Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 05 November 2015
Sec. Psychology of Language

Commentary: Attentional control and the self: The Self-Attention Network (SAN)

  • 1Laboratory of Experimental Psychology and Neuroscience, Institute of Cognitive Neurology, Favaloro University, Buenos Aires, Argentina
  • 2National Scientific and Technical Research Council, Buenos Aires, Argentina
  • 3Faculty of Elementary and Special Education, National University of Cuyo, Mendoza, Argentina
  • 4UDP-INECO Foundation Core on Neuroscience, Diego Portales University, Santiago, Chile
  • 5Faculty of Psychology, National University of Córdoba, Córdoba, Argentina
  • 6Universidad Autónoma del Caribe, Barranquilla, Colombia
  • 7Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Australian Research Council, Sydney, NSW, Australia

A commentary on
Attentional control and the self: The Self-Attention Network (SAN)

by Humphreys, G. W., and Sui, J. (2015). Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 1–13. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2015.1044427

The Self-Attention Network (SAN) model (Humphreys and Sui, 2015) is a recent neurocognitive model to account for self-biases in the allocation of attention. It emerges from psychological, neuropsychological, and neuroimaging evidence on three phenomena: own-name effects, own-face effects, and self-biases in associative matching. Specifically, it posits that our responses to self-related stimuli are differentially subserved by a network comprising three nodes: (i) a general-purpose top-down attentional control network which involves the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the intra-parietal sulcus; (ii) a self-representation hub located in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), and (iii) a bottom-up orientating mechanism which depends on the posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS). Accordingly, attentional shifts upon hearing our own name or seeing our own face would rely on interactions among such nodes, mimicking perceptual-saliency effects, and determining emergent behavior.

Though attractive, this proposal features two major caveats. First, the evidence for own-name effects is inconsistent and undermined by psycholinguistic confounds. Second, the node proposed to subserve self-specific information lacks neurofunctional specificity. Here we discuss both issues and advance relevant methodological recommendations.

The model resorts to own-name studies allegedly showing biases for self-related information. However, confirmatory evidence has not been consistently replicated (Yang et al., 2013), especially when own names are compared to other familiar names (e.g., Harris et al., 2004; Kawahara and Yamada, 2004; Tacikowski et al., 2011). Moreover, when they do emerge, own-name effects may be related to non-self-specific psycholinguistic confounds, such as familiarity and frequency. Familiar and frequent words are processed faster (Guttentag and Carroll, 1994) and yield distinctive electrophysiological modulations (Kutas and Federmeier, 2011). Subjectively, own names are typically more frequent and familiar than other proper and common names. Thus, it is difficult to rule out the impact of such variables on the observed effects.

Moreover, there is no neuroanatomical evidence for a dissociation between own names and other proper or familiar names. A review of patients with selective deficits in recalling people's names found no support for a specialized mechanism supporting proper-name—let alone own-name—processing (Hanley and Kay, 1998). Finally, neuroimaging and lesion studies indicate that proper-name processing mainly involves left temporal structures and, less notably, subcortical structures (Semenza, 2006). None of these regions is part of the neuroanatomical components of the SAN model, and there is no rationale for why one's own name should rely on a network separate from that specialized in processing its overarching category. At the very least, the model should specify the relationship between the regions proposed to support own-name biases and the broader networks subserving lexical processing, in general, and proper names, in particular.

The SAN model further posits that self-specific information is subserved by a putative brain region. The model includes “a self-representation hub housed in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC)” (Humphreys and Sui, 2015: 15; emphasis ours). However, this association lacks neurofunctional specificity. The vmPFC is critically engaged by any affective response shaped by conceptual information about specific outcomes (Roy et al., 2012), and by bottom-up processing of external and internal salient stimuli (Cona et al., 2015). Crucially, the vmPFC is key to assess the familiarity of others' faces (Gilboa et al., 2009) and to discriminate them as a function of their relevance (Pegors et al., 2015). Finally, when one's own name is compared with stimuli of similar personal relevance (e.g., the name of a significant other), vmPFC activity is not differentially modulated by self-biased information. So, rather than self-attention in particular, vmPFC activations seem to index increased affective meaning, relevance, or familiarity of self-faces. In brief, the vmPFC does not seem specific enough to constitute a distinct node subserving sui generis self-attention.

A second caveat concerning the role of the vmPFC has been noted by Vallesi (2015). As this author argues, the model assumes strong and mostly unidirectional excitatory connections from the vmPFC to the pSTS. However, this claim is incompatible with lesion data showing that self-bias effects decrease after damage to the former region, but increase following damage to the latter (Sui et al., 2015a). Accordingly, Vallesi (2015) posits that vmPFC activity may be modulated via inhibitory feedback connections from the pSTS, a specification that is not captured by the putative SAN model.

