Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 23 December 2016
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings
This article is part of the Research Topic Consumer engagement in health and well-being: theoretical and empirical perspectives in patient centered medicine View all 12 articles

Illness Representations of HIV Positive Patients Are Associated with Virologic Success

\r\nDaniela LeoneDaniela Leone1Lidia Borghi*Lidia Borghi1*Giulia LamianiGiulia Lamiani2Luca BarlasciniLuca Barlascini3Teresa BiniTeresa Bini4Antonella d&#x;Arminio MonforteAntonella d’Arminio Monforte4Elena Vegni,Elena Vegni1,3
  • 1Unit of Clinical Psychology, Department of Health Sciences, University of Milan, Milan, Italy
  • 2Department of Biomedical Sciences, Humanitas University, Milan, Italy
  • 3Unit of Clinical Psychology, ASST Santi Paolo e Carlo, Presidio San Paolo University Hospital, Milan, Italy
  • 4Clinic of Infectious Diseases, Department of Health Sciences, ASST Santi Paolo e Carlo, Presidio San Paolo University Hospital, University of Milan, Milan, Italy

Introduction: It is important for HIV positive patients to be engaged in their care and be adherent to treatment in order to reduce disease progression and mortality. Studies found that illness representations influence adherence through the mediating role of coping behaviors. However, no study has ever tested if patient engagement to the visits mediate the relationship between illness perceptions and adherence. This study aimed to explore illness representations of HIV positive patients and test the hypothesis that illness representations predict adherence through the mediating role of a component of behavioral engagement.

Methods: HIV-positive patients treated with highly active antiretroviral therapy (HAART) for at least one year and presenting to a check-up visit were eligible to participate in the study. Patients completed the Illness Perception Questionnaire-Revised. Behavioral engagement was measured based on the patients’ clinical attendance to the check-up visits; adherence to HAART was measured by viral load. Undetectable viral load or HIV-RNA < 40 copies/ml were considered indexes of virologic success.

Results: A total of 161 patients participated in the study. Most of them coherently attributed the experienced symptoms to HIV/HAART; perceived their condition as chronic, stable, coherent, judged the therapy as effective, and attributed their disease to the HIV virus and to their behavior or bad luck. The majority of patients (80.1%) regularly attended check-up visits and 88.5% of them reached virologic success. The mediation model did not show good fit indexes. However, a significant direct effect of two independent variables on virologic success was found. Specifically, the perception that the disease does not have serious consequences on patient’s life and the prevalence of negative emotions toward HIV were associated with virologic success. On the contrary, the patient’s perception that the disease has serious consequences on his/her life and the prevalence of positive emotions were associated with virologic failure. This model showed good fit indexes (CFI = 1; TLI = 1; RMSEA = 0.00; and WRMSR = 0.309).

Discussion: Results do not support the mediating role of behavioral engagement in the relationship between illness representations and adherence. As perception of serious consequences coupled with positive emotions are directly associated with virologic failure, clinicians should take them into account to promote treatment adherence.

Introduction

In the last 20 years, the care of HIV positive patients has changed dramatically. The introduction of highly active antiretroviral therapy (HAART) has allowed patients to achieve an undetectable plasma viral load (HIV – RNA level) and so a virologic success (Das et al., 2010; Gill et al., 2010), thus reducing mortality and morbidity (Mocroft et al., 2003). Due to this pharmacological success, HIV infection has moved from a mortal disease to a chronic condition. The key to virologic success seems to be the adherence to HAART (Gardner et al., 2011). Although viral suppression has become possible with moderate adherence (less than 95%) to HAART regimens, adherence is fundamental in reducing HIV related symptoms, mortality, and the side effects related to the treatments (Paterson et al., 2000; García et al., 2002; Bangsberg, 2006).

It is known that adherence is a complex behavior, influenced by the type of treatment regimen, patient–provider relationship and patient characteristics, including psychological factors (Chesney, 2003; Mills et al., 2006). In order to improve adherence and promote treatment success it is crucial to develop models that predict adherence (DiMatteo et al., 2012) and identify risk factors for non-adherence (Paterson et al., 2000; Ickovics and Meade, 2002; Wood et al., 2003). Psychological models that posit a relation between psychological factors and the management of a chronic illness can be broadly categorized (Horne and Weinman, 1998) as social cognitive models, such as the health belief model (Rosenstock, 1974), stage models, such as the precaution adoption process model (Weinstein, 1988), and the hybrid common-sense self-regulatory model (CS-SRM) of illness representations (Leventhal et al., 1980). The CS-SRM has been adopted by several researchers to predict adherence in several patient populations (e.g., Meyer et al., 1985; Weinman et al., 1996). This model suggests that patients create mental representations of their illness experience through cognitive and emotional processes in order to make sense of it (Leventhal et al., 1997; Cameron and Leventhal, 2003). Cognitive illness representations are composed of five essential dimensions: (1) identity (the nature of the illness and the symptoms the patient considers associated to the disease), (2) cause (personal beliefs about the cause of the illness), (3) timeline (the perceived chronicity of the illness), (4) consequences (perceptions about the short- and long-term effects of the illness), and (5) control (beliefs about the degree of illness and if treatment can be controlled). Beside these cognitive representations, emotions elicited by illness, such as fear, anger, or anxiety, are also integral to illness representations and develop simultaneously with the cognitive components (Leventhal et al., 1997). Based on the CS-SRM, Weinman et al. (1996) developed the Illness Perception Questionnaire (IPQ), in order to measure cognitive illness representations. The IPQ was later revised by Moss-Morris et al. (2002) into the Illness Perception Questionnaire Revised (IPQ-R) to include also the emotional components of patients’ representations.

