Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 25 March 2021
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Children's Competencies Development in the Home Learning Environment View all 24 articles

The Home Learning Environment in the Digital Age—Associations Between Self-Reported “Analog” and “Digital” Home Learning Environment and Children’s Socio-Emotional and Academic Outcomes

  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Bamberg, Bamberg, Germany
  • 2Department of Social Monitoring and Methodology, German Youth Institute, Munich, Germany
  • 3Department of Psychology, LMU Munich, Munich, Germany

We analyzed the association between the analog and the digital home learning environment (HLE) in toddlers’ and preschoolers’ homes, and whether both aspects are associated with children’s social and academic competencies. Here, we used data of the national representative sample of Growing up in Germany II, which includes 4,914 children aged 0–5 years. The HLE was assessed via parental survey that included items on the analog HLE (e.g., playing word games, reading, and counting) and items on the digital HLE (e.g., using apps or playing with apps). Children’s socio-emotional, practical life skills, and academic competencies were assessed via standardized parental ratings. Our results indicate that there are two dimensions of the HLE, an analog and a digital, that are slightly positively associated, especially in the toddler age group. For toddlers, only analog HLE activities were associated with better socio-emotional outcomes and practical life skills. However, interaction effects indicate that toddlers with less frequent analog HLE activities showed better socio-emotional skills in households with more frequent digital activities. For preschoolers, digital HLE activities were associated with weaker socio-emotional skills but higher academic skills, although the analog HLE shows higher effect sizes for the academic outcomes. Our study points out that analog and digital HLE activities seem to be partly associated, but not interchangeable. Further, they seem to be important variables that can explain individual differences in young children’s socio-emotional, practical life, and academic competencies. However, digital media usage at home may also have negative effects on children’s social–emotional competencies. This association needs to be investigated further.

Introduction

It is becoming increasingly evident that the nature of activities in the home learning environment (HLE) in the digital age of the 21st century is rapidly changing in terms of the resources available and the ways in which these resources are used in different contexts (Marsh et al., 2005). Digital media are commonplace nowadays in families, and European children grow up in media-rich homes (Chaudron et al., 2015). As children’s immediate caregivers usually interact with digital media daily, children consider digital devices as very important (Wirth et al., 2020b). Toddlers and preschoolers learn by observing their parents and by interacting with older siblings, and from an early age onward, they are in contact with a wide range of digital tools daily and imitate older family members’ usage (Wong, 2015).

On average, many 3- to 5-year-olds use digital technologies more than 30 min on weekdays and even longer during weekends and thus use computer-based and internet-based digital technologies at home on a regular basis (Palaiologou, 2016). Further, about a third of the children aged 0–3 years already participate in computer- and internet-based activities at home, regularly (Palaiologou, 2016). Given that children use digital media from an early age onwards, these tools can be utilized to support children’s competencies development.

The social context is crucial for learning, and this also applies to interactions with digital media (Buckingham, 2007). Given the changes that have taken place in our digital environment, concepts that have been developed to describe the early years HLE may need reconsideration. In this paper, we therefore investigate the association between the analog and the digital HLE and its associations with children’s social and academic outcomes from the first year of life onwards.

The Concept of the “Analog” HLE and Its Effects on Child Outcomes

Children’s HLEs are typically described by the access to books, the frequency of reading to the children, and the availability of learning-oriented materials and toys. Further, parent–child interactions during a variety of learning opportunities within and out of the home, such as singing songs to the child, rhyming, and visiting cultural places (e.g., Bradley and Caldwell, 1984; Melhuish et al., 2008), and the quality of interactions during play or shared reading are considered important aspects of the HLE (e.g., Son and Morrison, 2010; Linberg, 2018; Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2019; Lehrl et al., 2020a). Although many research studies currently conceptualize the HLE domain-specifically according to the home literacy and/or numeracy model into aspects that capture formal and informal stimulation of language, literacy, and mathematics (e.g., Sénéchal and LeFevre, 2002; Manolitsis et al., 2013; Skwarchuk et al., 2014; Niklas et al., 2016; Lehrl et al., 2020a), there are also numerous studies that combine the different facets of the HLE into one indicator to capture the overall stimulation of the HLE (e.g., Melhuish et al., 2008; Son and Morrison, 2010; Niklas and Schneider, 2017; Kuger et al., 2019).

For instance, within the Effective Preschool, Primary and Secondary Education Project (EPPSE 3–16), an indicator of the early years HLE was developed that combined the frequency of seven educational activities, such as the frequency of shared book reading, visits to the library, playing games with numbers, teaching the child the alphabet, playing with letters, and teaching the child songs or nursery rhymes (Melhuish et al., 2008). This measure predicted preschooler’s literacy and numeracy outcomes, reading, and mathematics 2 years later (Melhuish et al., 2008), as well as second grader’s school grades (Bywater et al., 2015). Similarly, Son and Morrison (2010) found positive associations between the quality of the home environment as measured by a global indicator of the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment Inventory (HOME, Bradley and Caldwell, 1984) and child outcomes including general cognitive ability and language. Furthermore, Niklas and Schneider (2017) found positive links between a kindergarten HLE-Measure comprising similar activities as in the EPPSE–HLE Index (Melhuish et al., 2008) and children’s literacy and math concurrent and grade 4 outcomes in a German sample. Consequently, when predicting various child outcomes, a broad HLE-Measure might serve as an economic, readily assessable alternative. This assumption is supported by findings of absent domain-specific effects of specific HLE measures on specific developmental domains when contrasted to each other (for an overview see, e.g., Lehrl et al., 2020a).

In addition to the well-documented positive effects of the HLE on language and academic outcomes, there is also evidence that HLE effects are not limited to these domains but are also important for children’s socio-emotional and self-regulation skills (Huntsinger et al., 2016; Rose et al., 2018; Wirth et al., 2019). In a longitudinal study, Rose et al. (2018) investigated the predictive role of the early HLE on children’s cooperative behavior, physical aggression, and emotional self-regulation at age 8, mainly mediated through early language competencies (see Wirth et al., 2019 for similar results). Further, intervention studies showed that enhancing the quality of the HLE also impacts on very young (i.e., 12 months old; O’Farrelly et al., 2018) and older (i.e., 4 years old; Bierman et al., 2015) children’s socio-emotional competencies.

