dimension at each age: Stranger Approach and Masks (fear) scores were related at 6 (r = 0.15) and 12 months (r = 0.18); Arm Restraint and Car Seat Anger were related at 6 (r = 0.28) and 12 months (r = 0.17); Arm Restraint and Car Seat Sadness were related at 6 (r = 0.29) and 12 months (r = 0.27); and finally, Puppets and Peekaboo were related at 6 (r = 0.28) and 12 months (r = 0.28). Thus, we created Lab-TAB dimensions for Fear, Anger, Sadness, and Positive Affect at each age.

In general, episodes within the negative temperament dimension were significantly intercorrelated (Stranger Approach, Masks, Arm Restraint, Car Seat). This pattern was also reflected in the relations among the composites, such that Fear, Anger, and Sadness were consistently related to each other at 6 and 12 months. Activity level was related to Anger, Sadness and Positive Affect episodes at each age as well. Further, Positive Affect was negatively correlated with Anger at 6 and 12 months.

Relations between IBQ and LAB-TAB

Associations between conceptually related questionnaire and observational measures speak to criterion-oriented validity, and those associations were not strong, generally speaking. Parent reported infant fear was related to Car Seat anger and sadness at 6 months (but not the expected fear measures from Lab-TAB), but by 12 months of age parent rated fear was correlated with laboratory measured fear, anger, and sadness. Parent reported distress/anger was modestly correlated, in most but not all cases, with increased anger and decreased sadness in the laboratory at both 6 and 12 months. Parent reported smiling/positivity was unrelated to any laboratory assessed temperament at 6 months, but by 12 months it was related to increased anger and decreased sadness during the Arm Restraint episode. Parent rated infant regulation, namely the Soothability and Orienting scales, were inversely related to laboratory positive affect at each age, but increased fear and interest and decreased anger and sadness at 12 months.

Stability and Gender Differences in Temperament across Time

Our third aim was to examine stability of temperament across time, and determine whether this was similar for boys and girls. Table 5 lists the mean levels of each parent reported and laboratory observed temperament dimension by age and gender. Parents reported girls to be very modestly higher in Fear at both 6 (Cohen's d = 0.21) and 12 months (Cohen's d = 0.15), which was a salient finding, given no other gender differences for parent reported temperament. A similar trend occurred for observed fear for the Stranger and in the Masks episodes of the Lab-TAB although the differences between boys and girls did not reach statistical significance (p = 0.06). However, when Lab-TAB episodes were combined to create an overall observed fear composite, girls exhibited more fear than boys at 6 months but not 12 months.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Correlations between 12-month temperament measures.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Means, standard deviations, and gender comparisons for infant temperament measures.

In the positive affect dimension, no differences in Puppets or overall positive affect at either age occurred, but a significant gender difference did occur for the Peekaboo episode. Boys were rated higher in positive affect coded from the Peekaboo episode of the Lab-TAB at both 6 and 12 months. Boys were scored higher in interest/persistence as measured by the Slides episode and in activity level across time (see Table 5).

Because we standardize the Lab-TAB raw data prior to creating episode scores, and are unable to examine mean level differences in observed temperament. However, we can look at rank order stability across time. Bivariate correlations indicated longitudinal (rank-order) stability in both the parent reported and observed measures of temperament; see correlations across time in Table 6 (left side of the table). The stabilities of IBQ scales (mother + father averages) ranged from 0.41 to 0.59. Although the stabilities of observed behavior reflected in the Lab-TAB composites was substantially lower than the questionnaire scales, only one Lab-TAB episode, Car Seat (Anger), did not show significant longitudinal stability between 6 and 12 months of age. As expected, the cross-episode dimension composites (last 4 rows of Table 6) were more stable than the single episode measures.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Cross-time (rank order) stability for infant temperament measures.

Regarding the stability of temperament across gender, correlations between 6 and 12 months for are presented on the right side of Table 6. We first correlated infant temperament measures across time for girls and boys, and then transformed the Pearson r values to z-scores to compare the degree to which stability existed across gender. For parent reported temperament, significant stability was observed for each scale, and this stability did not differ by infant gender. For Lab-TAB, boys exhibited significantly greater stability than girls in sadness and activity level, indicated by significant z-scores for Car Seat sadness, activity level, and the overall sadness dimension.

Discussion

The Lab-TAB is a useful tool to measure an infant's tendency to respond to situations with emotional incentives. By exposing infants to the same stimuli in a structured laboratory setting, researchers gain control that questionnaires cannot afford. Here, we describe the theoretical background as well as psychometric properties of the Lab-TAB using an existing sample of more than 1,000 infants. It is not our aim to prescribe the Lab-TAB as a fixed entity, nor to provide rigid rules for tis use. Instead, we illustrate the Lab-TAB's utility across multiple systems. We support its development by other researchers who have used Lab-TAB episodes with different coding and data reduction approaches (e.g., Dyson et al., 2012, 2015). We leave it to each researcher to incorporate those episodes that best fit the researcher's hypothesis but we encourage researchers to consider assessing multiple temperament dimensions using multiple episodes.

