Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 08 November 2023
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

Investigating conspiracy theories in the light of narrative persuasion

  • Cosmic Lab, Department of Philosophy, Communication and Performing Arts, Roma Tre University, Rome, Italy

1. Introduction

Conspiracy theories (CTs) are beliefs that “two or more actors have coordinated in secret to achieve an outcome, and that their conspiracy is of public interest, but not public knowledge” (Douglas and Sutton, 2023, p. 287). CTs report malicious activities, usually ascribing agency (i.e., enormous capability) to an individual or a group, and are set up in opposition to publicly accepted versions of events. In recent years, the study of CTs has become an important field of research in the psychological sciences (e.g., Swami et al., 2014; Brotherton, 2015; Douglas et al., 2017; van Prooijen and van Vugt, 2018; Georgiou et al., 2019; van Prooijen, 2022; Pilch et al., 2023; van Der Linden et al., 2023). Investigations have focused on proximate mechanisms, such as some cognitive bias (e.g., Brotherton and French, 2015; Wagner-Egger et al., 2018), and the distal evolutionary origins underlying belief in CTs (e.g., van Prooijen and van Vugt, 2018). For example, it has been shown that when people are biased toward inferring intentional explanations for ambiguous actions, they are more likely to endorse CTs (Brotherton and French, 2015), according to which some relevant events are the product of intentional (malevolent) agency rather than ascribable to impersonal forces. Evolutionary psychological models suggest that such cognitive bias is the result of a hyperactive agency detection device, i.e., a human-evolved cognitive system attributing agency to stimuli perceived as intentional, in order to discover potential threats to the individual (Barrett, 2004). From this view, “hyperactive agency detection may facilitate conspiracy thinking as a non-functional consequence” (van Prooijen and van Vugt, 2018, p. 773).

Other lines of research have focused on psychological motives, which Douglas et al. (2017) characterize as epistemic, existential, and social, that may lead people to endorse CTs. Epistemic motives include the need for causal explanations to satisfy the desire for understanding (Van Der Wal et al., 2018). Existential motives relate to compensatory satisfactions: people may be drawn to CTs when they feel unable to exert control over their environment (Goertzel, 1994; van Prooijen and Acker, 2015). Social motives relate to the desire to preserve a positive image of the self or group: CTs exalt the self and the in-group as qualified and moral but as sabotaged by powerful and malevolent others. From this view, people may adhere to CTs defensively, i.e., “to relieve the sense of self or in-group from a sense of culpability for their disadvantaged position” (Douglas et al., 2017, p. 540). Supporting this view, studies have shown that CTs are associated with individual and collective narcissism (Cichocka et al., 2016, 2022) and paranoid ideation (Darwin et al., 2011; Brotherton and Eser, 2015).1

In this article, my aim is to contribute to this field of research by highlighting the potential relevance of another factor that has not been explored systematically in the literature so far, i.e., the fact that CTs are great narratives: “[CTs] are easy ways of telling complicated stories. Officially conspiracy theorists tell one story about an event.” (Olmsted, 2011, p. 6). I suggest that insights coming from the field of narrative persuasion (NP) could fruitfully inform the study of the psychological factors accounting for the endorsement of CTs.

2. Mechanisms of narrative persuasion

In the context of persuasion, narrative has for a long time been regarded in opposition to argumentation. As pointed out by Bilandzic and Busselle (2013), this opposition can be traced back to Aristotle's distinction between logos—the domain of logic and reason—and pathos—the domain of emotion, poetry, and stories. Therefore, over time, persuasion has become synonymous with argument—the putting forth of claims and supporting evidence linked by rational or logical coherence. Conversely, narrative—a set of temporally and causally connected sequences of events, determined by the goals and motives of one or more characters, which unfold toward a conclusion (Adornetti et al., 2022, 2023) providing resolution to unanswered questions or unresolved conflicts (Hinyard and Kreuter, 2007, p. 778)—was conceived as a mere description of events with the aim of entertaining. In the last decades, this dichotomy has been challenged. It has been shown that, like argumentation, narrative is a powerful tool of persuasion as it activates some mechanisms that turn out to be particularly effective to modify or change people's beliefs and attitudes (e.g., Mazzocco et al., 2010; Shen et al., 2014; Braddock and Dillard, 2016; Gustafson et al., 2020; Ferretti, 2022; Glaser and Reisinger, 2022).