To sum up, an extensive body of research has shown that the HLE is positively associated with children’s language, literacy, math, and socio-emotional skills development in early childhood (Anders et al., 2012; Niklas et al., 2018; Rose et al., 2018; Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2019; Lehrl et al., 2020b) and beyond (e.g., Niklas and Schneider, 2017; Rose et al., 2018; Lehrl et al., 2020a). However, against the background of the increasing use of digital media within the home, we are in need of research investigating how such “analog” HLEs are associated with “digital” HLEs and how both concepts relate to child outcomes in different age groups and to various developmental domains.

The Concept of “Digital” HLE and Its Effects on Child Outcomes

Similar to the “analog” HLE, children’s “digital” HLEs may be described by the access and the frequency of usage as well as the quality of assistance/support while interacting with digital tools in the family context. This includes the access to and frequency of using, for instance, electronic toys and touchscreen devices and the parental stimulation within such contexts. In addition to e-books, digital game-based learning, which uses the entertaining power of digital games, serves an educational purpose, such as teaching math or language (All et al., 2016). An explosion in available e-books and apps has been noted over the last couple of years, especially for young children, and the majority of top-selling paid apps in 2011 were targeted for young children (Judge et al., 2015).

Although there is widespread concern that time spent with screen media replaces more traditional forms of learning (e.g., Cristia and Seidl, 2015), other researchers point out that digital technologies should be viewed as being complimentary to other resources, rather than alternatives or in competition with traditional modalities (Yelland, 2018). Through employing animated images and sound effects, digital technologies provide new and interesting experiences to the child that might motivate children more than analog tools to participate in learning opportunities (Hirsh-Pasek et al., 2015). Clearly, the HLE is an important space where digital literacy can be both employed and cultivated, and thus children’s learning and development can be supported (Meyers et al., 2013).

Actually, young children’s usage of touchscreen tablets is positively associated with emergent literacy, print awareness, print knowledge, and sound knowledge (Neumann, 2016). Further, digital media can be adapted more easily to match children’s needs and interests concerning content selection and text layout (Biancarosa and Griffiths, 2012). Consequently, in many countries, a higher number of digital devices in households coincide with better reading skills in children (Mullis and Martin, 2017). According to these findings, a digital HLE offers new possibilities to support children’s literacy development and reflects the current convergence of literacy and multimedia skills (Wirth et al., 2020a). In addition, Korat and Shneor (2019) showed that joint mother–child e-book reading compared with independent e-book reading is more effective for children’s receptive and expressive word learning. Consequently, it is preferable for young children not to use digital media alone or only passively as digital media cannot act as a substitute for human interaction (see also Hirsh-Pasek et al., 2015). However, in Germany, where this study was conducted, parents in only every second household supervise their (preschool-aged) children’s use of digital media (Marci-Boehncke et al., 2012).

Similarly, research has shown that digital learning tools can support the development of children’s numeracy competencies when used together with parents. For instance, Berkowitz et al. (2015) used an iPad app to deliver short numerical story problems to first graders and their parents. Compared with a reading control group, children’s mathematical achievement increased significantly.

As the quality of the analog HLE is associated with children’s socio-emotional competencies (e.g., O’Farrelly et al., 2018; Wirth et al., 2019), it is to be expected that the digital learning environment in families should also influence these competencies. However, research evidence regarding this point is mixed, and thus further studies and analyses are needed. In general, reviews suggest that screen time (TV, computer use, and video game) is not or negatively associated with children’s social skills. For instance, Ogelman et al. (2016) analyzed the association between 162 5- and 6-year-old children’s screen time and their social skills, which were rated by teachers. Their results revealed that children’s digital media usage duration had no effect on social skills.

Gómez et al. (2013) investigated the effect of collaborative learning on a single display computer on the social skills of 268 5- and 6-year-old children in 10 classrooms in a quasi-experimental design. The control group followed the collaborative planned activities based on the national kindergarten curriculum in an analog way. In the experimental group, children engaged in collaborative activities in a computer classroom twice each week for a period of 4 months. The activities included exchange, sort, and roleplay applications. The control and experimental groups did not differ concerning the content of activities. The experimental group showed significantly greater scores on social skills than the control group (d = 0.51). Some further conclusions can be drawn from Radesky et al. (2016) who examined how parents use digital media to calm difficult infants/toddlers. Toddlers rated as having social–emotional difficulties were more likely to be exposed to digital media to calm down when upset than their peers without social–emotional difficulties.

To sum up, there is some evidence that digitally supported learning with very specific high-quality digital media can support children’s learning and development (e.g., Gómez et al., 2013; Berkowitz et al., 2015; Neumann, 2016). However, there is a lack of research evidence concerning the importance of the broader defined digital HLE, i.e., access and frequency of usage of digital media, for various child outcomes, and its conjunction with the analog HLE in this context.

The Present Study

With the widespread use of tablets and smartphones, children have increasing opportunities for interacting and learning via electronic devices. Unfortunately, research has not kept up with the speed of the spread of digital media-based interactions during childhood. Although research on preschool-aged children’s digital media use is increasing to some extent, studies of infant and toddlers’ digital media use are still rare (Chaudron et al., 2015). Consequently, research is needed to find out whether early shared use of digital media might harm or foster children’s development across various domains and whether digital media interactions replace traditional interactions between parents and children that have been effective in supporting children’s learning in the past century.

The present study investigates the following:

(1) The frequency of shared digital media activities, also referred to as digital HLE, in the home from an early age on, to discover the prevalence of shared digital media use in the home,

(2) How these aspects of digital HLE are associated with the analog HLE, to investigate a possible shift from analog to digital home learning environmental activities,

(3) How digital HLE and analog HLE are associated with concurrent child academic and socio-emotional outcomes, and

(4) Whether possible beneficial or harmful effects of early digital HLE are moderated by analog HLE or children’s language/practical life skills.