Dimensional vs. Episodic LAB-TAB Usage

Although fine grained measurement of temperament is informative, researchers often examine infant behaviors in terms of much broader factors such as positivity or surgency, negative affect, and regulation (Putnam et al., 2001). In our study, within measurement method, temperament scales indicative of broader positivity, negativity, and regulatory dimensions were related. That is, infant smiling and laughter, or positive affect, was inversely related to negative dimensions such as fear; fear, and anger were positively related at a given age; and soothability and orienting were positively related. In addition, similar to Bridges et al. (1993), anger and orienting were inversely related.

When incorporating Lab-TAB assessment of infant behaviors, some research questions may pertain to an infant's discrete response to a specific situation, for example, a tendency to respond by struggling to distressing goal blockage such as in the Arm Restraint episode. The use of a single episode from the Lab-TAB may be sufficient to incorporate into such studies. However, to examine inter-relationships among temperament constructs, multiple episodes are obviously needed, and multiple episodes within a domain, such as anger, are needed to claim cross-situational generality. In our study, we first determined scores for individual laboratory episodes by calculating the mean and peak of each infant behavior. In this way, we consider an infant's characteristic reaction throughout the relevant stimulus exposure (mean of response) as well as the most extreme behavior exhibited (peak).

Additionally, temperament theory intimates that temperament reflects an individual's propensity toward emotional reactions; that is, individuals' tendency to respond similarly across multiple situations that share affective incentive characteristics. Accordingly, we also created composite scores to reflect broad (cross-response modality, cross-episode) temperament propensities. Scores from each of the two episodes chosen within each temperament dimension (i.e., fear, anger) were correlated at each age, indicating that the episodes designed to represent a temperament composite elicited similar behaviors from infants. These correlations, however, were only modest, indicating potent situational effects.

In behavioral research, numerous factors typically affect observed behaviors. The adult personality literature offers support for moderate cross-situational consistency of behavior, with many caveats and qualifications (Kenrick and Funder, 1988). Modest cross-episode convergence in the infant version of Lab-TAB may be due to multiple sources. First, infant temperament traits may be a weak organizer of behavior, relative to the situational differences between episodes with apparently similar affective incentives. Second, relative to adults, infants possess a less differentiated sense of self, less developed patterns of affective and self-regulation, and no developed social attitudes. Each of these features of personality may support temperament differences. Third, contextual or incentive differences between episodes may alter the patterning of individual differences in temperament. Or, stated another way, temperament traits may be narrower than researchers tend to believe; for example, “anger proneness” might be too broadly conceptualized. Fourth, state variables in infancy, such as hunger or tiredness, may be powerful enough to obscure temperamental differences. Fifth, the novelty of laboratory context is constant, in a sense, across all Lab-TAB episodes, and this novelty may also obscure temperamental differences that would be apparent in more familiar contexts. Thus, the finding that behaviors across episodes are only modestly correlated should not be surprising. Despite these competing influences on behavior, some evidence of cross-situational consistency in Lab-TAB responses is apparent.

Convergence between Laboratory and Parent Report of Infant Temperament

Although most of what we know about infant temperament comes from research using parent reports, laboratory based assessment can elucidate patterns of emotional reactivity during specific situations. Parent reports and observed behavior are inconsistently correlated, and several investigators have elaborated potential reasons for this inconsistency (Rothbart and Goldsmith, 1985; Goldsmith and Hewitt, 2003; Saudino, 2003; Stifter et al., 2008). In a previous report on scale development and validation of the Preschool Lab-TAB (Gagne et al., 2011), laboratory-based assessments of fear, shyness, activity level and inhibitory control showed low to moderate correlations with corresponding mother- reported temperament dimensions. We and others (e.g., Gartstein and Marmion, 2008; Braungart-Rieker et al., 2010) found moderate convergence between parent reported and laboratory observed infant fear and anger, but not positive affect (Gartstein and Marmion, 2008) or interest/persistence (Matheny and Wilson, 1981; Carranza Carnicero et al., 2000).

The reason for discrepant findings for different aspects of temperament might be that infants and young children actually do exhibit distress across the home and laboratory environments more consistently than they express positive affect and interest/persistence in the two types of environment. Alternatively, parents may be more attuned to interpersonally disruptive infant distress or anger (Huebner and Izard, 1988) than to other less obtrusive temperamental traits. Thus, plausible reasons exist for lack of convergence between seemingly corresponding IBQ and Lab-TAB assessments in some cases. Nonetheless, by considering both observed and parent measures of temperament, we gain a clearer picture of the development of multiple temperament dimensions across infancy. Moreover, the pattern of significant correlations of IBQ by Lab-TAB findings in Table 4 is fairly systematic and sensible. For instance, fear is perhaps the most salient aspect of temperament to parents. Parents' IBQ Fear reports are related to a variety of the more objective Lab-TAB measures, a finding that suggests parents may over-assimilate infant behaviors to their own version of a fear/inhibition/shyness concept, which is broader than our concept of fear. An alternate interpretation of this finding is that the novelty of the laboratory context affects the assessment of multiple dimensions of temperament. Also, we observe cross-method correlations between activity level and positive affect, suggesting a role for approach motivation in structuring these correlations.