The new way of conceiving narrative in the light of persuasion has been strongly influenced by the transportation theory advanced by Gerrig (1993) and further expanded by Green and Brock (2000, 2002) with the Transportation-Imagery Model (TIM). According to the TIM, in narrative processing, the dominant experience is transportation into the story plot, i.e., a state of intense cognitive and emotional focus on the story that prevents or reduces counterarguing: “while the person is immersed in the story, he or she may be less aware of the real-world facts that contradict assertions made in the narrative” (Green and Brock, 2002, p. 703). Losing access to some real-world facts, when immersed in a narrative, people are thus more prone to accept the story world that the author has created, being less likely to counterargue what the story asserts (see also Busselle and Bilandzic, 2009). According to another important theoretical model, the Extended Elaboration Likelihood Model (Slater and Rouner, 2002), transportation (or involvement) in the narrative is not the only mechanism accounting for NP: a crucial role is also played by identification with the story's character (usually mediated by emotional processes). This mechanism leads the audience to adopt the perspective of a character and see the narrated events through the character's eyes, thus embracing their beliefs and way of thinking (e.g., de Graaf et al., 2012; Murphy et al., 2013).

Such a theoretical framework generates predictions about the specific effects of NP and about the correlations between those effects. For example, some studies comparing narratives and non-narratives found that the former are more persuasive (e.g., Murphy et al., 2013; Bullock et al., 2021). Moreover, it has been shown that the more people are transported in a narrative, the more they tend to endorse the beliefs advocated in that story (e.g., Green and Brock, 2002; Dal Cin et al., 2004; Wang and Calder, 2006). Research has also found that the more the audience identifies with a story's character, the more it tends to adhere to their belief system, taking on the character's goals and plans (de Graaf et al., 2012; Hoeken et al., 2016; Igartua and Casanova, 2016). This is especially true in advertising, in which characters extol the virtues of a product in the format of a drama. As demonstrated by Deighton et al. (1989), stories in television commercials are processed empathically; thus, viewers are less disposed to argue and tend to accept the commercial's story world.

3. Conspiracy theories and narrative persuasion

Although the narrative format of CTs is widely recognized (e.g., Brotherton, 2015), surprisingly, the relevance of NP in investigating CTs has not been acknowledged as much as it deserves. To the best of my knowledge, only one study has explicitly referred to the theoretical framework of NP to explore the endorsement of CTs (Nera et al., 2018). Other investigations studied the mere exposure to conspiracy narratives, e.g., the film JKF about the assassination of President John F. Kennedy (Butler et al., 1995), or the usefulness of narrative intervention to counterargue and reduce conspiracy beliefs (CBs) (e.g., Lazić and ŽeŽelj, 2021; Biddlestone et al., 2023). I will briefly discuss the study by Nera et al. (2018) since it is the only research conducted on the topic. My aim is to highlight some methodological limitations of this research so that future research could take these into account.

Nera et al. (2018) conducted two studies in which they exposed four groups of participants (two for each study) to an X-Files episode in which the protagonists discover a global conspiracy. In both studies,2 participants of the experimental condition filled in a questionnaire measuring the endorsement of CBs after viewing the episode; in the control condition, participants responded to the same questionnaire before the viewing. The authors hypothesized that people in the experimental condition would show greater endorsement of CBs; however, the results did not confirm this hypothesis. Moreover, an unexpected effect was found: the control group (in Study 1) reported greater accord with two CBs related to the episode than the experimental group did. According to these results, narrative does not have a persuasive effect, i.e., a role in the endorsement of CTs.

In my opinion, the results by Nera et al. (2018) are affected by some methodological weaknesses. The authors used a design similar to those of previous studies (e.g., Butler et al., 1995; Igartua and Barrios, 2012), comparing two groups exposed to the same narrative condition, with the only difference being that the experimental group answers the questionnaire about CBs after viewing the episode, while the control group does it before. Therefore, this design does not measure a possible change of beliefs about CTs in a single group of participants; it focuses on the differences between two different groups regarding their CBs. It is implicitly assumed that the two groups are homogeneous in their prior knowledge and belief systems. This is a relevant point because, as Nera et al. (2018, p. 7) recognize, an audience's prior knowledge about the topics developed in the story influences NP (e.g., Green and Brock, 2000). Only if (before the experiment) the groups are homogenous in their beliefs about the narrative's theme can it be reasonably assumed that comparing their beliefs (measured in one group after the exposure to the content and in the other before) might provide reliable data about the persuasive effect of a message. In the study by Nera et al. (2018), the prior beliefs of the groups about CTs were not investigated. The cognitive style of the groups was not explored as well (see on this point Gjoneska, 2021). Thus, it cannot be excluded that the experimental groups tended to adopt cognitive styles (e.g, reasoning strategies) decreasing conspiracy mentality more than the control groups such as, for example, analytic thinking, which has been shown to reduce belief in CTs (Swami et al., 2014). Indeed, in Study 1 (Nera et al., 2018) the control group, which answered the questionnaire before viewing the episode, “showed slightly greater agreement with two conspiracy beliefs associated with the episode than did the experimental group” (p. 5). This result opens to the possibility that, at the start, the control group was more conspiratorial than the experimental group. It cannot be excluded that this affected the results showing that narrative does not have a persuasive effect. Therefore, it is important, when adopting this methodology, to control for these variables and other aspects linked to topics developed in the story.