We assume that the analog and the digital HLE can be differentiated in two independent facets that are only slightly correlated and thus represent two distinct facets of the HLE. We furthermore assume that the frequency of digital HLE increases with age. We expect the effects of the analog and digital HLEs to be more pronounced regarding academic skills, whereas the digital HLE might have a stronger impact on socio-emotional skills, as children could be more confronted with exercises, e.g., in literacy apps.

Materials and Methods

Sample

We use data of the national representative sample of Growing up in Germany (AID:A II; Bien et al., 2015). The AID:A II study was carried out between 2013 and 2015 and assessed information on over 20.000 persons up to the age of 32 years (N = 22.424). The sample includes persons who had already taken part in the AID:A I study, a representative register-based survey, which was conducted in 2009, and a register-based refreshment sample (response rate: 34.2%). For our analyses, we used data of children aged 0 to 5 years (before school enrollment) and split them into two age-dependent samples, as we were a) interested in the differences between the very early (toddler) and the later HLE (preschool) of young children, and as b) children’s competencies and skills were assessed age-specifically. Sample 1, the “toddler sample,” includes all children at the age of 11–46 months (n = 2,637), and sample 2, the “preschool sample”, includes all children at the age from 47 to 71 months (n = 1,399).

Measures

Indicators of the HLE: Analog and Digital Activities

Information on analog as well as digital media activities in the HLE was derived from an interview in which parents indicated the frequency of joint activities of the child and the parent or other persons in the household on a six-point scale, ranging from (1) never to (6) daily. The “analog HLE activities” indicator includes the mean of 11 items (e.g., reading to the child, counting, playing with alphabet toys, attending cultural activities, singing; αtoddler = 0.70, αpreschool = 0.67). “Digital HLE activities” indicator was assessed with three items by asking the parents about the frequency of joint digital media-related activities (i.e., looking at/playing with apps, going online, doing something with the computer; αtoddler = 0.67, αpreschool = 0.71). These three items capture a very general assessment of the shared use of digital media in the home environment.

Child Functioning

Children’s competencies and skills were assessed age-specifically for the toddler and preschool age groups.

In the toddler age group, socio-emotional as well as life skills were assessed using a selection of age-specific items derived from the monitoring of child development within the health screening of pediatricians (Petermann and Macha, 2003). Here, parents were asked to report whether the described behavior is true for the child (0 = no,1 = yes). These values were classified into categories, which were generated age-specifically within a 2-month interval: (2) maximum to smaller than one standard deviation from the mean, (1) one to under two standard deviation from the mean, and (0) two or more standard deviations from the mean. An index of practical life skills was summed up across eight items, and another set of eight items make up an index for socio-emotional development. Practical life skills for children at the age of 10 months included items such as pointing to an object in order to get the parent’s attention or removing barriers in order to reach an object or speaking double-sounds (such as baba, dada). Socio-emotional development at the age of 24 months included aspects such as displaying signs of joy when another child appears, if the child responds to a calmly spoken “no,” and if the child can be quickly calmed in everyday irritations.

In the group of preschool children, socio-emotional as well as domain-specific skills were also assessed via parent report. Socio-emotional skills were captured using the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; Klasen et al., 2003).

Parents reported on five items about the children’s prosocial behavior, e.g., if the child is considerate of other people’s feelings or shares readily with other children (treats, toys, pencils, etc.), ranging from (0) not true to (2) certainly true. All five items were summed up (αpreschool = 0.62). Additionally, the four subscales on total difficulties, including 20 items1 covering emotional problems, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer problems, were summed up to build another index. Items referred to whether the child often seems to be worried; often has tantrums or hot tempers; is restless, overactive, and fidgety; is rather solitary; or tends to play alone were captured with the same procedure (αpreschool = 0.74).

Domain-specific skills in the age group of the 3–5-year-olds were assessed using items of the TIMMS/IGLU 2011 study (Wendt et al., 2016), which capture language as well as math skills. Parents were asked to indicate their child’s level of ability on a four-point scale, ranging from (1) not at all to (4) very good. Language skills comprise six items, such as recognizing letters, reading some words, reading sentences, writing letters, and writing some words (αpreschool = 0.78), whereas math skills capture abilities, such as counting, recognizing numbers from 0 to 10, simple summation, and simple subtraction within five items (αpreschool = 0.80).

Indicators of Child and Family Background

As indicators for socio-economic background of the family, we included the number of siblings living in the household, the weighted household income (OECD, 2013), the highest socio-economic status, measured via the International Socio-Economic Index of Occupational Status (HISEI; Ganzeboom et al., 1992), as well as the highest educational level in the household. Here, we used the CASMIN-classification (Comparative Analysis of Social Mobility in Industrial Nations; Müller et al., 1989), which contains information on school and vocational training certificates, ranging from 1 to 8 with (1) indicating general elementary education, (4) secondary school leaving certificate with vocational training, and (8) higher tertiary education (university degree). As child background information, children’s age (in months) and their gender (0 = girl, 1 = boy) were considered. Additionally, we controlled for “positive parenting” as an omnibus indicator for the socio-emotional support within the family. Parents answered four questions, such as if they praise their child or comfort their child if he/she is sad. For parents of children aged 24 months, two additional items were presented, such as “I talk to my child about what he/she has experienced” (α = 0.58).

Tendency of Agreement

Acquiescence has long been known to influence survey data. A particular thread to the data’s validity is differential effects depending on the respondent’s education, age, or gender (O’Muircheartaigh et al., 1999). All data used for this study were collected in a phone-based survey (CATI) with parents. In order to prevent our analyses to be corroborated by such a response bias, all multivariate analyses include a correction factor “tendency to agree.” For this, we built a ratio “tendency to agree” by dividing the number of agreeing responses across all six-point rating scales throughout the full questionnaire by the number of all valid responses. Descriptive statistics for all variables included are provided in Tables 1, 2.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the toddler age group.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics for the preschool age group.