According to the CS-SRM, when faced with a health-related problem, patients undergo a process, named the self-regulation process, which encompasses three phases: (1) illness representations; (2) coping strategies; and (3) evaluation (Broadbent et al., 2006). First, the patient seeks to understand the illness. From the various internal stimuli, such as illness activity or side effects of the disease, and external information, such as relationship with healthcare providers or public opinion, the cognitive and emotional representations of the health threat are constructed. Thus, illness representations are not necessarily scientifically based as they are often formulated from personal experience, social influences, and/or interaction with healthcare providers. Second, illness representations will lead to the selection of coping strategies to eliminate or control the threat posed by the disease (Broadbent et al., 2006). There are several classifications of coping strategies. One of the most common classifications divided the coping strategies into problem-focused strategies, such as gathering information, and taking action to manage the problem, and emotion-focused strategies aimed to manage distress by minimizing, reducing, or preventing the emotional components of a stressor, such as venting emotions, distracting or using avoidance strategies (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984). Third, patients evaluate the effectiveness of the coping strategy on the outcome or goal of the disease, as, for example, virologic success.

The usefulness and validity of the CS-SRM model has been confirmed by numerous studies on patients suffering from several chronic diseases, such as diabetes, cardiovascular disease, asthma, and cancer (Petrie and Weinman, 1997; Cameron and Leventhal, 2003; French et al., 2013; Mickevičienë et al., 2013). Several studies on different chronic illnesses have confirmed the mediating role of coping strategies in the relationship between illness representations and outcomes, such as mood, quality of life, and patients’ satisfaction (Diefenebach and Leventhal, 1996; Leventhal et al., 2001; Rutter and Rutter, 2002; Hagger and Orbell, 2003; Llewellyn et al., 2007; Gould et al., 2010; Catunda et al., 2016). However, some studies have suggested that illness representations are also directly associated with outcomes regardless of coping strategies, and that illness representations are more strongly associated with outcomes than coping strategies (e.g., Moss-Morris et al., 1996; Scharloo et al., 1998). In addition, some studies that did not measure coping in their research designs have shown strong direct relationships between some illness representations and outcome measures (Petrie et al., 1996; Schiaffino et al., 1998). Illness representations were found to predict patients’ self-management of their disease (Leventhal et al., 1997; Petrie and Weinman, 1997; Cameron and Leventhal, 2003), decisions to seek health care and comply with medical advice (Leventhal et al., 1980), functional adaption and adherence (Moss-Morris et al., 2002).

Overall, these studies highlight that coping strategies may not be the only mediator in the relationship between illness representations and adherence. Recently, patient engagement has received increased attention in the healthcare literature as a psychological factor that may influence adherence. The increasing number of people living with a chronic condition brings to light the importance of engaging patients in their health, helping them to integrate the disease into their identity and life, self-manage their disease and properly use the healthcare resources (Graffigna et al., 2014). A recent model developed by Graffigna et al. (2014) defined patient engagement as a dynamic process, in which patients experience four phases (blackout, arousal, adhesion, and eudaimonic project), each encompassing emotional, cognitive, and behavioral dimensions. According to this model, engagement is the final outcome of a series of emotional, cognitive, and behavioral reframing of the patient’s health condition (Barello and Graffigna, 2014). More specifically, fully engaged patients are able to integrate the disease into their identity and life, manage their own care and mobilize healthcare services proactively if needed. Among the different aspects of engagement, a behavioral dimension is highlighted that relates to the patient adhesion to medical prescriptions, retention in care and visit attendance. The importance of engagement in HIV care has been acknowledged by several authors as it was found to be associated with HIV outcomes and reduced risk transmission behaviors (Marks et al., 2005; Giordano et al., 2007; Metsch et al., 2008; Yehia et al., 2014).

Illness representations and engagement seem to play an important role in understanding HIV positive patients’ self-care behaviors and treatment adherence (Sacajiu et al., 2007; McGavock and Treharne, 2011). However, to our knowledge, no study has ever explored the relationship between illness representations, patient engagement and adherence.

Few studies have explored the relationship between illness representation and adherence in HIV positive patients (Cooper et al., 2009; Reynolds et al., 2009; Pala Norcini and Steca, 2015). Reynolds et al. (2009) found that cognitive illness representations were associated with self-care frequency and effectiveness in the context of HIV care. Cooper et al. (2009) found that adherence to HAART was influenced by individuals’ experiences of both HIV and HAART-related symptoms. Pala Norcini and Steca (2015) identified three configurations of perceptions of illness influence (low, moderate, and high) using the Brief IPQ-R questionnaire. They found that a higher perception of illness influence on patients’ lives (in terms of consequences, negative emotions and intensity of symptoms) was associated with greater viral load, with the mediating role of dysfunctional coping strategies in response to HIV-related stressors. In particular, this research found that high and moderate illness influence perception correlated with passive coping, which consisted in a lack of action in response to HIV-related stressors. Passive coping might also be seen as patients’ perception of helplessness or behavioral disengagement due to their expectation of poor outcomes (Carver et al., 1989).

Based on the results of the literature presented, we hypothesized that the relationship between illness representations and adherence in HIV positive patients could be mediated by behavioral engagement. The present study has two aims: (1) to explore illness representations of HIV positive patients in HAART and (2) to test the hypothesis that illness representations predict adherence through the mediating role of a component of behavioral engagement. Specifically, we tested if regular visit attendance mediates the relationships between illness representations and virologic success.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited at the outpatient clinic of infectious diseases of a university hospital in the north of Italy. This center cares for an average of 1000 HIV positive patients per year.