These caveats may be circumvented via methodological innovations. The confounds surrounding own-name research may be avoided through “new nickname” studies. Participants could be given ad hoc nicknames and be referred to by them systematically throughout an experimental session. All names would have the same frequency and familiarity at baseline (namely, zero), and they could be matched for other relevant psycholinguistic variables, such as length or phonological complexity. If an advantage for own nicknames is thus observed, claims for a self-attention bias could be more validly entertained. A conceptually similar paradigm, designed by the very proponents of the SAN model (Sui et al., 2012, 2015b), illustrates the potential usefulness of “new nickname” studies. For example, upon establishing arbitrary associations between geometric shapes and themselves or other people, participants then show reliable self-prioritization effects, independent of psycholinguistic confounds (Sui et al., 2012). This evidence supports the possible benefits of “new-nickname” studies: while these would involve a similar design, they would decrease perceptual-matching demands and more directly address biases in the specific domain of proper-name processing.

Regarding neuroanatomical concerns, functional and structural connectivity analyses would help clarify whether there is a relationship among SAN hubs and regions for selective processing of own names/faces. Moreover, the (relative) specificity for self-information in the vmPFC could be tested by comparing stimuli with similar relevance and familiarity but with different degrees of self-related information. Finally, additional neuropsychological as well as effective and functional connectivity studies could help elucidate the role of excitatory and inhibitory connections between the vmPFC and the pSTS, showing whether the latter structure modulates self-information processing in the former (Vallesi, 2015). Until these limitations are addressed, the SAN model will remain psycholinguistically and neuroanatomically underspecified.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was partially supported by grants from CONICET, CONICYT/FONDECYT Regular (1130920), FONCyT-PICT 2012-0412, 2012-1309, and the INECO Foundation.

References

Cona, G., Scarpazza, C., Sartori, G., Moscovitch, M., and Bisiacchi, P. S. (2015). Neural bases of prospective memory: a meta-analysis and the “Attention to Delayed Intention” (AtoDI) model. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 52, 21–37. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilboa, A., Alain, C., He, Y., Stuss, D. T., and Moscovitch, M. (2009). Ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions produce early functional alterations during remote memory retrieval. J. Neurosci. 29, 4871–4881. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5210-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guttentag, R. E., and Carroll, D. (1994). Identifying the basis for the word frequency effect in recognition memory. Memory 2, 255–273. doi: 10.1080/09658219408258948

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanley, J. R., and Kay, J. (1998). Proper name anomia and anomia for the names of people: functionally dissociable impairments? Cortex 34, 155–158. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70745-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, C. R., Pashler, H. E., and Coburn, N. (2004). Moray revisited: high-priority affective stimuli and visual search. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 57, 1–31. doi: 10.1080/02724980343000107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Humphreys, G. W., and Sui, J. (2015). Attentional control and the self: the Self-Attention Network (SAN). Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 1–13. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2015.1044427

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawahara, J., and Yamada, Y. (2004). Does one's name attract visual attention? Vis. cogn. 11, 997–1017. doi: 10.1080/13506280444000049a

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kutas, M., and Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 621–647. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pegors, T. K., Kable, J. W., Chatterjee, A., and Epstein, R. A. (2015). Common and unique representations in pFC for face and place attractiveness. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 959–973. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00777

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roy, M., Shohamy, D., and Wager, T. D. (2012). Ventromedial prefrontal-subcortical systems and the generation of affective meaning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 147–156. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Semenza, C. (2006). Retrieval pathways for common and proper names. Cortex 42, 884–891. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70432-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sui, J., Enock, F., Ralph, J., and Humphreys, G. W. (2015a). Dissociating hyper and hypoself biases to a core self-representation. Cortex 70, 202–212. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sui, J., He, X., and Humphreys, G. W. (2012). Perceptual effects of social salience: evidence from self-prioritization effects on perceptual matching. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 38, 1105–1117. doi: 10.1037/a0029792

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sui, J., Yankouskaya, A., and Humphreys, G. W. (2015b). Super-capacity me! Super-capacity and violations of race independence for self- but not for reward-associated stimuli. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 41, 441–452. doi: 10.1037/a0038288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tacikowski, P., Brechmann, A., Marchewka, A., Jednorog, K., Dobrowolny, M., and Nowicka, A. (2011). Is it about the self or the significance? An fMRI study of self-name recognition. Soc. Neurosci. 6, 98–107. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2010.490665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallesi, A. (2015). Connectivity between ventromedial prefrontal cortex and posterior superior temporal sulcus. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 1–2. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2015.1075487

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, H., Wang, F., Gu, N., Gao, X., and Zhao, G. (2013). The cognitive advantage for one's own name is not simply familiarity: an eye-tracking study. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 20, 1176–1180. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0426-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Self-Attention Network model, own-name effect, own-face effect, familiarity, ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC)

Citation: García AM, Huepe D, Martinez D, Morales JP, Huepe D, Hurtado E, Calvo N and Ibáñez A (2015) Commentary: Attentional control and the self: The Self-Attention Network (SAN). Front. Psychol. 6:1726. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01726

Received: 25 August 2015; Accepted: 26 October 2015;
Published: 05 November 2015.

Edited by:

Carlo Semenza, University of Padova, Italy

Reviewed by:

Giorgia Cona, University of Padova, Italy
Antonino Vallesi, University of Padova, Italy

Copyright © 2015 García, Huepe, Martinez, Morales, Huepe, Hurtado, Calvo and Ibáñez. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Agustin Ibanez, aibanez@ineco.org.ar

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.