Analytic Strategy

First, to examine whether the analog and the digital HLE can be differentiated into two separate facets of the HLE, we conducted confirmatory factor analyses and used chi-square difference testing to decide on model selection. Second, we explored (a) the frequency of shared digital media use by investigating the proportion of families sharing digital media at least seldom with their child, as an indicator of having overall shared contact with digital devices at several ages (the age groups were split to get a better impression of the increase across ages within the toddler and preschool age groups) and (b) the relation between analog and digital HLEs using bivariate correlations. Third, the associations between digital and analog HLEs with child outcomes were analyzed using multiple regression models for both age groups. Here, we included the tendency of agreement to capture the variance between HLE and child outcomes that can be attributed to a general tendency of agreement to different items. Note that we did not control for child age in the toddler age group years as the used instrument (Petermann and Macha, 2003) comprises age-specific items and thus age-specific practical life skills as well as socio-emotional development (2-month intervals) were used in the analyses.

Fourth, to analyze whether the analog HLE moderates the effects of the digital HLE, we included interaction terms for all regression models. Furthermore, for the models with the socio-emotional skills as outcomes, we analyzed whether practical life skills (toddlers) or language skills (preschoolers) moderate the effects of the digital HLE by including interaction terms into the regression models. To visualize the interaction effects, we plotted predictive margins in which we only visualized results for low (mean −1 SD), medium (mean), and high (mean +1 SD) value of the respective scale, for visual clarity. Interaction effects were plotted when p was smaller than 0.10. All models were run using Stata 15.

Results

Digital and Analog HLE: Frequencies and Internal Associations

We first explored whether digital and analog HLEs can be differentiated into two separate dimensions. We ran confirmatory factor analyses to decide whether a one-factor solution or the proposed two factor solution would fit the data better. Results of chi2 difference testing were in favor of the two-factor model (Δχ2 = 2,380.734, df = 1, p < 0.001). In addition, the correlational analyses (Tables 3, 4) showed only small correlations between the two dimensions.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Bivariate correlations of the study variables in the toddler age group.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Bivariate correlations of the study variables in the preschooler age group.

As depicted in Table 5, shared digital media activities increased across age, in particular concerning the shared use of a computer. However, about 15% of children in the sample had experiences with sharing digital media already in their toddler years.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Proportion of shared digital media activities in the home (at least seldom).

Relations Between HLE and Child Outcomes and Potential Moderators

As can be seen in Table 6, toddler’s socio-emotional skills were associated with analog HLE (β = 0.23, p < 0.001), but not with digital HLE (β = −0.03, ns) (Model 1). A similar pattern is visible for the association of practical life skills (Model 4, analog HLE: β = 0.24, p < 0.001; digital HLE: β = 0.03, p < 0.10). In addition, the interaction between digital and analog HLEs was significant for socio-emotional skills (Model 2, β = −0.34, p < 0.01). As depicted in Figure 1, digital HLE moderates the effect of analog HLE in that way, that socio-emotional skills increase for children with low analog HLE when being involved in more digital HLE. No other interaction terms were significant (Models 3 and 5).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Multivariate regressions: associations between toddler’s socio-emotional outcomes, practical life skills, and digital and analog HLEs.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Moderating effects of analog HLE on the association between digital HLE and socioemotional development (toddler age group).

Table 7 shows the results for the preschoolers, that differ slightly: While the analog HLE is positively associated with prosocial behavior (Model 6, β = 0.06, p < 0.05) but not with total difficulties (Model 9, β = −0.02, ns), a greater experience of digital HLE goes along with less socio-emotional skills (Models 6 and 9, prosocial behavior: β = −0.06, p < 0.001; total difficulties: β = 0.07, p < 0.01). No significant interaction terms were found (Models 7, 8, 10, and 11). For parent-rated academic skills (see Table 8), we found positive associations of both, the analog and the digital HLE with language (Model 12) and math skills (Model 14). Here, the effects were more pronounced for the analog HLE. For language skills, we found a small moderator effect (Model 13). For children with high analog HLEs, the digital HLE makes no difference for their language skills. However, children with low analog HLE showed greater language skills when experiencing a higher level of digital HLE (Figure 2). No significant interaction terms were found for math skills (Model 14).

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Multivariate regressions: associations between preschooler’s socio-emotional outcomes and digital and analog HLEs.

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Multivariate regressions: associations between preschooler’s academic outcomes and digital and analog HLEs.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Moderating effects of analog HLE on the association between digital HLE and language skills (preschooler age group).

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to investigate a potentially new dimension of the HLE, i.e., a digital HLE, that includes the frequency of shared digital media activities from an early age on, its association with analog HLE, and how these two dimensions of the HLE are related to children’s socio-emotional, practical life, and academic outcomes at toddler and preschool age. Furthermore, we explored whether potentially beneficial or harmful effects of digital HLE are enhanced or compensated by an analog HLE or by children’s language/practical life skills. Our results indicate that digital and analog HLEs can be seen as separate dimensions that are only marginally related to each other and differentially related to children’s outcomes.

The positive correlations between digital and analog HLEs in the toddler age group show that parents who actively involve their children in educational “analog” activities in this early phase of development do so with digital media, too. This finding aligns with previous research that showed that parents of children below the age of 3 years who read more often to their children also tended to show a greater frequency in various other activities, such as singing, playing with dolls, and doing crafts (Wirth et al., 2020b). We found that children’s digital HLE activities rapidly increase with age. However, in the older age group, digital and analog HLEs were no longer associated. Consequently, there seems to be neither a shift from an analog to a digital HLE, which would have been indicated by a negative correlation, nor a hint to a general indicator of the HLE comprising analog and digital aspects, which would have been indicated by a high, positive correlation. Rather, families’ HLE seems to follow differential developmental patterns, as their children grow older. Therefore, as shown in many studies before, the HLE is no unitary construct and needs to be differentiated (e.g., Skwarchuk et al., 2014; Wirth et al., 2019; Lehrl et al., 2020a). The present study gives further evidence that the medium through which stimulation in the home takes place is another variable that distinguishes dimensions of the HLE.