Inclusion criteria were: (1) HIV patients under treatment for at least 1 year; (2) age ≥ 18 years; (3) able to understand and provide informed consent; (4) able to understand Italian according to (depending on) the physician’s and/or researcher’s judgment; (5) no history of psychiatric symptoms; and (6) no actual alcohol or drugs abuse.

Data Collection

Participants were recruited in the waiting room, before their check-up visit. A researcher with training in clinical psychology presented the study to the patients. Patients who accepted to participated in the study signed an informed consent and were asked to complete the IPQ-R (Giardini et al., 2007). Socio-demographic, clinical, and adherence data were collected from inspection of the medical records. The research protocol was approved by the hospital ethics committee.

Measures

Socio-Demographic and Clinical Data

Socio-demographic information that was collected included gender, age, relationship status, educational level, nationality. Clinical information included HIV mode of transmission, year of HIV diagnosis, and therapeutic regimen (fix dose, other drugs, and number of pills per day).

Illness Representations

Illness representations were measured with the Italian version of the IPQ-R (Giardini et al., 2007). The questionnaire is composed of three sections: (a) identity; (b) opinions, and (c) causes. While items on identity section have dichotomous responses (yes/no), items regarding opinions and causes are rated by patients on a 5-point Likert scale (from “1 = completely disagree” to “5 = completely agree”).

(a) The identity section explores patients’ beliefs about the disease’s nature. Out of 14 symptoms, patients are asked to mark the ones they have experienced since being diagnosed with HIV and, which they believed to be linked to the disease/treatment. Two subscales were measured: reported symptoms and associated symptoms. A high score on identity scales indicates a great number of symptoms experienced and attributed to the disease.

(b) The opinions section is composed of 38 items exploring patients’ illness representations. Items are grouped into seven subscales: (1) timeline (perception of the disease as chronic); (2) cyclical symptoms (perception of a cyclic disease); (3) consequences (perception that the disease has serious physical, psychological, and social consequences on the patient’s life); (4) personal control (perception that actions can be taken to effectively manage the disease); (5) treatment control (high trust in the treatment and its efficacy); (6) coherence (high understanding of the disease); and (7) emotional representations (prevalence of negative emotions related to the disease).

(c) The causes section lists 18 possible illness causes and patients have to rate their level of agreement with each item as a cause of their disease. Only the three most represented causes were used for statistical analysis.

Behavioral Engagement

According to Graffigna et al. (2014), engagement is a complex concept encompassing cognitive, emotional, and behavioral dimensions. In this study, a behavioral component of engagement was assessed by measuring the patients’ regular attendance to the last check-up visit. Check-up visits are scheduled by the clinicians every three months, according to the internal guidelines of the clinic of infectious disease where the study was conducted.

Adherence

Based on the literature on adherence measures in HIV (Marcellin et al., 2013), an objective index measuring the viral load (level of HIV-RNA copies/ml) was adopted. The use of an objective index is a headway compared to previous studies on predictors of HIV patients’ adherence that commonly used self-reported measures (Ubbiali et al., 2008). An undetectable viral load or HIV-RNA < 40 copies/ml, observed at least at the two previous visits, was considered as virologic success. Virologic success as such is a clinical outcome. However, it was considered as an index of adherence because it is reached only through a regular and correct therapy intake. Indeed, in the clinic of infectious disease where the study was conducted, patients undergo a screening for HIV/RNA baseline, CD4+ baseline and drug resistance before starting the HAART treatment. The optimal HAART therapy, which is appropriated also for drug resistance, is then chosen for each patients according to these screening results. Therefore, regardless of their baseline viral load, patients are expected to reach virologic success after three to six months of treatment if they adhere to prescriptions.

Data Analysis

Means, standard deviations, frequencies, percentages, skewness, and kurtosis were used to describe demographic, clinical, IPQ-R, and adherence data. Subscales with skewness and kurtosis indexes > |1| were normalized with a logarithmic transformation.

Correlations between IPQ-R dimensions (identity and opinions) and socio-demographic variables were conducted. Coefficients were calculated according to the type of socio-demographic variable.

A mediation model was tested through structural equation modeling. IPQ-R dimensions (identity and opinions) were entered as independent variables, clinical attendance as a mediator, and virologic success as dependent variable. The socio-demographic variables which significantly correlated with IPQ-R dimensions were used as control variables. All direct and indirect effects were calculated. The model was evaluated by using the following fit indices: root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker Lewis index (TLI), and weighted root mean square residual (WRMSR). Values below 0.08 at the RMSEA (Browne and Cudeck, 1993), and values above 0.90 or higher at the CFI and TLI (Bentler, 1990) were judged as indicating an acceptable fit. Values below 0.06 at the RMSEA, and values above 0.95 at the CFI and TLI were judged as indicating a good fit (Hu and Bentler, 1999). We considered values <1 at WRMR as indicating a good fit (Yu, 2002).

The model was modified by removing the variables which did not show significant relations, until a good fit was reached. Data were analyzed using SPSS version 21.0 for Windows and Mplus version 4.0 for Windows.

Results

Of 231 eligible patients, 171 (74%) participated in the study. Ten patients did not return the questionnaire or returned it incomplete, therefore 161 questionnaires were collected.

Demographic and clinical characteristics of participants are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Participants’ socio-demographic and clinical data.