Stability and Gender Differences within Temperament Dimensions

Consistent with previous literature (Else-Quest et al., 2006) we found gender differences in parent reported and observed temperament, such that girls were higher in the fear dimensions of temperament, and boys were higher in approach related behaviors such as positive affect and activity level.

Further, few studies use laboratory measures of infant temperament when assessing continuity. In ours and others' research, infants' expressions of fear, anger, and positive affect as well as activity level increase from 6 to 12 months (Carranza Carnicero et al., 2000; Rothbart and Bates, 2006; Planalp et al., 2016); however, less is known about infant sadness and interest/persistence. We found rank order stability of temperament across time, reflected by longitudinal correlations within scale, episode, or dimension.

Although our results indicate significant stability within temperament dimension, temperament can be less stable across developmental stages, such as the widely implicative transition at about age 8–9 months (Emde et al., 1975). For example, infants who express more distress at 6 months are not necessarily higher in anger or fear by school age (Lemerise and Dodge, 2008). Cognitive and motor development may impact the ways in which individuals can express emotion. Thus, although temperament is relatively stable within developmental periods such as mid-infancy we are not able to predict stability across longer periods of time. We discuss findings within each temperament dimension below.

Fear

As infants become more aware of their surroundings and develop expectations for their safety and security, expressions of fear become progressively more apparent between 6 and 12 months of age. Fear in the presence of a stranger emerges, on average, around 8 months of age and increases through the first year, although substantial individual differences exist in developmental trajectories of stranger fear. Parents rated girls higher than boys on fear, and girls exhibited more overall fear than boys at 6 months, consistent with previous research indicating gender differences in parent-reported fear (Else-Quest et al., 2006; Gartstein et al., 2010). In addition, although girls' observed fear was fairly stable from 6 to 12 months and boys' fear was less so, longitudinal correlations did not significantly differ by gender. According to social learning theory, girls learn to be more wary of environmental novelty and discomfort than boys (Kochanska, 1993; Chaplin and Aldao, 2013); thus, initial socialization processes for both parents and infants might account for our findings for gender differences in the development of fear (Malatesta and Haviland, 1982; Kochanska, 1993; Chaplin et al., 2005; Chaplin and Aldao, 2013).

Anger

Similar to previous research (Else-Quest et al., 2006), we did not observe differences between boys' and girls' anger, regardless of measurement method. However, rank order stability was apparent in how infants expressed anger in early infancy, with stronger stability within episode for girls than boys (though this was not significantly different) during the Arm Restraint episode. Comparable to fear responses, anger seems to peak and then dissipate as infants age; by age 3 years, most children do not become overtly angry in the lab-based anger assessments (Gagne and Goldsmith, 2011). The emergence of self-regulation may allow children to better regulate their negative affect; they begin to show more individual differences in expressions of distress which decrease as regulation increases (Gagne and Goldsmith, 2011).

Sadness

Boys were higher in Car Seat Sadness at six, but not 12, months. In addition, although all infants' expressions of sadness during Arm Restraint were stable across time, boys exhibited stability in Car Seat sadness whereas girls did not. At 6 months of age, girls might already be beginning to regulate their expressions of distress to limitations better than boys, or alternatively, girls might be more variable in how they express distress to limitations—sometimes withdrawing and other times approaching—resulting in lower stability in sadness. Other than Car Seat, individual differences in observed infant sadness were relatively stable across time and, similar to previous studies concerning fear (Goldsmith and Lemery, 2000). Perhaps their common association with motivation to withdraw accounts for the links between sadness and fear (Buss and Goldsmith, 1998).

We also found that anger and sadness measures from the same episodes were substantially correlated. Anger and sadness were the only affects that were not measured independently, because we were unable to devise ethical (non-harsh) episodes involving loss or goal blockage that reliably elicited sadness but no anger, or vice-versa. The result of this inability is that our estimates of anger by sadness inter-correlations are confounded by their common context of assessment. Perhaps whether infants react to loss or goal blockage with a preponderance of anger vs. sadness is an interesting individual difference in and of itself.