This differentiation gains further importance when inspecting the differential association between the digital and analog HLEs and child outcomes in the two age groups. In the younger age group, analog HLE activities were important for explaining variance in self-reported socio-emotional and practical life skills, whereas digital HLE activities were not. However, moderation analyses revealed that children with less frequent analog HLE activities showed greater socio-emotional skills when experiencing more frequent digital activities. Research has shown that low stimulating (analog) HLEs might be a risk factor for children’s developing academic and social skills (e.g., Mistry et al., 2008, 2010). Consequently, for children being at risk in terms of their low stimulating analog HLEs, sharing digital media may bring parents and children together in meaningful interactions that in turn may protect children from unfavorable developmental trajectories. There are hints that low socioeconomic status (SES) families are more involved in the education of their children when using digital tools, such as electronic books or apps (e.g., Erdogan et al., 2019; Wirth et al., 2020a). However, one has to bear in mind that we measured the digital HLE as the frequency of parent–child interaction when sharing digital devices, and not, for instance, as the frequency of the child passively watching TV alone.

In the preschooler’s age group, however, digital HLE activities were associated with weaker self-reported socio-emotional skills. Although the effect sizes are small, these results are alarming, especially when considering the non-significant moderator effects, which indicate that the negative digital HLE effect cannot be compensated by a high-quality analog HLE. As children grow older, interactions with digital media might be less communicative or guided by parents than the same interactions conducted with toddlers. For instance, a Korean study with 5-year-old children showed that an earlier onset of computer usage was associated with longer computer usage later. In addition, children with an early onset were more likely to play computer games and were less likely to be supervised while using the computer, which, in turn, was associated with higher scores on internet addiction and lower scores in socio-emotional competencies (Seo et al., 2011). Contrary to these findings, Gómez et al. (2013) reported positive effects of collaborative activities around computers on children’s social development skills. Obviously, the social and emotional effects of shared or non-shared digital media use on young children are underexplored, and more research is needed, especially in light of the often-proclaimed negative effects (e.g., Cordes and Miller, 2000; Sigman, 2012; Fröhlich-Gildhoff and Fröhlich-Gildhoff, 2017) and the resulting possible lost resources that may instead foster academic skills (Plowman and McPake, 2013).

Although only used as a control variable, our analyses show that the effects of overall warm and responsive interactions as indicated through the variable “positive parenting” compared with the digital HLE were twice as large (Tables 6, 7) and seemed to be more meaningful in the development of socio-emotional skills of children (see also Thomas and Zimmer-Gembeck, 2007 for an overview on the effects of positive-parenting interventions). The digital HLE was significantly associated with academic skills, although the analog HLE showed higher effect sizes. Concerning vocabulary acquisition, some studies also reported positive effects of digital media use, yet the greatest effects were observed when such use was guided by adults (Teepe et al., 2016; Walter-Laager et al., 2016). In addition, a meta-analysis showed that animated pictures as additions to stories can boost vocabulary development when they are congruent to the story (Takacs et al., 2015). Similar improvements have been shown in early number skills after interacting with math apps (Mattoon et al., 2015; Schacter and Jo, 2016; Watts et al., 2016). Consequently, our research is in line with the huge body of results that emphasizes the role of the analog HLE in shaping children’s language and mathematical skills (e.g., Hindman and Morrison, 2012; Niklas and Schneider, 2017; Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2019; Lehrl et al., 2020a,b). In addition, the present study adds to our knowledge by showing that the digital HLE adds to the effects of analog HLE activities.

However, the following limitations mark this study: HLE activities as well as child’s competencies were reported by parents, and although they are based on established measures often used in other studies (such as the SDQ), they might be biased. Although we tried to diminish this effect by controlling for the general tendency of agreement, we cannot completely rule this possibility out.

Furthermore, our analyses are only cross-sectional; thus, we do not know the direction of effects and no causal claims can be made. Potentially, child’s competence level may influence the frequency of digital HLE activities and not the other way around (Hygen et al., 2019). Indications of such associations are mainly found in the area of socio-emotional competences. For example, parents might react to social–emotional difficulties by trying to calm difficult infants/toddlers with digital media activities (Radesky et al., 2016). To disentangle such complex interrelations and the impact of both digital and analog HLEs on the development of socio-emotional and academic skills is certainly an avenue for future research. Furthermore, the data of our study are representative for families in 2013. The rapid growth in the digital sector in the last years underlines the need for further research in this area to investigate possible drifts and changes regarding the analog and digital HLEs.

Additionally, it must be considered that our digital HLE measure was a very general assessment of the shared use of digital media in the home environment that consists of only three items, and that we have no information on the content of the digital media activities and the quality of the interaction between parent and child during shared digital media use. Some study results point out that parent–child interaction and parents’ talk may be impoverished when they use digitally enhanced books (Zosh et al., 2015). However, as already mentioned, digital media might also foster children’s learning (e.g., Takacs et al., 2015), but only if it is designed appropriately (Hirsh-Pasek et al., 2015). For example, Parish-Morris et al. (2013) examined and compared interactions of parents with their 3-year-old children with digital books, containing different enhanced features (e.g., pre-recorded sounds), and found more enhanced features to be less beneficial for the parent’s use of high-quality language (dialogic reading strategies) and the child’s learning (story comprehension). This finding might even be true for younger children. Sosa (2016) demonstrated in a controlled experiment that in the condition with electronic toys, both adults and children produced fewer words, and conversation turns occurred less often than parent–child play with traditional toys and books.

Therefore, more research is needed to understand under which circumstances digital devices and apps may benefit children’s development. Here, policy for children’s media as well as research should also move away from focus on “screen time” and provide parents with specific guidelines to select quality content to optimize media experiences for young children (Huber et al., 2018). Detailing children’s screen media experience in a digital HLE will provide a better understanding of whether digital media can be used to promote learning in young children.

Conclusion

Children’s experiences with technology and interactive digital media are increasingly a part of their daily lives. With the present study, we have shown that sharing digital media at home may be seen as the digital facet of the HLE, which can be differentiated from the analog HLE. Our findings show once more the important role of the analog HLE for children’s competencies, but extend research by showing that the digital HLE also affects aspects of children’s development. Digital HLE means that children and parents share digital devices, and that parents are actively involved. Thus, here, parents are clearly the most important partners for young children’s interaction with digital technologies, and it is to be expected that the effect of digital media will depend on parents’ choice of suitable media and the support of their children. Useful support for parents in deciding how children can best benefit from digital technologies might be needed.