Illness Perception and Relationship with Socio-Demographic and Clinical Variables

Identity

One hundred and forty patients (87%) reported to have suffered from at least one of the symptoms listed since being diagnosed with HIV (mean = 4.8 ± 3.5, range 0–14). The most frequently reported symptoms were: fatigue (67.5%), loss of strength (52.2%), and sleep difficulties (41.3%); 98 patients (60.9%) deemed that at least one of the symptoms experienced was linked to HIV/HAART (mean = 2.6 ± 3.1, range 0–13). The most frequently symptoms associated to HIV/HAART were: loss of strength (72.8%), weight loss (68.5%), and nausea (65%).

Opinions

Means illness representation of the seven opinions are reported in Table 2. Timeline, personal control, treatment control, and coherence’ means are skewed to the higher values of the IPQ-R range; while cyclical symptoms’ mean is skewed to the lower values of the IPQ-R range.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Descriptive statistics of participants’ illness representation (IPQ-R opinions).

Causes

The three most frequently perceived reasons for becoming HIV were: a germ or virus (88.6%), my own behavior (70.4%), and chance or bad luck (54.1%) (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Participants’ causal attributions for acquiring HIV.

Relationship between Illness Representation, Behavioral Engagement, and Adherence

The majority of patients (80.1%) showed a regular attendance to the check-up visits, and 88.5% of the patients reached a virologic success. The correlation matrix between IPQ-R dimensions (identity and opinions) and socio-demographic variables is reported in Table 3. As gender and educational level correlated with at least one of the study variables, they were included as control variables in the mediation model.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Correlation matrix between IPQ-R dimensions (identity and opinions) and socio-demographic variables.

The initial mediation model did not show good fit indexes (Figure 2). Coefficients are shown in standardized form. Specifically, IPQ-R dimensions were not significantly associated with visit attendance, and visit attendance was not significantly associated with virologic success. Significant indirect effects were not detected. Since the mediation model did not fit the data, we tested a direct model between illness representations and virologic success and we proceeded by removing the variables which did not show significant relations, until a model with good fit indexes was obtained. The final path-analysis model is reported in Figure 3 and presented good fit indexes (CFI = 1; TLI = 1; RMSEA = 0.00; and WRMSR = 0.309). Coefficients are shown in standardized form. Only two independent variables were significantly associated with virologic success: the consequences scale, as a negative predictor, and the emotional representations scale, as a positive predictor. In other words, the perception that the disease does not have serious consequences on the patient’s life and the prevalence of negative emotions were associated with virologic success.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Mediation model between illness representations, visit attendance, and virologic success.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. The path-analysis model of virologic success predictors.

Discussion

Literature on chronic disease and HIV showed that patients’ illness representations affect illness experiences, management, and adherence (Moss-Morris et al., 1996; Petrie and Weinman, 1997; Spire et al., 2002; Cameron and Leventhal, 2003; Reynolds et al., 2009). The present study aimed to (1) explore illness representations of HIV positive patients in HAART and (2) test the hypothesis that illness representations predict adherence through the mediating role of a component of behavioral engagement.

For what concerns the first aim, results showed that most participants (87%) experienced various symptoms since HIV diagnosis and linked them to HIV and/or HAART coherently with their clinical condition. Overall, patients perceived HIV as a chronic and stable disease. They presented a good understanding of HIV, a general sense of personal control over the disease management and high trust in the treatment. Around 50% of patients did not perceive HIV as having serious consequences on their life and did not report negative related emotions. These results probably reflect the effectiveness of HAART treatment, which has transformed HIV from a terminal to a chronic disease, allowing patients to develop good disease control (Siegel and Lekas, 2002). With regard to the causal attribution for acquiring HIV, our findings highlighted that patients mainly attributed HIV to an “objective” cause (a germ or virus), consistently with a biomedical interpretation of their disease. The majority of patients seemed to be engaged in terms of their attendance to the check-up visits and to have a good adherence to HAART therapy.

As to the relationship between illness representations, behavioral engagement and adherence, our findings show that patients’ attendance to the check-ups does not mediate the relationship between illness representations and virologic success. However, illness representations were found to be directly associated with virologic success. Our findings highlighted that virologic success is related to a specific combination of cognitive and emotional representations in HIV patients. Notably, patients who perceived HIV as not having serious consequences on their life but reported negative emotions connected to the disease were found to reach virologic success. On the contrary, patients who experienced HIV as having more serious consequences on their life, but reported positive emotions, were found to be less adherent, showing a high viral load. According to the theoretical model of patient health engagement (PHE) (Graffigna et al., 2014), which describes the patient engagement process as a transition across four phases, each encompassing emotional, cognitive and behavioral dimensions, our findings seem to describe patients who are positioned at different phases of their health journey. Patients who failed to reach virologic success are those who perceive HIV as having more serious consequences on their lives but, at the same time, present more positive emotions. These patients are probably in the earlier stages of the PHE model and we could hypothesize that, faced with serious illness consequences, they use maladaptive coping strategies, such as emotional detachment and denial, that lead them to refer a prevalence of positive emotions. These patients may not perceive the importance of taking medications for maintaining good health in the long term. These results are consistent with previous studies on HIV infection and heart conditions, which found that patients perceiving their disease as having serious consequences reported higher viral load (Pala Norcini and Steca, 2015) and lower adherence to exercise therapy (Flora et al., 2015). On the contrary, patients who reached virologic success seem to be at an advanced stage of PHE process (such as adhesion and eudaimonic project) as they referred negative emotions related to HIV instead of denying them and, at a cognitive level, they did not perceive HIV as having too many consequences on their lives. Patients in these stages may present a correct intake of the HAART therapy, and thus are more likely to reach virologic success.