Positive Affect

Individual differences in infant positivity are relatively stable during infancy and early childhood (Lemery et al., 1999; Bridgett et al., 2013; Planalp and Braungart-Rieker, 2015). In general, girls exhibit more positive affect than boys (Else-Quest et al., 2006). Early in life, however, boys and girls do not show significant differences in positive affect, although boys tend to show more behaviors related to exuberance and activity level by 5 years of age (Degnan et al., 2011). In our study, boys exhibited more positivity during the Peekaboo episode, which involves anticipation and excitement (possibly exuberance) upon seeing the mothers face. Girls, however, showed stronger stability across time in the Peekaboo episode, indicating that perhaps girls are less variable in how they respond to positively valenced stimuli. Nonetheless, our measurement of positive affect reflects the underlying stability evident in many temperamental dimensions.

Interest/Persistence

Infant interest/persistence is stable starting in the second year of life (Rothbart et al., 2000; Auerbach et al., 2004), but results regarding stability from the first year of life are mixed. Some studies find moderate relations in parent ratings of interest or orienting across ages 3 to 12 months (Matheny and Wilson, 1981) whereas others find no relation when assessment intervals are 6 months or greater (Carranza Carnicero et al., 2000). In addition, observed measures of interest tend to be uncorrelated until later in infancy (Carranza Carnicero et al., 2000). These findings are likely a consequence of the differential development of two attentional systems, an orienting system and a more voluntary system subsuming inhibition and planning (Ruff and Rothbart, 2001). Why boys show more interest/persistence during the Slides episode than girls is unclear. Attentional differences are not typically reported until later in development (Putnam et al., 2001), and parents did not report differences in duration of orienting on the IBQ.

Activity Level

Similar to previous research, boys were higher in activity level during tasks designed to elicit active responses than girls (Chaplin and Aldao, 2013). Boys may have higher reactive tendencies, evidenced by higher scores on approach or externalizing domains (exuberance, anger) as well as lower regulation in childhood (Weinberg et al., 1999; Degnan et al., 2011; Gagne and Goldsmith, 2011). In addition, we confirmed prior findings that boys were more active than girls in the laboratory (Campbell and Eaton, 1999; Else-Quest et al., 2006). Boys tend to be higher on approach based behaviors such as exuberance and activity level (Weinberg et al., 1999).

Additionally, activity level tends to be stable, such that more active infants are also more active as older children. A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies of temperament showed stability for individual differences in activity level from infancy to childhood (r = 0.28, p < 0.05; Roberts and DelVecchio, 2000), and in a study of children ages 2 to 3, parent-report and observer ratings evidenced both continuity and change across age (Saudino, 2012). We found similar stability in activity level across infancy in both IBQ and Lab-TAB assessment.

Limitations and Conclusions

Although we conclude that the Lab-TAB offers numerous advantages over subjective parental report, several limitations are also noteworthy. First, administering the full Lab-TAB battery, which we did not do in this study, is time-consuming and expensive, especially when the episodes are repeated to assess stability. Even with the 20 episodes described in the locomotor version of the Lab-TAB manual, we still only sample limited components of the infant's behavioral repertoire and thus only a limited range of affective individual differences. For instance, we do not tap the affective dimension of disgust. Also, for ethical reasons, we do not use more intense elicitors of negative affect that occasionally occur in daily life.

Another set of limitations is characteristic of most laboratory assessments in the social, affective, and even cognitive domains. Examiner differences, scorer sensitivity effects, infant state effects (hunger, tiredness), effects of the novel laboratory environment, and other subtle effects may impact results. We also examined convergent validity of the Lab-TAB episodes and dimensions with parent reported temperament, but did not address discriminant validity between temperament dimensions. Although we considered discriminant validity in the construction of Lab-TAB, we did not consider it in this paper. The issue of discriminant validity, particularly as it applies to Lab-TAB measurement of anger and sadness issue is complex. As explained above, we score anger and sadness from the same episodes, and anger and sadness are correlated due to the common element of measurement, much as questionnaire items on the IBQ that follow from the same stem are correlated.

More generally, formulations about discriminant validity often occur in the absence of theory that specifies how latent constructs should be related. We posit that temperament is affective individuality, so we must use affect theories to offer expectations about discriminant validity. For instance, popular circumplex theories of affect (e.g., Plutchik, 1980) specify that affects at opposite poles of the circumplex would be uncorrelated, but those nearby on the circumplex would be highly correlated. Other theories of affect generate different expectations for the association of components of affect and thus for expectation of what would constitute evidence of discriminant validity. Given these complications, we did not address discriminant validity comprehensively in this manuscript although we do so in more limited contexts in other papers (Goldsmith and Gagne, 2012; Clifford et al., 2015).

Despite these limitations, we provide more support and validation for the use of an infant laboratory based temperament assessment. The Lab-TAB indicated moderate relations with parent reports, consistency across time, and replicated gender differences from prior research. In addition, we provide information on the use of both episode-level vs. dimensional scoring, which allows researchers flexibility depending on their question of interest.