Another design that could be employed to evaluate CTs in the light of NP might focus on evaluating a possible change in participants' belief systems. To this aim, CBs of both the experimental and control groups should be measured twice: at T0 before exposure to a conspiracy narrative (the experimental group) and a non-conspiracy narrative (the control group) and at T1 after the exposure. Provided that the two groups are homogeneous in their prior beliefs in CTs, if a difference in beliefs can be observed at T1 compared to T0 only in the experimental group, it can reasonably be assumed that this change occurred because of the exposure to the conspiracy narrative. Several studies on narrative persuasion have employed this design, with some investigations finding a persuasive effect of the stories, i.e., a change in participants' beliefs (e.g., Gustafson et al., 2020) before and after the exposure to a narrative message. This point is crucial. Since persuasion is an act to “modify/change the beliefs and attitudes of other people in order to make the recipient act in a certain way” (Ferretti and Adornetti, 2021, p. 2), in my opinion, a design of this kind, allowing the researchers to investigate a possible change in the participants' beliefs system (more directly than the design used by Nera et al., 2018), could provide useful tools to investigate CTs in the light of NP.

Author contributions

IA: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Open access funding was provided by the Department of Philosophy, Communication and Performing Arts, Roma Tre University.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^Political, demographic, and contextual factors involved in the endorsement of CTs have also been investigated (e.g., Claassen and Ensley, 2016; Freeman and Bentall, 2017).

2. ^The designs of the two studies are quite similar; some differences relate to the number of variables, which was higher in Study 2, and the duration of the episode, which was longer in Study 1.

References

Adornetti, I., Chiera, A., Altavilla, D., Deriu, V., Lecci, C. M., Marini, A., et al. (2022). How do we comprehend linguistic and visual narratives? A study in children with typical development. Lang. Sci. 93, 101500. doi: 10.1016/j.langsci.2022.101500

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adornetti, I., Chiera, A., Deriu, V., Altavilla, D., and Ferretti, F. (2023). Comprehending stories in pantomime. A pilot study with typically developing children and its implications for the narrative origin of language. Lang. Commun. 93, 155–171. doi: 10.1016/j.langcom.2023.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, J. L. (2004). Why Would Anyone Believe in God? Lanham: AltaMira Press.

Google Scholar

Biddlestone, M., Roozenbeek, J., and van der Linden, S. (2023). Once (but not twice) upon a time: narrative inoculation against conjunction errors indirectly reduces conspiracy beliefs and improves truth discernment. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 37, 304–318. doi: 10.1002/acp.4025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bilandzic, H., and Busselle, R. (2013). “Narrative persuasion,” in The Sage Handbook of Persuasion: Developments in Theory and Practice. 2nd edition, ed. J.P. Dillard and L. Shen (Thousand Oaks: Sage), 200–219. doi: 10.4135/9781452218410.n13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braddock, K., and Dillard, J. P. (2016). Meta-analytic evidence for the persuasive effect of narratives on beliefs, attitudes, intentions, and behaviors. Commun. Monogr. 83, 446–467. doi: 10.1080/03637751.2015.1128555

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brotherton, R. (2015). Suspicious Minds: Why We Believe Conspiracy Theories. New York, NY: Bloomsbury Sigma. doi: 10.5040/9781472944528

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brotherton, R., and Eser, S. (2015). Bored to fears: boredom proneness, paranoia, and conspiracy theories. Pers. Ind. Differ. 80, 1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.02.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brotherton, R., and French, C. C. (2015). Intention seekers: Conspiracist ideation and biased attributions of intentionality. PLoS ONE 10, e0124125. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0124125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bullock, O. M., Shulman, H. C., and Huskey, R. (2021). Narratives are persuasive because they are easier to understand: examining processing fluency as a mechanism of narrative persuasion. Front. Commun. 6, 719615. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2021.719615