The fact that negative emotions were found to be associated with virologic success appears to be in contradiction with previous literature which identified depressive symptoms as the most consistent predictor of treatment non-adherence in HIV (Paterson et al., 2000; Ickovics and Meade, 2002; Ammassari et al., 2004) and other diseases (DiMatteo et al., 2000). This discrepancy may be due to the conceptual difference between emotional representations and depression. According to the CS-SRM, the emotional representations of illness reflect the patient’s cognitive evaluation of the emotional impact of the illness (Moss-Morris et al., 2002). Emotional representations refer to a cognitive elaboration of the emotional impact of the disease and play an important role in the adaptation process. Emotional representations can indeed motivate the patient to develop an action plan, or can be so overwhelming, resulting in less or no action taken with respect to the disease (Diefenebach and Leventhal, 1996). On the contrary, according to the CS-SRM the depressive symptoms are considered an illness outcome and refer to a dysfunctional emotional reaction that is not necessarily processed by a cognitive elaboration. Future studies should explore the mechanism by which emotional representations are translated into lived emotions, and should investigate the relationship between emotional representation and emotional outcomes (e.g., depression) in relation to adherence to HAART among HIV positive patients.

Interestingly, visit attendance did not mediate the relationship between illness representations and adherence. The lack of mediation may be differently discussed. It is possible that the use of visit attendance as an index of behavioral engagement may have reduced the complexity of the construct of patient engagement yielding non-significant results. It is also possible that attendance to the visits, as a form of problem-focused coping strategy, was not effective in order to reduce viral load among HIV patients. A recent meta-analysis (Dempster et al., 2015) highlighted that different coping strategies may work best for different conditions. It is possible that other problem-focused coping strategies should be considered or that emotional-focused strategies (e.g., acceptance/self-blame, avoidance, or social support) may be more effective to mediate or moderate the relationship between illness representations and virologic success in HIV positive patients.

Overall, our findings provided us with a more comprehensive picture of the relationships between illness representation, attendance to the visits and HAART adherence among HIV positive patients. Significant implications for theory and practice can be highlighted. At a theoretical level, our study has advanced research on adherence to HAART compared to previous literature which has mainly focused on socio-demographic, treatment-related or psychological characteristics without exploring the relationships between variables. As illness representations were not found to be associated with visit attendance, but were found to predict adherence, future research could explore the impact of other variables on visit attendance and virologic success, such as coping strategies. Self-blame or acceptance coping were found to mediate the effect of perceived consequences on outcomes among patients with irritable bowel syndrome. Patients who reported little consequences were more likely to accept the illness, achieve a better quality of life and be more satisfied with their health (Rutter and Rutter, 2002). In addition, the role of perceived social stigma could be assessed in relationship to visit attendance and virologic success.

At a practical level, our results shed light on specific illness representations that lead to virologic failure in HIV positive patients. Perceived consequences and emotional representations are potentially modifiable factors that may be targeted in future interventions to enhance patient adherence to HAART therapy, as already done for other illness and outcomes (Petrie et al., 2002; Llewellyn et al., 2007; Phillips et al., 2012). As reported by Llewellyn et al. (2007) in a study on patients with head and neck cancer, illness representations could be targeted for intervention in the time period between diagnosis and shortly after treatment in order to maximize longitudinal outcomes. Psychological group interventions could be implemented for HIV patients presenting specific illness representations in order to help them cope with the disease and process their emotions, thus promoting illness adaptation and empowerment. Another practical implication concerns also clinician-patient communication. The importance of exploring patients’ illness experience to promote adherence has been mentioned by several authors (Zolnierek and DiMatteo, 2009) and has been embraced by a patient-centered model of care (Borghi et al., 2016). During clinical encounters, clinicians should pay particular attention to their patients’ illness representations regarding HIV as a deeper understanding of the patients’ perspective could promote patient adherence (Borghi et al., 2016).

Our study has several limitations that are principally due to the convenience sample recruited, which presented a high prevalence of adherent patients as well as the small number of patients. Therefore, the generalizability of the findings is limited. Moreover, as the study is cross-sectional, the relationship between illness perception, attendance to visits and viral load have to be interpreted with caution especially for what concerns the direction of causality. Finally, our study focused on illness representations and visit attendance, but did not investigate the role of other variables in affecting adherence in HIV patients.

Conclusion

Our findings revealed a perception of good personal control in disease management, high trust in the therapy and good adherence to treatment. Since patients’ perceived consequences and emotions influence virologic success, clinicians should explore them in order to promote adherence to treatment.

Ethics Statement

The research protocol was approved by the San Paolo Hospital Ethics Committee. Patients who accepted to participate in the study signed an informed consent.

Author Contributions

DL, EV, TB, and AdM contributed to the conception and design of the work; DL contributed to the acquisition of data; LiB and LuB contributed to the data analysis; all authors contributed to the interpretation of data. DL, LiB, GL, and LuB contributed to the draft of the work; EV, TB, and AdM revised the work critically. All authors gave their final approval of the version to be published and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Roberta Di Donato, Marina Lorefice, and Enza Tesoro for their help in data collection and Stefano Solaro for clinical data managing. We thank Marika Pozzi for editing the manuscript. The authors would also thank all the patients who participated in the study.