We conclude that employing the pre-locomotor and locomotor versions of the Lab-TAB as a behavioral assessment of temperament in infancy is feasible and fruitful for examining temperament using an individual differences approach. Researchers will vary on administration (single episodes for each temperament dimension vs. multiple episodes), coding and scoring (e.g., some will prefer to use more global scoring than we did, cf. Dyson et al., 2012). Lab-TAB can be used to examine temperament within age, longitudinally across infancy (as we did) and into childhood, and within and across temperament dimensions. In sum, the Lab-TAB is a useful tool for examining fine grained individual differences in early emerging infant temperament.

Author Contributions

EP conceived the paper, ran statistical analyses and contributed to the manuscript. CV did statistical analyses and contributed to the manuscript. JG did statistical analyses and contributed to the manuscript. HG conceived the paper and contributed to the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Data collection for this project was supported by RO1 MH050560 from the National Institute of Mental Health. Data analysis and writing of this manuscript was partly supported by T32 MH018931, P50 MH100031, P50 MH084051, and RO1 MH101504. Infrastructure support was also provided by core grants to the Waisman Center (P30 HD03352, U54 HD09025). We thank the families who participated in the study and the staff members who helped with data collection.

References

Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality. New York, NY: Holt.

Google Scholar

Anzman-Frasca, S., Liu, S., Gates, K. M., Paul, I. M., Rovine, M. J., and Birch, L. L. (2013). Infants' transitions out of a fussing/crying state are modifiable and are related to weight status. Infancy 18, 662–686. doi: 10.1111/infa.12002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anzman-Frasca, S., Stifter, C. A., Paul, I. M., and Birch, L. L. (2014). Negative temperament as a moderator of intervention effects in infancy: testing a differential susceptibility model. Prevent. Sci. 15, 643–653. doi: 10.1007/s11121-013-0408-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Auerbach, J. G., Atzaba-Poria, N., Berger, A., and Landau, R. (2004). Emerging developmental pathways to ADHD: possible path markers in early infancy. Neural Plast. 11, 29–43. doi: 10.1155/NP.2004.29

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braungart-Rieker, J. M., Hill-Soderlund, A. L., and Karrass, J. (2010). Fear and anger reactivity trajectories from 4 to 16 months: the roles of temperament, regulation, and maternal sensitivity. Dev. Psychol. 46, 791. doi: 10.1037/a0019673

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bridges, L. J., Palmer, S. A., Morales, M., Hurtado, M., and Tsai, D. (1993). Agreement between affectively based observational and parent-report measures of temperament at infant age 6 months. Infant Behav. Dev. 16, 501–506. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(93)80008-V

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bridgett, D. J., Oddi, K. B., Laake, L. M., Murdock, K. W., and Bachmann, M. N. (2013). Integrating and differentiating aspects of self-regulation: effortful control, executive functioning, and links to negative affectivity. Emotion 13, 47. doi: 10.1037/a0029536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooker, R. J., Buss, K. A., Lemery-Chalfant, K., Aksan, N., Davidson, R. J., and Goldsmith, H. H. (2013). The development of stranger fear in infancy and toddlerhood: normative development, individual differences, antecedents, and outcomes. Dev. Sci. 16, 864–878. doi: 10.1111/desc.12058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooker, R. J., Kiel, E. J., and Buss, K. A. (2016). Early social fear predicts kindergarteners' socially anxious behaviors: direct associations, moderation by inhibitory control, and differences from nonsocial fear. Emotion 16, 997–1010. doi: 10.1037/emo0000135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buss, K. A. (2011). Which fearful toddlers should we worry about? Context, fear regulation, and anxiety risk. Dev. Psychol. 47, 804. doi: 10.1037/a0023227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buss, K. A., and Goldsmith, H. H. (1998). Fear and anger regulation in infancy: effects on the temporal dynamics of affective expression. Child Dev. 69, 359–374. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06195.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, D. W., and Eaton, W. O. (1999). Sex differences in the activity level of infants. Infant Child Dev. 8, 1–17. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1522-7219(199903)8:1<1::AID-ICD186>3.0.CO;2-O

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campos, J. J., Barrett, K. C., Lamb, M. E., Goldsmith, H. H., and Stenberg, C. (1983). Socioemotional development. Handb. Child Psychol. 2, 783–915.

Carranza Carnicero, J. A., Pérez-López, J., Del Carmen, G. S., and Martínez-Fuentes, M. T. (2000). A longitudinal study of temperament in infancy: stability and convergence of measures. Eur. J. Pers. 14, 21–37. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0984(200001/02)14:1<21::AID-PER367>3.0.CO;2-A

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaplin, T. M., and Aldao, A. (2013). Gender differences in emotion expression in children: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 139, 735–765. doi: 10.1037/a0030737

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaplin, T. M., Cole, P. M., and Zahn-Waxler, C. (2005). Parental socialization of emotion expression: gender differences and relations to child adjustment. Emotion 5:80. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.80

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clifford, S., Lemery-Chalfant, K., and Goldsmith, H. H. (2015). The unique and shared genetic and environmental contributions to fear, anger, and sadness in childhood. Child Dev. 86, 1538–1556. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12394

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colombo, J. (2002). Infant attention grows up: the emergence of a developmental cognitive neuroscience perspective. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 11, 196–200. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00199

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Darwin, C. (1872). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. London: John Murray.