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Busselle, R., and Bilandzic, H. (2009). Measuring narrative engagement. Media Psychol. 12, 321–347. doi: 10.1080/15213260903287259

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, L. D., Koopman, C., and Zimbardo, P. (1995). The psychological impact of viewing the film “JFK”: emotions, beliefs and political behavioral intentions. Polit. Psychol. 16, 237–257. doi: 10.2307/3791831

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cichocka, A., Marchlewska, M., and Biddlestone, M. (2022). Why do narcissists find conspiracy theories so appealing? Curr. Opin. Psychol. 47, 101386. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2022.101386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cichocka, A., Marchlewska, M., Golec de Zavala, A., and Olechowski, M. (2016). “They will not control us”: Ingroup positivity and belief in intergroup conspiracies. B. J. Psychol. 107, 556–576. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Claassen, R. L., and Ensley, M. J. (2016). Motivated reasoning and yard-sign-stealing partisans: Mine is a likable rogue, yours is a degenerate criminal. Polit. Behav. 38, 317–335. doi: 10.1007/s11109-015-9313-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dal Cin, S., Zanna, M. P., and Fong, G. T. (2004). “Narrative persuasion and overcoming resistance,” in Resistance and persuasion, ed. E.S. Knowles and J.A. Linn (New York: Psychology Press), 175–191. doi: 10.1037/e633872013-232

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Darwin, H., Neave, N., and Holmes, J. (2011). Belief in conspiracy theories. The role of paranormal belief, paranoid ideation and schizotypy. Pers. Ind. Differ. 50, 1289–1293. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.02.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Graaf, A., Hoeken, H., Sanders, J., and Beentjes, J. W. (2012). Identification as a mechanism of narrative persuasion. Commun. Res. 39, 802–823. doi: 10.1177/0093650211408594

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deighton, J., Romer, D., and McQueen, J. (1989). Using drama to persuade. J. Consum. Res. 16, 335–343. doi: 10.1086/209219

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Douglas, K. M., and Sutton, R. M. (2023). What are conspiracy theories? A definitional approach to their correlates, consequences, and communication. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 74, 271–298. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-032420-031329

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., and Cichocka, A. (2017). The psychology of conspiracy theories. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 26, 538–542. doi: 10.1177/0963721417718261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferretti, F. (2022). Narrative Persuasion. A Cognitive Perspective on Language Evolution (Vol. 7). Cham: Springer Nature. doi: 10.1007/978-3-031-09206-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferretti, F., and Adornetti, I. (2021). Persuasive conversation as a new form of communication in Homo sapiens. Philos. T. R. Soc. B. 376, 20200196. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2020.0196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, D., and Bentall, R. P. (2017). The concomitants of conspiracy concerns. Soc. Pysch. Psych. Epid. 52, 595–604. doi: 10.1007/s00127-017-1354-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Georgiou, N., Delfabbro, P., and Balzan, R. (2019). Conspiracy beliefs in the general population: the importance of psychopathology, cognitive style and educational attainment. Pers. Ind. Differ. 151, 109521. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.109521

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerrig, R. J. (1993). Experiencing Narrative Worlds: On the Psychological Activities of Reading. New Haven: Yale University Press. doi: 10.12987/9780300159240

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gjoneska, B. (2021). Conspiratorial beliefs and cognitive styles: an integrated look on analytic thinking, critical thinking, and scientific reasoning in relation to (dis)trust in conspiracy theories. Front. Psychol. 12, 736838. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.736838

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glaser, M., and Reisinger, H. (2022). Don't lose your product in story translation: HOW product–story link in narrative advertisements increases persuasion. J. Adver. 51, 188–205. doi: 10.1080/00913367.2021.1973623

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goertzel, T. (1994). Belief in conspiracy theories. Polit. Psychol. 15, 731–742. doi: 10.2307/3791630

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, M. C., and Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public narratives. J. Pers. Soci. Psychol. 79, 701. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.5.701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, M. C., and Brock, T. C. (2002). “In the mind's eye: Transportation-imagery model of narrative persuasion,” in Narrative Impact: Social and Cognitive Foundations, ed. M. C. Green, J. J. Strange, and T. C. Brock (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum), 315–341. doi: 10.4324/9781410606648

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gustafson, A., Ballew, M. T., Goldberg, M. H., Cutler, M. J., Rosenthal, S. A., and Leiserowitz, A. (2020). Personal stories can shift climate change beliefs and risk perceptions: the mediating role of emotion. Commun. Rep. 33, 121–135. doi: 10.1080/08934215.2020.1799049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinyard, L. J., and Kreuter, M. W. (2007). Using narrative communication as a tool for health behavior change: a conceptual, theoretical, and empirical overview. Health Educ. Behav. 34, 777–792. doi: 10.1177/1090198106291963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoeken, H., Kolthoff, M., and Sanders, J. (2016). Story perspective and character similarity as drivers of identification and narrative persuasion. Human Commun. Res. 42, 292–311. doi: 10.1111/hcre.12076