Data Availability Statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. Data can be retrieved under this URL: https://surveys.dji.de.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

SL and AL developed the research ideas, the constructs, and were responsible for the analyses. SL wrote the first draft of the manuscript. FN and SK contributed to the theoretical and discussion section and revised the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

Data collection has been supported by funding of the German Federal Ministry for Family Affairs, Senior Citizens, Women, and Youth.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ Please note that as the SDQ version for children from the age of 4–17 years was used, two items (“lies” and “steals”) were only answered if children were 48 months or older.

References

All, A., Nunez Castellar, E. P., and van Looy, J. (2016). Assessing the effectiveness of digital game-based learning: best practices. Comput. Educ. 9, 90–103. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2015.10.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anders, Y., Rossbach, H. G., Weinert, S., Ebert, S., Kuger, S., Lehrl, S., et al. (2012). Home and preschool learning environments and their relations to the development of early numeracy skills. Early Child. Res. Q. 27, 231–244. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.08.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berkowitz, T., Schaeffer, M. W., Maloney, E. A., Peterson, L., Gregor, C., Levine, S. C., et al. (2015). Math at home adds up to achievement in school. Science 350, 196–198. doi: 10.1126/science.aac7427

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biancarosa, G., and Griffiths, G. G. (2012). Technology tools to support reading in the digital age. Future Child 22, 139–160. doi: 10.1353/foc.2012.0014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bien, W., Pötter, U., and Quellenberg, H. (2015). “Methodische grundlagen von AID:A II. Stichprobe und Fallzahlen. Stichprobe und Fallzahlen,” in Aufwachsen in Deutschland heute. Erste Befunde aus dem DJI-Survey AID:A 2015, eds S. Walper, W. Bien, and T. Rauschenbach (München: Verlag Deutsches Jugendinstitut), 63–68.

Google Scholar

Bierman, K. L., Welsh, J. A., Heinrichs, B. S., Nix, R. L., and Mathis, E. T. (2015). Helping head start parents promote their children’s kindergarten adjustment: the research-based developmentally informed parent program. Child Dev. 86, 1877–1891. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, R. H., and Caldwell, B. M. (1984). The HOME Inventory and family demographics. Dev. Psychol. 20, 315. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.20.2.315

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckingham, D. (2007). Youth, Identity, and Digital Media. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press, 216.

Google Scholar

Bywater, T., Sammons, P., Toth, K., Sylva, K., Melhuish, E., Siraj, I., et al. (2015). The long-term role of the home learning environment in shaping students’ academic attainment in secondary school. J. Childrens Serv. 10, 189–201. doi: 10.1108/jcs-02-2015-0007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaudron, S., Beutel, M., Cernikova, M., Navarette, V. D., Dreier, M., Fletcher-Watson, B., et al. (2015). Young Children (0-8) and Digital Technology. A Qualitative Exploratory Study Across Seven Countries. Ispra: Joint Research Centre European Commission.

Google Scholar

Cordes, C., and Miller, E. (2000). Fool’s Gold: A Critical Look at Computers in Childhood. Alliance for Childhood. Available online at: https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED445803 (accessed April, 2020).

Google Scholar

Cristia, A., and Seidl, A. (2015). Parental reports on touch screen use in early childhood. PLoS One 10:e0128338. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0128338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erdogan, N. I., Johnson, J. E., Dong, P. I., and Qiu, Z. (2019). Do parents prefer digital play? Examination of parental preferences and beliefs in four nations. Early Childhood Educ. J. 47, 131–142. doi: 10.1007/s10643-018-0901-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fröhlich-Gildhoff, K., and Fröhlich-Gildhoff, M. (2017). Digitale medien in der Kita–die Risiken werden unterschätzt! Frühe Bildung 6, 225–228. doi: 10.1026/2191-9186/a000332

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ganzeboom, H. B., De Graaf, P. M., and Treiman, D. J. (1992). A standard international socio-economic index of occupational status. Soc. Sci. Res. 21, 1–56. doi: 10.1016/0049-089x(92)90017-b

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gómez, F., Nussbaum, M., Weitz, J. F., Lopez, X., Mena, J., and Torres, A. (2013). Co-located single display collaborative learning for early childhood education. Int. J. Comput. Supp. Collab. Learn. 8, 225–244. doi: 10.1007/s11412-013-9168-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hindman, A. H., and Morrison, F. J. (2012). Differential contributions of three parenting dimensions to preschool literacy and social skills in a middle-income sample. Merrill Palmer Q. 58, 191–223. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2012.0012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirsh-Pasek, K., Zosh, J. M., Golinkoff, R. M., Gray, J. H., Robb, M. B., and Kaufman, J. (2015). Putting education in “educational” apps: Lessons from the science of learning. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 16, 3–34. doi: 10.1177/1529100615569721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, B., Highfield, K., and Kaufman, J. (2018). Detailing the digital experience: parent reports of children’s media use in the home learning environment. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 49, 821–833. doi: 10.1111/bjet.12667

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huntsinger, C. S., Jose, P. E., and Luo, Z. (2016). Parental facilitation of early mathematics and reading skills and knowledge through encouragement of home-based activities. Early Childhood Res. Q. 37, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hygen, B. W., Belsky, J., Stenseng, F., Skalicka, V., Kvande, M. N., Zahl-Thanem, T., et al. (2019). Time spent gaming and social competence in children: reciprocal effects across childhood. Child Dev. 91, 861–875. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Judge, S., Floyd, K., and Jeffs, T. (2015). “Using mobile media devices and apps to promote young children’s learning,” in Young Children and Families in the Information Age: Applications of Technology in Early Childhood, eds K. L. Heider and M. Renck Jalongo (Dordrecht: Springer), 117–131. doi: 10.1007/978-94-017-9184-7_7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klasen, H., Woerner, W., Rothenberger, A., and Goodman, R. (2003). German version of the strength and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ-German)–overview and evaluation of initial validation and normative results. Prax. Kinderpsychol. Kinderpsychiatr 52, 491.