Google Scholar

Davidson, R. J. (1998). Affective style and affective disorders: perspectives from affective neuroscience. Cogn. Emot. 12, 307–330. doi: 10.1080/026999398379628

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Degnan, K. A., Hane, A. A., Henderson, H. A., Moas, O. L., Reeb-Sutherland, B. C., and Fox, N. A. (2011). Longitudinal stability of temperamental exuberance and social–emotional outcomes in early childhood. Dev. Psychol. 47, 765. doi: 10.1037/a0021316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dyson, M. W., Olino, T. M., Durbin, C. E., Goldsmith, H. H., Bufferd, S. J., Miller, A. R., et al. (2015). The structural and rank-order stability of temperament in young children based on a laboratory-observational measure. Psychol. Assess. 27, 1388. doi: 10.1037/pas0000104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dyson, M. W., Olino, T. M., Durbin, C. E., Goldsmith, H. H., and Klein, D. N. (2012). The structure of temperament in preschoolers: a two-stage factor analytic approach. Emotion 12, 44. doi: 10.1037/a0025023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P. (1982). “Methods for measuring facial action,” in Handbook of Methods in Nonverbal Behavior Research, eds K. R. Scherer and P. Ekman (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 45–135.

Google Scholar

Ekman, P. (1989). “The argument and evidence about universals in facial expressions of emotion,” in Handbook of Social Psychophysiology, eds H. Wagner and A. Manstead (New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons), 143–164.

Google Scholar

Else-Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., Goldsmith, H. H., and Van Hulle, C. A. (2006). Gender differences in temperament: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 132:33. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.33

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emde, R. N., Gaensbauer, T. J., and Harmon, R. J. (1975). Emotional expression in infancy; a biobehavioral study. Psychol. Issues 10, 1–200.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied Missing Data Analysis. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Escalona, S. K. (1968). The Roots of Individuality: Normal Patterns of Development in Infancy. Chicago, IL: Aldine Publishing Company.

Gagne, J. R., and Goldsmith, H. H. (2011). A longitudinal analysis of anger and inhibitory control in twins from 12 to 36 months of age. Dev. Sci. 14, 112–124. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00969.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gagne, J. R., Van Hulle, C. A., Aksan, N., Essex, M. J., and Goldsmith, H. H. (2011). Deriving childhood temperament measures from emotion-eliciting behavioral episodes: scale construction and initial validation. Psychol. Assess. 23, 337–353. doi: 10.1037/a0021746

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gagne, J. R., Vendlinski, M. K., and Goldsmith, H. H. (2009). “The genetics of childhood temperament,” in Handbook of Behavior Genetics (Springer), 251–267. Available online at: from http://link.springer.com/10.1007/978-0-387-76727-7_18

Google Scholar

Gartstein, M. A., Bridgett, D. J., Rothbart, M. K., Robertson, C., Iddins, E., Ramsay, K., et al. (2010). A latent growth examination of fear development in infancy: contributions of maternal depression and the risk for toddler anxiety. Dev. Psychol. 46, 651. doi: 10.1037/a0018898

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gartstein, M. A., and Marmion, J. (2008). Fear and positive affectivity in infancy: convergence/discrepancy between parent-report and laboratory-based indicators. Infant Behav. Dev. 31, 227–238. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gartstein, M. A., and Rothbart, M. K. (2003). Studying infant temperament via the revised infant behavior questionnaire. Infant Behav. Dev. 26, 64–86. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00169-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldsmith, H. H. (1993). “Temperament: variability in developing emotion systems,” in Handbook of Emotions, eds M. Lewis and J. Haviland (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 353–364.

Google Scholar

Goldsmith, H. H. (1994). “Parsing the emotional domain from a developmental perspective,” in The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions, eds R. J. Davidson and P. Ekman (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 68–73.

Google Scholar

Goldsmith, H. H. (1996). Studying temperament via construction of the toddler behavior assessment questionnaire. Child Dev. 67, 218–235. doi: 10.2307/1131697

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldsmith, H. H., Buss, A. H., Plomin, R., Rothbart, M. K., Thomas, A., Chess, S., et al. (1987). Roundtable: what is temperament? Four approaches. Child Dev. 58, 505–529. doi: 10.2307/1130527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldsmith, H. H., Buss, K. A., and Lemery, K. S. (1997). Toddler and childhood temperament: expanded content, stronger genetic evidence, new evidence for the importance of environment. Dev. Psychol. 33:891. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.891

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldsmith, H. H., and Campos, J. J. (1986). Fundamental issues in the study of early temperament: the denver twin temperament study. Adv. Dev. Psychol. 4, 231–283.