References

Ammassari, A., Antinori, A., Aloisi, M. S., Trotta, M. P., Murri, R., Bartoli, L., et al. (2004). Depressive symptoms, neurocognitive impairment, and adherence to highly active antiretroviral therapy among HIV-infected persons. Psychosomatics. 45, 394–402. doi: 10.1176/appi.psy.45.5.394

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bangsberg, D. R. (2006). Less than 95% adherence to nonnucleoside reverse-transcriptase inhibitor therapy can lead to viral suppression. Clin. Infect. Dis. 43, 939–941. doi: 10.1086/507526

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barello, S., and Graffigna, G. (2014). Engaging patients to recover life projectuality: an Italian cross-disease framework. Qual. Life Res. 24, 1087–1096. doi: 10.1007/s11136-014-0846-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol. Bull. 107, 238–246. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borghi, L., Galli, F., and Vegni, E. A. M. (2016). “The patient-centered medicine as the theoretical framework for patient engagement,” in Promoting Patient Engagement and Participation for Effective Healthcare Reform, ed. G. Graffigna (Hershey, PA: IGI Global), 25–39. doi: 10.4018/978-1-4666-9992-2.ch002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broadbent, E., Petrie, K. J., Main, J., and Weinman, J. (2006). The brief illness perception questionnaire. J. Psychosom. Res. 60, 631–637. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.10.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Browne, M. W., and Cudeck, R. (1993). “Alternative ways to assess model fit,” in Testing Structural Equation Models, eds K. A. Bollen and J. S. Long (Newbury Park, CA: Sage), 136–162.

Google Scholar

Cameron, L. D., and Leventhal, H. (2003). The Self-Regulation of Health and Illness Behaviour. New York, NY: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., and Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: a theoretically based approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 56, 267–283. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.56.2.267

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catunda, C., Seidl, E. M. F., and Lemétayer, F. (2016). Illness perception and quality of life of HIV-positive persons: mediation effects of tenacious and flexible goal pursuit. Psychol. Health Med. 22, 129–137. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2016.1146404

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chesney, M. (2003). Adherence to HAART regimens. AIDS Patient Care STDS. 17, 169–177. doi: 10.1089/108729103321619773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, V., Gellaitry, G., Hankins, M., Fisher, M., and Horne, R. (2009). The influence of symptom experiences and attributions on adherence to highly active anti-retroviral therapy (HAART): a six-month prospective, follow-up study. AIDS Care 21, 520–528. doi: 10.1080/09540120802301824

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Das, M., Chu, P. L., Santos, G. M., Scheer, S., Vittinghoff, E., McFarland, W., et al. (2010). Decreases in community viral load are accompanied by reductions in new HIV infections in San Francisco. PLoS ONE 5:e11068. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dempster, M., Howell, D., and McCorry, N. K. (2015). Illness perceptions and coping in physical health conditions: a meta-analysis. J. Psychosom. Res. 79, 506–513. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2015.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diefenebach, M. A., and Leventhal, H. (1996). The common-sense model of illness representation: theoretical and practical considerations. J. Soc. Distress Homeles 5, 11–38. doi: 10.1007/BF02090456

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DiMatteo, M. R., Haskard-Zolnierek, K. B., and Martin, L. R. (2012). Improving patient adherence: a three-factor model to guide practice. Health Psychol. Rev. 6, 74–91. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2010.537592

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DiMatteo, M. R., Lepper, H. S., and Croghan, T. W. (2000). Depression is a risk factor for noncompliance with medical treatment: meta-analysis of the effects of anxiety and depression on patient adherence. Arch. Intern. Med. 160, 2101–2107. doi: 10.1001/archinte.160.14.2101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flora, P. K., Anderson, T. J., and Brawley, L. R. (2015). Illness perceptions and adherence to exercise therapy in cardiac rehabilitation participants. Rehabil. Psychol. 60, 179–186. doi: 10.1037/a0039123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

French, D. P., Wade, A. N., and Farmer, A. J. (2013). Predicting self-care behaviours of patients with type 2 diabetes: the importance of beliefs about behaviour, not just beliefs about illness. J. Psychosom. Res. 74, 327–333. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2012.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, D. O. P., Knobel, H., Carmona, A., Guelar, A., López-Colomés, J. L., and Caylà, J. A. (2002). Impact of adherence and highly active antiretroviral therapy on survival in HIV-infected patients. J. Acquir. Immune Defic. Syndr. 30, 105–110. doi: 10.1097/00042560-200205010-00014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardner, E. M., McLees, M. P., Steiner, J. F., del Rio, C., and Burman, W. J. (2011). The spectrum of engagement in HIV care and its relevance to test-and-treat strategies for prevention of HIV infection. Clin. Infect. Dis. 52, 793–800. doi: 10.1093/cid/ciq243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giardini, A., Majani, G., Pierobon, A., Gremigni, P., and Catapano, I. (2007). Contributo alla validazione italiana dell’IPQ-R. G. Ital. Med. Lav. Ergon. 29, A64–A74.