Google Scholar

Korat, O., and Shneor, D. (2019). Can e-books support low SES parental mediation to enrich children’s vocabulary? First Lang. 39, 344–364. doi: 10.1177/0142723718822443

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuger, S., Marcus, J., and Spieß, C. K. (2019). Day care quality and changes in the home learning environment of children. Educ. Econ. 27, 265–286. doi: 10.1080/09645292.2019.1565401

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehrl, S., Ebert, S., Blaurock, S., Rossbach, H. G., and Weinert, S. (2020a). Long-term and domain-specific relations between the early years home learning environment and students’ academic outcomes in secondary school. Sch. Effect. Sch. Improv. 31, 102–124. doi: 10.1080/09243453.2019.1618346

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehrl, S., Evangelou, M., and Sammons, P. (2020b). The home learning environment and its role in shaping children’s educational development. Sch. Effect. Sch. Improv. 31, 1–6. doi: 10.1080/09243453.2020.1693487

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linberg, A. (2018). Interaktion zwischen Mutter und Kind. Dimensionen, Bedingungen und Effekte. Empirische Erziehungswissenschaft. Münster: Waxmann.

Google Scholar

Manolitsis, G., Georgiou, G. K., and Tziraki, N. (2013). Examining the effects of home literacy and numeracy environment on early reading and math acquisition. Early Childhood Res. Q. 28, 692–703. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marci-Boehncke, G., Müller, A., and Rath, M. (2012). Medienkompetent zum Schulübergang, Erste Ergebnisse einer Forschungs- und Interventionsstudie zum Medienumgang in der frühen Bildung [Being media-literate for school transition: first results of a research and intervention study on media usage in early education]. Medienpädagogik. Zeitschrift Theorie Praxis Medienbildung 22, 1–22. doi: 10.21240/mpaed/22/2012.12.27.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsh, J., Brooks, G., Hughes, J., Ritchie, L., Roberts, S., and Wright, K. (2005). Digital Beginnings: Young Children’s Use of Popular Culture, Media and New Technologies. Sheffield: University of Sheffield.

Google Scholar

Mattoon, C., Bates, A., Shifflet, R., Latham, N., and Ennis, S. (2015). Examining computational skills in prekindergartners: the effects of traditional and digital manipulatives in a prekindergarten classroom. Early Childhood Res. Pract. 17, 1–9.

Google Scholar

Melhuish, E. C., Phan, M. B., Sylva, K., Sammons, P., Siraj-Blatchford, I., and Taggart, B. (2008). Effects of the home learning environment and preschool center experience upon literacy and numeracy development in early primary school. J. Soc. Issues 64, 95–114. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2008.00550.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyers, E. M., Erickson, I., and Small, R. V. (2013). Digital literacy and informal learning environments: an introduction. Learn. Media Technol. 38, 355–367. doi: 10.1080/17439884.2013.783597

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mistry, R. S., Benner, A. D., Biesanz, J. C., and Clark, S. L. (2010). Family and social risk, and parental investments during the early childhood years as predictors of low-income children’ s school readiness outcomes. Early Childhood Res. Q. 25, 432–449. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mistry, R. S., Biesanz, J. C., Chien, N., Howes, C., and Benner, A. D. (2008). Socioeconomic status, parental investments, and the cognitive and behavioral outcomes of low-income children from immigrant and native households. Early Childhood Res. Q. 23, 193–212. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2008.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, W., Lüttinger, P., König, W., and Karle, W. (1989). Class and education in industrial nations. Int. J. Sociol. 19, 3–39. doi: 10.1080/15579336.1989.11769981

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mullis, I. V., and Martin, M. O. (2017). TIMSS 2019 Assessment Frameworks. Amsterdam: International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement.

Google Scholar

Neumann, M. M. (2016). Young children’s use of touch screen tablets for writing and reading at home: relationships with emergent literacy. Comput. Educ. 97, 61–68. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2016.02.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niklas, F., Cohrssen, C., and Tayler, C. (2018). Making a difference to children’s reasoning skills before school-entry: the contribution of the home learning environment. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 54, 79–88. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niklas, F., Nguyen, C., Cloney, D. S., Tayler, C., and Adams, R. (2016). Self-report measures of the home learning environment in large scale research: measurement properties and associations with key developmental outcomes. Learn. Environ. Res. 19, 181–202. doi: 10.1007/s10984-016-9206-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niklas, F., and Schneider, W. (2017). Home learning environment and development of child competencies from kindergarten until the end of elementary school. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 49, 263–274. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

OECD (2013). “Framework for integrated analysis,” in OECD Framework for Statistics on the Distribution of Household Income, Consumption and Wealth (OECD Publishing). doi: 10.1787/9789264194830-11-en

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Farrelly, C., Doyle, O., Victory, G., and Palamaro-Munsell, E. (2018). Shared reading in infancy and later development: evidence from an early intervention. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 54, 69–83. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2017.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogelman, H. G., Gungor, H., Korukcu, O., and Sarkaya, H. E. (2016). The examination of the relationship between technology use of 5-6-year-old children and their social skills and social status. Early Childhood Dev. Care 188, 168–182. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2016.1208190

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Muircheartaigh, C., Krosnick, J. A., and Helic, A. (1999). “Middle alternatives, acquiescence, and thequality of questionnaire data,” in Paper Presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Association for Public Opinion Research, (St. Petersburg, FL: AAPOR).

Google Scholar

Palaiologou, I. (2016). Children under five and digital technologies: implications for early years pedagogy. Eur. Early Childhood Educ. Res. J. 24, 5–24. doi: 10.1080/1350293x.2014.929876

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parish-Morris, J., Mahajan, N., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., and Collins, M. F. (2013). Once upon a time: parent–child dialogue and storybook reading in the electronic era. Mind Brain Educ. 7, 200–211. doi: 10.1111/mbe.12028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petermann, F., and Macha, T. (2003). Elternfragebögen zur ergänzenden Entwicklungsbeurteilung bei den kinderärztlichen Vorsorgeuntersuchungen U6 bis U9 (EEE U6-U9). Frankfurt: Swets Test Services.