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Igartua, J. J., and Barrios, I. (2012). Changing real-world beliefs with controversial movies: processes and mechanisms of narrative persuasion. J. Commun. 62, 514–531. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2012.01640.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Igartua, J. J., and Casanova, J. V. (2016). Identification with characters, elaboration, and counterarguing in entertainment-education interventions through audiovisual fiction. J. Health Commun. 21, 293–300. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2015.1064494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazić, A., and ŽeŽelj, I. (2021). A systematic review of narrative interventions: Lessons for countering anti-vaccination conspiracy theories and misinformation. Public Underst. Sci. 30, 644–670. doi: 10.1177/09636625211011881

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazzocco, P. J., Green, M. C., Sasota, J. A., and Jones, N. W. (2010). This story is not for everyone: transportability and narrative persuasion. Soci. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 1, 361–368. doi: 10.1177/1948550610376600

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, S. T., Frank, L. B., Chatterjee, J. S., and Baezconde-Garbanati, L. (2013). Narrative versus nonnarrative: the role of identification, transportation, and emotion in reducing health disparities. J. Commun. 63, 116–137. doi: 10.1111/jcom.12007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nera, K., Pantazi, M., and Klein, O. (2018). “These are just stories, mulder”: exposure to conspiracist fiction does not produce narrative persuasion. Front. Psychol. 9, 684. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olmsted, K. (2011). Real Enemies: Conspiracy Theories and American Democracy, World War I to 9/11. New York: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Pilch, I., Turska-Kawa, A., Wardawy, P., Olszanecka-Marmola, A., and Smołkowska-Jedo, W. (2023). Contemporary trends in psychological research on conspiracy beliefs: a systematic review. Front. Psychol. 14, 1075779. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1075779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, F., Ahern, L., and Baker, M. (2014). Stories that count: influence of news narratives on issue attitudes. J. Mass. Comm. Q. 91, 98–117. doi: 10.1177/1077699013514414

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slater, M. D., and Rouner, D. (2002). Entertainment-education and elaborationlikelihood: Understanding the processing of narrative persuasion. Commun. Theor. 12, 173–191. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2885.2002.tb00265.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swami, V., Voracek, M., Stieger, S., Tran, U. S., and Furnham, A. (2014). Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories. Cognitive 133, 572–585. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Der Linden, S., Thompson, B. S., and Roozenbeek, J. (2023). Editorial - The truth is out there: The psychology of conspiracy theories and how to counter them. App. Cog. Psychol. 37, 252–255. doi: 10.1002/acp.4054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Der Wal, R. C., Sutton, R. M., Lange, J., and Braga, J. P. (2018). Suspicious binds: conspiracy thinking and tenuous perceptions of causal connections between co-occurring and spuriously correlated events. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 48, 970–989. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Prooijen, J. W. (2022). Psychological benefits of believing conspiracy theories. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 47, 101352. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2022.101352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Prooijen, J. W., and Acker, M. (2015). The influence of control on belief in conspiracy theories: Conceptual and applied extensions. App. Cog. Psychol. 29, 753–761. doi: 10.1002/acp.3161

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Prooijen, J. W., and van Vugt, M. (2018). Conspiracy theories: evolved functions and psychological mechanisms. Perspect. psychol. sci. 13, 770–788. doi: 10.1177/1745691618774270

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner-Egger, P., Delouvée, S., Gauvrit, N., and Dieguez, S. (2018). Creationism and conspiracism share a common teleological bias. Curr. Biol. 28, R867–R868. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.06.072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., and Calder, B. J. (2006). Media transportation and advertising. J. Consum. Res. 33, 151–162. doi: 10.1086/506296

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: conspiracy beliefs, conspiracy theories, narrative, narrative persuasion, transportation

Citation: Adornetti I (2023) Investigating conspiracy theories in the light of narrative persuasion. Front. Psychol. 14:1288125. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1288125

Received: 03 September 2023; Accepted: 24 October 2023;
Published: 08 November 2023.

Edited by:

Sharon Coen, University of Salford, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Dan Romer, University of Pennsylvania, United States

Copyright © 2023 Adornetti. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ines Adornetti, ines.adornetti@uniroma3.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.