Google Scholar

Gill, V. S., Lima, V. D., Zhang, W., Wynhoven, B., Yip, B., Hogg, R. S., et al. (2010). Improved virological outcomes in British Columbia concomitant with decreasing incidence of HIV type 1 drug resistance detection. Clin. Infect. Dis. 50, 98–105. doi: 10.1086/648729

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giordano, T. P., Gifford, A. L., White, A. C., Suarez-Almazor, M. E., Rabeneck, L., Hartman, C., et al. (2007). Retention in care: a challenge to survival with HIV infection. Clin. Infect. Dis. 44, 1493–1499. doi: 10.1086/516778

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gould, R. V., Brown, S. L., and Bramwell, R. (2010). Psychological adjustment to gynaecological cancer: patients’ illness representations, coping strategies and mood disturbance. Psychol. Health 25, 633–646. doi: 10.1080/08870440902811163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graffigna, G., Barello, S., Riva, G., and Bosio, A. C. (2014). Patient engagement: the key to redesign the exchange between the demand and supply for healthcare in the era of active ageing. Stud. Health Technol. Inform. 203, 85–95. doi: 10.3233/978-1-61499-425-1-85

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagger, M. S., and Orbell, S. (2003). A meta-analytic review of the common-sense model of illness representations. Psychol. Health 18, 141–184. doi: 10.1080/088704403100081321

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horne, R., and Weinman, J. (1998). “Predicting treatment adherence,” in Adherence to Treatment in Medical Conditions, eds L. Myers and K. Midence (Amsterdam: Harwood Academic Publishers), 25–50.

Google Scholar

Hu, L. T., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equat. Mod. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ickovics, J. R., and Meade, C. S. (2002). Adherence to HAART among patients with HIV: breakthroughs and barriers. AIDS Care 14, 309–318. doi: 10.1080/09540120220123685

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S., and Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. New York, NY: Springer.

Google Scholar

Leventhal, H., Benyamini, Y., Brownlee, S., Deifenbach, M., Leventhal, E. A., Patrick-Miller, L., et al. (1997). “Illness representations: theoretical foundations,” in Perceptions of Health and Illness: Current Research and Applications, eds K. J. Petrie and J. A. Weinman (Amsterdam: Harwood Academic Publishers), 19–45.

Google Scholar

Leventhal, H., Leventhal, E. A., and Cameron, L. (2001). “Representations, procedures, and affect in illness self-regulation: a perceptual-cognitive model,” in Handbook of Health Psychology, eds A. Baum, T. Revenson, and J. Singer (New York, NY: Erlbaum), 19–48.

Google Scholar

Leventhal, H., Meyer, D., and Nerenz, D. (1980). “The common sense representation of illness danger,” in Medical Psychology, Vol. II, ed. S. Rachman (Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press), 7–30.

Google Scholar

Llewellyn, C. D., McGurk, M., and Weinman, J. (2007). Illness and treatment beliefs in head and neck cancer: is Leventhal’s common sense model a useful framework for determining changes in outcomes over time? J. Psychosom. Res. 63, 17–26. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2007.01.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcellin, F., Spire, B., Carrieri, M. P., and Roux, P. (2013). Assessing adherence to antiretroviral therapy in randomized HIV clinical trials: a review of currently used methods. Exp. Rev. Anti Infect Ther. 11, 239–250. doi: 10.1586/eri.13.8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marks, G., Crepaz, N., Senterfitt, J. W., and Janssen, R. S. (2005). Meta-analysis of high-risk sexual behavior in persons aware and unaware they are infected with HIV in the United States: implications for HIV prevention programs. J. Acquir. Immune Defic. Syndr. 39, 446–453. doi: 10.1097/01.qai.0000151079.33935.79

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGavock, Z. C., and Treharne, G. J. (2011). Young adults’ beliefs about people living with HIV/AIDS and rheumatoid arthritis: thematic analysis of a think-aloud questionnaire investigation. NZ. J. Psychol. 40, 71–78.

Google Scholar

Metsch, L. R., Pereyra, M., Messinger, S., Del Rio, C., Strathdee, S. A., Anderson-Mahoney, P., et al. (2008). HIV transmission risk behaviors among HIV-infected persons who are successfully linked to care. Clin. Infect. Dis. 47, 577–584. doi: 10.1086/590153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, D., Leventhal, H., and Gutmann, M. (1985). Common-sense models of illness: the example of hypertension. Health Psychol. 4, 115–135. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.4.2.115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mickevičienë, A., Vanagas, G., Jievaltas, M., and Ulys, A. (2013). Does illness perception explain quality of life of patients with prostate cancer? Medicina (Kaunas) 49, 235–241.

Google Scholar

Mills, E. J., Nachega, J. B., Bangsberg, D. R., Singh, S., Rachlis, B., Wu, P., et al. (2006). Adherence to HAART: a systematic review of developed and developing nation patient-reported barriers and facilitators. PLoS Med. 3:e438. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0030438

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mocroft, A., Ledergerber, B., Katlama, C., Kirk, O., Reiss, P., D’Arminio Monforte, A., et al. (2003). Decline in the AIDS and death rates in the EuroSIDA study: an observational study. Lancet 362, 22–29. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)13802-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moss-Morris, R., Petrie, K. J., and Weinman, J. (1996). Functioning in chronic fatigue syndrome: do illness perceptions play a regulatory role? Br. J. Health Psychol. 1, 15–25. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8287.1996.tb00488.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moss-Morris, R., Weinman, J., Petrie, K., Horne, R., Cameron, L., and Buick, C. (2002). The revised illness perception questionnaire (IPQ-R). Psychol. Health 17, 1–16. doi: 10.1080/08870440290001494

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pala Norcini, A., and Steca, P. (2015). Illness perceptions and coping strategies among individuals diagnosed with HIV. J. Behav. Med. 38, 620–631. doi: 10.1007/s10865-015-9639-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paterson, D. L., Swindells, S., Mohr, J., Brester, M., Vergis, E. N., Squier, C., et al. (2000). Adherence to protease inhibitor therapy and outcomes in patients with HIV infection. Ann. Intern. Med. 133, 21–30. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-133-1-200007040-00025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrie, K. J., Cameron, L. D., Ellis, C. J., Buick, D., and Weinman, J. (2002). Changing illness perceptions after myocardial infarction: an early intervention randomized controlled trial. Psychosom. Med. 64, 580–586. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200207000-00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrie, K. J., and Weinman, J. (1997). Perceptions of Health and Illness: Current Research and Applications. Amsterdam: Harwood Academic Publishers.