Google Scholar

Plowman, L., and McPake, J. (2013). Seven myths about young children and technology. Childhood Educ. 89, 27–33. doi: 10.1080/00094056.2013.757490

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radesky, J. S., Peacock-Chambers, E., Zuckerman, B., and Silverstein, M. (2016). Use of mobile technology to calm upset children: associations with social-emotional development. JAMA Pediatr. 170, 397–399. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.4260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rose, E., Lehrl, S., Ebert, S., and Weinert, S. (2018). Long-term relations between children’s language, the home literacy environment, and socioemotional development from ages 3 to 8. Early Educ. Dev. 29, 342–356. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2017.1409096

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schacter, J., and Jo, B. (2016). Improving low-income preschoolers mathe-matics achievement with Math Shelf, a preschool tablet computercurriculum. Comput. Hum. Behav. 55, 223–229. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.09.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sénéchal, M., and LeFevre, J. A. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skill: a five-year longitudinal study. Child Dev. 73, 445–460. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seo, H. A., Chun, H. Y., Jwa, S. H., and Choi, M. H. (2011). Relationship between young children’s habitual computer use and influencing variables on socio-emotional development. Child Dev. Care 181, 245–265. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2011.536644

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sigman, A. (2012). Time for a view on screen time. Arch. Dis. Child. 97, 935. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2012-302196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skwarchuk, S. L., Sowinski, C., and LeFevre, J. A. (2014). Formal and informal home learning activities in relation to children’s early numeracy and literacy skills: the development of a home numeracy model. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 121, 63–84. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Son, S. H., and Morrison, F. J. (2010). The nature and impact of changes in home learning environment on development of language and academic skills in preschool children. Dev. Psychol. 46, 1103–1118. doi: 10.1037/a0020065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sosa, A. V. (2016). Association of the type of toy used during play with the quantity and quality of parent-infant communication. JAMA Pediatr. 170, 132–137. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.3753

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takacs, Z. K., Swart, E. K., and Bus, A. G. (2015). Benefits and pitfalls of multimedia and interactive features in technology-enhanced storybooks: a meta-analysis. Rev. Educ. Res 85, 698–739. doi: 10.3102/0034654314566989

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Luo, R., McFadden, K. E., Bandel, E. T., and Vallotton, C. (2019). Early home learning environment predicts children’s 5th grade academic skills. Appl. Dev. Sci. 23, 153–169. doi: 10.1080/10888691.2017.1345634

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teepe, R. C., Molenaar, I., and Verhoeven, L. (2016). Technology-enhanced storytelling stimulating parent—Child interaction and preschool children’s vocabulary knowledge. J. Comput. Assist. Learn. 33, 123–136. doi: 10.1111/jcal.12169

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, R., and Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J. (2007). Behavioral outcomes of parent-child interaction therapy and triple P-positive parenting program: a review and meta-analysis. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 35, 475–495. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9104-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walter-Laager, C., Brandenberg, K., Tinguely, L., Pfiffner, M. R., and Moschner, B. (2016). Media-assisted language learning for young children: effects of a word-learning app on the vocabulary acquisition of two-year-olds. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 48, 1062–1072. doi: 10.1111/bjet.12472

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watts, C. M., Moyer-packenham, P. S., Tucker, S. I., Bullock, E. P., Shumway, J. F., Westenskow, A., et al. (2016). An examination of children’s learning progression shifts while using touch screen virtual manipulative mathematics apps. Comput. Hum. Behav. 64, 814–828. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.07.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wendt, H., Bos, W., Tarelli, I., Vaskova, A., and Hussmann, A. (2016). IGLU and TIMSS 2011. Skalenhandbuch zur Dokumentation der Erhebungsinstrumente und Arbeit mit den Datensätzen. Münster: Waxmann.

Google Scholar

Wirth, A., Ehmig, S. C., Drescher, N., Guffler, S., and Niklas, F. (2019). Facets of the early home literacy environment and children’s linguistic and socioemotional competencies. Early Educ. Dev. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2019.1706826 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wirth, A., Ehmig, S. C., Heymann, L., and Niklas, F. (2020a). “Promising interactive functions in digital storybooks for young children,” in International Perspectives on Digital Media and Early Literacy (pp.), eds K. J. Rohlfing and C. Müller-Brauers (Milton Park: Routledge).

Google Scholar

Wirth, A., Ehmig, S. C., Heymann, L., and Niklas, F. (2020b). Das Vorleseverhalten von Eltern mit Kindern in den ersten drei Lebensjahren in Zusammenhang mit familiärer Lernumwelt und Sprachentwicklung [Reading to Children Aged 0 – 3 years and the association with home literacy environment and early langugage development]. Frühe Bildung 9, 26–32. doi: 10.1026/2191-9186/a000464

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, S. S. H. (2015). Mobile digital devices and preschoolers’ home multiliteracy practices. Lang. Literacy 17, 75–90. doi: 10.20360/g2cp49

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yelland, N. J. (2018). A pedagogy of multiliteracies: young children and multimodal learning with tablets. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 49, 847–858. doi: 10.1111/bjet.12635

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zosh, J. M., Verdine, B. N., Filipowicz, A., Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., and Newcombe, N. S. (2015). Talking shape: parental language with electronic versus traditional shape sorters. Mind Brain Educ. 9, 136–144. doi: 10.1111/mbe.12082

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: preschoolers, toddlers, home learning environment (HLE), digital media and learning, socio-emotional competencies, academic competencies

Citation: Lehrl S, Linberg A, Niklas F and Kuger S (2021) The Home Learning Environment in the Digital Age—Associations Between Self-Reported “Analog” and “Digital” Home Learning Environment and Children’s Socio-Emotional and Academic Outcomes. Front. Psychol. 12:592513. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.592513

Received: 07 August 2020; Accepted: 26 January 2021;
Published: 25 March 2021.

Edited by:

Sergio Tobon, University Center CIFE, Mexico

Reviewed by:

Rabiu Muazu Musa, University Malaysia Terengganu, Malaysia
Christian Tarchi, University of Florence, Italy
Jason Small, Oregon Research Institute, United States

Copyright © 2021 Lehrl, Linberg, Niklas and Kuger. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Simone Lehrl, simone.lehrl@uni-bamberg.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.