Goldsmith, H. H., and Gagne, J. R. (2012). “Behavioral assessment of temperament,” in Handbook of Temperament, eds M. Zentner and R. L. Shiner (New York, NY: Guilford), 209–228.

Google Scholar

Goldsmith, H. H., and Hewitt, E. C. (2003). Validity of parental report of temperament: distinctions and needed research. Infant Behav. Dev. 26, 108–111. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00172-8

CrossRef Full Text

Goldsmith, H. H., and Lemery, K. S. (2000). Linking temperamental fearfulness and anxiety symptoms: a behavior–genetic perspective. Biol. Psychiatry 48, 1199–1209. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(00)01003-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldsmith, H. H., and Rothbart, M. K. (1991). “Contemporary instruments for assessing early temperament by questionnaire and in the laboratory,” in Explorations in Temperament: International Perspectives on Theory and Measurement, eds J. Strelau and A. Angleitner (New York, NY: Springer), 249–272.

Google Scholar

Goldsmith, H. H., and Rothbart, M. K. (1996). Prelocomotor and Locomotor Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery, Lab-TAB; version 3.0. Technical Manual, Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI.

Goldsmith, H. H., and Rieser-Danner, L. (1990). “Assessing early temperament,” in Handbook of Psychological and Educational Assessment of Children, Vol. 2, Personality, Behavior, and Context, eds C. R. Reynolds and R. Kamphaus (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 345–378.

Hane, A. A., Fox, N. A., Polak-Toste, C., Ghera, M. M., and Guner, B. M. (2006). Contextual basis of maternal perceptions of infant temperament. Dev. Psychol. 42:1077. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, J., Hane, A. A., Degnan, K. A., Henderson, H. A., Xu, Q., and Fox, N. A. (2013). Anger and positive reactivity in infancy: Effects on maternal report of surgency and attention focusing in early childhood. Infancy 18, 184–201. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00113.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huebner, R. R., and Izard, C. E. (1988). Mothers' responses to infants' facial expressions of sadness, anger, and physical distress. Motiv. Emot. 12, 185–196. doi: 10.1007/BF00992173

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izard, C. E. (1972). Patterns of Emotions. New York, NY: Academic Press.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Izard, C. E., and Malatesta, C. Z. (1987). Perspectives on Emotional Development I: Differential Emotions Theory of Early Emotional Development. New York, NY: John Wiley and Sons.

Google Scholar

Kagan, J. (1997). Temperament and the reactions to unfamiliarity. Child Dev. 68, 139–143. doi: 10.2307/1131931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenrick, D. T., and Funder, D. C. (1988). Profiting from controversy: lessons from the person-situation debate. Am. Psychol. 43:23. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.43.1.23

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiel, E. J., and Buss, K. A. (2011). Prospective relations among fearful temperament, protective parenting, and social withdrawal: the role of maternal accuracy in a moderated mediation framework. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 39, 953–966. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9516-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kochanska, G. (1993). Toward a synthesis of parental socialization and child temperament in early development of conscience. Child Dev. 64, 325–347. doi: 10.2307/1131254

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kopala-Sibley, D. C., Dougherty, L. R., Dyson, M. W., Laptook, R. S., Olino, T. M., Bufferd, S. J., et al. (2017). Early childhood cortisol reactivity moderates the effects of parent–child relationship quality on the development of children's temperament in early childhood. Dev. Sci. 20:e12378. doi: 10.1111/desc.12378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Lemerise, E. A., and Dodge, K. A. (2008). “The development of anger and hostile interactions,” in Handbook of Emotions, 3rd Edn., eds M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, and L. F. Barrett (New York, NY: Guildford Press), 730–741.

Google Scholar

Lemery-Chalfant, K., Goldsmith, H. H., Schmidt, N. L., Arneson, C. L., and Van Hulle, C. A. (2006). Wisconsin Twin Panel: current directions and findings. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 9, 1030–1037. doi: 10.1375/twin.9.6.1030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemery, K. S., Goldsmith, H. H., Klinnert, M. D., and Mrazek, D. A. (1999). Developmental models of infant and childhood temperament. Dev. Psychol. 35:189. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.35.1.189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malatesta, C. Z., and Haviland, J. M. (1982). Learning display rules: the socialization of emotion expression in infancy. Child Dev. 53, 991–1003. doi: 10.2307/1129139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matheny, A. P., and Wilson, R. S. (1981). Developmental tasks and rating scales for the laboratory assessment of infant temperament. JSAS Catal. Select. Doc. Psychol. 11, 81–82.