Google Scholar

Petrie, K. J., Weinman, J., Sharpe, N., and Buckley, J. (1996). Role of patients’ view of their illness in predicting return to work and functioning after myocardial infarction: longitudinal study. BMJ 312, 1191–1194. doi: 10.1136/bmj.312.7040.1191

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, L. A., Leventhal, H., and Leventhal, E. A. (2012). Physicians’ communication of the common-sense self-regulation model results in greater reported adherence than physicians’ use of interpersonal skills. Br. J. Health Psychol. 17, 244–257. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8287.2011.02035.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynolds, N. R., Eller, L. S., Nicholas, P. K., Corless, I. B., Kirksey, K., Hamilton, M. J., et al. (2009). HIV illness representation as a predictor of self-care management and health outcomes: a multi-site, cross-cultural study. AIDS Behav. 13, 258–267. doi: 10.1007/s10461-007-9297-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenstock, I. M. (1974). Historical origins of the health belief model. Health Educ. Monogr. 2, 328–335. doi: 10.1177/109019817400200403

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutter, C. L., and Rutter, D. R. (2002). Illness representation, coping and outcome in irritable bowel syndrome (IBS). Br. J. Health Psychol. 7, 377–391. doi: 10.1348/135910702320645372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sacajiu, G., Fox, A., Ramos, M., Sohler, N., Heller, D., and Cunningham, C. (2007). The Evolution of HIV illness representation among marginally housed persons. AIDS Care 19, 539–545. doi: 10.1080/09540120701243440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scharloo, M., Kaptein, A. A., Weinman, J., Hazes, J. M., Willems, L. N. A., Bergman, W., et al. (1998). Illness perceptions, coping and functioning in patients with rheumatoid arthritis, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease and psoriasis. J. Psychosom. Res. 44, 573–585. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(97)00254-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiaffino, K. M., Shawaryn, M. A., and Blum, D. (1998). Examining the impact of illness representations on psychological adjustment to chronic illnesses. Health Psychol. 17, 262–268. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.17.3.262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, K., and Lekas, H. M. (2002). AIDS as a chronic illness: psychosocial implications. AIDS 16, S69–S76. doi: 10.1097/00002030-200216004-00010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spire, B., Duran, S., Souville, M., Leport, C., Raffi, F., and Moatti, J. P. (2002). Adherence to highly active antiretroviral therapies (HAART) in HIV-infected patients: from a predictive to a dynamic approach. Soc. Sci. Med. 54, 1481–1496. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(01)00125-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ubbiali, A., Donati, D., Chiorri, C., Bregani, V., Cattaneo, E., Maffei, C., et al. (2008). The usefulness of the Multidimensional Health Locus of Control Form C (MHLC-C) for HIV+ subjects: an Italian study. AIDS Care 20, 495–502. doi: 10.1080/09540120701867115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinman, J., Petrie, K. J., Moss-Morris, R., and Horne, R. (1996). The Illness Perception Questionnaire: a new method for assessing the cognitive representations of illness. Psychol. Health 11, 431–445. doi: 10.1080/08870449608400270

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinstein, N. D. (1988). The precaution adoption process. Health Psychol. 7, 355–386. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.7.4.355

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, E., Hogg, R. S., Yip, B., Harrigan, P. R., O’Shaughnessy, M. V., and Montaner, J. S. (2003). Effect of medication adherence on survival of HIV-infected adults who start highly active antiretroviral therapy when CD4 count is 0.200 to 0.350 × 109 cells/L. Ann. Intern. Med. 139, 810–816. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-139-10-200311180-0000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yehia, B. R., French, B., Fleishman, J. A., Metlay, J. P., Berry, S. A., Korthuis, P. T., et al. (2014). Retention in care is more strongly associated with viral suppression in HIV-infected patients with lower versus higher CD4 counts. J. Acquir. Immune Defic. Syndr. 65, 333–339. doi: 10.1097/QAI.0000000000000023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, C. Y. (2002). Evaluating Cutoff Criteria of Model fit Indices for Latent Variable Models with Binary and Continuous Outcomes. Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.

Google Scholar

Zolnierek, K. B., and DiMatteo, M. R. (2009). Physician communication and patient adherence to treatment: a meta-analysis. Med. Care 47, 826–834. doi: 10.1097/MLR.0b013e31819a5acc

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: adherence, HAART, HIV, illness representations, IPQ-R, patient engagement, virologic success

Citation: Leone D, Borghi L, Lamiani G, Barlascini L, Bini T, d’Arminio Monforte A and Vegni E (2016) Illness Representations of HIV Positive Patients Are Associated with Virologic Success. Front. Psychol. 7:1991. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01991

Received: 30 June 2016; Accepted: 06 December 2016;
Published: 23 December 2016.

Edited by:

Gian Mauro Manzoni, Università degli Studi eCampus, Italy

Reviewed by:

Martin S. Hagger, Curtin University, Australia
Daniela Villani, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Italy
Pythia Nieuwkerk, University of Amsterdam, Netherlands

Copyright © 2016 Leone, Borghi, Lamiani, Barlascini, Bini, d’Arminio Monforte and Vegni. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lidia Borghi, lidia.borghi@unimi.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.