Matheny, A. P. Jr (1980). Bayley's infant behavior record: behavioral components and twin analyses. Child Dev. 51, 1157–1167. doi: 10.2307/1129557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Morris, M. D. S., Steinberg, L., Aucoin, K. J., and Keyes, A. W. (2011). The influence of mother–child emotion regulation strategies on children's expression of anger and sadness. Dev. Psychol. 47, 213–225. doi: 10.1037/a0021021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Planalp, E. M., and Braungart-Rieker, J. M. (2015). Trajectories of regulatory behaviors in early infancy: determinants of infant self-distraction and self-comforting. Infancy 20, 129–159. doi: 10.1111/infa.12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Planalp, E. M., Van Hulle, C., Lemery-Chalfant, K., and Goldsmith, H. H. (2016). Genetic and environmental contributions to the development of positive affect in Infancy. Emotion 17, 412–420. doi: 10.1037/emo0000238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plutchik, R. (1980). “A general psychoevolutionary theory of emotion,” in Emotion: Theory, Research, and Experience: Vol. 1, Theories of Emotion, eds R. Plutchik and H. Kellerman (New York, NY: Academic Press), 3–33.

Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., and Voelker, P. (2016). Developing brain networks of attention. Curr. Opin. Pediatr. 28, 720–724. doi: 10.1097/MOP.0000000000000413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Putnam, S., Ellis, L., and Rothbart, M. K. (2001). “The structure of temperament from infancy through adolescence,” in Advances in Research on Temperament, eds A. Eliasz and A. Angleitner (Lengerich: Pabst Science Publishers), 165–182.

Roberts, B. W., and DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: a quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychol. Bull. 126:3. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of Temperament in Infancy. Child Dev. 52, 569. doi: 10.2307/1129176

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothbart, M. K., and Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Rothbart, M. K., and Bates, J. E. (2006). “Temperament,” in Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol. 3. Social, Emotionaland Personality Development, 6th Edn., eds N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, and R. M. Lerner (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons), 99–166.

Google Scholar

Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., and Hershey, K. (2000). “Stability of temperament in childhood: laboratory infant assessment to parent report at seven years,” in Temperament and Personality Development Across the Life Span, eds V. J. Molfese, D. L. Molfese, and R. R. McCrae (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 85–119.

Google Scholar

Rothbart, M. K., and Goldsmith, H. H. (1985). Three approaches to the study of infant temperament. Dev. Rev. 5, 237–260. doi: 10.1016/0273-2297(85)90012-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruff, H. A., and Rothbart, M. K. (2001). Attention in Early Development: Themes and Variations. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Saudino, K. J. (2003). The need to consider contrast effects in parent-rated temperament. Infant Behav. Dev. 26, 118–120. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00175-3

CrossRef Full Text

Saudino, K. J. (2012). Sources of continuity and change in activity level in early childhood: activity level. Child Dev. 83, 266–281. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01680.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sroufe, L. A., and Waters, E. (1976). The ontogenesis of smiling and laughter: a perspective on the organization of development in infancy. Psychol. Rev. 83:173. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.83.3.173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stenberg, C. R., and Campos, J. J. (1990). The development of anger expressions in infancy. Psychol. Biol. Approaches Emotion 247, 82.

Google Scholar

Stenberg, C. R., Campos, J. J., and Emde, R. N. (1983). The facial expression of anger in seven-month-old infants. Child Dev. 54, 178–184. doi: 10.2307/1129875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stifter, C. A., Willoughby, M. T., and Towe-Goodman, N. (2008). Agree or agree to disagree? Assessing the convergence between parents and observers on infant temperament. Infant Child Dev. 17, 407–426. doi: 10.1002/icd.584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, R. A. (1991). Emotional regulation and emotional development. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 3, 269–307. doi: 10.1007/BF01319934

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomkins, S. S. (1982). “Affect theory,” in Emotions in the Human Face, Emotions in the Human 2, ed P. Ekman (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 353–395.

Weinberg, M. K., and Tronick, E. Z. (1994). Beyond the face: an empirical study of infant affective configurations of facial, vocal, gestural, and regulatory behaviors. Child Dev. 65, 1503–1515. doi: 10.2307/1131514

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinberg, M. K., Tronick, E. Z., Cohn, J. F., and Olson, K. L. (1999). Gender differences in emotional expressivity and self-regulation during early infancy. Dev. Psychol. 35:175. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.35.1.175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: temperament, emotion, assessment, Lab-TAB, infancy

Citation: Planalp EM, Van Hulle C, Gagne JR and Goldsmith HH (2017) The Infant Version of the Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery (Lab-TAB): Measurement Properties and Implications for Concepts of Temperament. Front. Psychol. 8:846. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00846

Received: 30 January 2017; Accepted: 09 May 2017;
Published: 24 May 2017.

Edited by:

Marcel Zentner, University of Innsbruck, Austria

Reviewed by:

Liudmila Liutsko, Barcelona Institute for Global Health, Spain
Amanda R. Tarullo, Boston University, United States

Copyright © 2017 Planalp, Van Hulle, Gagne and Goldsmith. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Elizabeth M. Planalp, planalp@wisc.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.