Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 15 May 2017
Sec. Stroke

Observational Study of 180° Turning Strategies Using Inertial Measurement Units and Fall Risk in Poststroke Hemiparetic Patients

\r\nRmi Pierre-Marie BarroisRémi Pierre-Marie Barrois1Damien Ricard,,Damien Ricard1,2,3Laurent Oudre,Laurent Oudre1,4Leila TliliLeila Tlili5Clment Provost,Clément Provost1,5Alinor VienneAliénor Vienne1Pierre-Paul VidalPierre-Paul Vidal1Stphane Buffat,,Stéphane Buffat1,3,6Alain P. Yelnik,*\r\nAlain P. Yelnik1,5*
  • 1Cognition and Action Group, Cognac-G, CNRS UMR 8257, Université Paris Descartes, Service de Santé des Armées, Paris, France
  • 2Service de Neurologie, Hôpital d’Instruction des Armées de Percy, Service de Santé des Armées, Clamart, France
  • 3École d’application du Val-de-Grâce, Service de Santé des Armée, Paris, France
  • 4Institut Galilée, Université Paris 13, Villetaneuse, France
  • 5PRM Department, GH St Louis Lariboisière F. Widal, AP-HP, Paris Diderot University, Paris, France
  • 6Institut de Recherche Biomédicale des Armées, Bretigny-sur-Orge, France

Objective: We analyzed spontaneous 180° turning strategies in poststroke hemiparetic patients by using inertial measurement units (IMUs) and the association of turning strategies with risk of falls.

Methods: We included right paretic (RP) and left paretic (LP) post-stroke patients, and healthy controls (HCs) from a physical and rehabilitation department in France between July 2015 and October 2015. All subjects were right-handed and right-footed for mobilization tasks. Participants were instructed to turn 180° in a self-selected direction after a 10-m walk while wearing three IMUs on their trunk and both feet. We defined three turning patterns based on the number of external steps (pattern I = 1; II = 2–4 steps; and III ≥ 5) and four turning strategies based on the side chosen to turn (healthy or paretic) and the stance limb used during the first step of the turn (healthy or paretic). Falls in the 6 months after measurement were investigated.

Results: We included 17 RP [mean (SD) age 57.5 (9.5) years (range 43–73)], 20 LP patients [mean age 60.7 (8.8) years (range 43–63)], and 15 HCs [mean age 56.7 (16.1) years (range 36–83)]. The LP and RP groups behaved similarly in turning patterns, but 90% of LP patients turned spontaneously to the paretic side versus 59% of RP patients. This difference increased with turning strategies: 85% of LP versus 29% of RP patients used strategy 4 (paretic turn side with paretic limb). Patients using strategy 4 had the highest rate of falls.

Conclusion: We propose to consider spontaneous turning strategies as new indicators to evaluate the risk of fall after stroke. IMU could be routinely used to identify this risk and guide balance rehabilitation programs.

Introduction

Falling is a common complication in poststroke ambulatory patients with hemiparesis, and depending on the study, it occurs in 14–65% of patients during hospitalization (1, 2) and 37–73% after discharge from a hospital (3, 4). Many fall risk factors such as the stroke location are debated (59). Activities at the time of fall commonly involve daily transferring tasks, which suggests that turning and more precisely the stepping pattern during turning could be particularly problematic for poststroke patients [see review in Ref. (10)].

In healthy populations, a 180° turn consists of a complex and varied foot-stepping pattern that leads to a smooth continuous top-down rotation from the head to the trunk (1116). Two main turning strategies have been identified: the spin turn involves a change in direction toward the side of the stance limb and the step turn involves a change in direction away from the stance limb (12, 13). The spin strategy is less stable because it leads to a reduced base of support (12, 13, 1719). Clinically, the number of external steps and the turn duration are used to evaluate turning (11). The spontaneous turn direction patients choose is also a good indicator because it demonstrates the strategies they are able to develop.

Poststroke consequences (paresis, hypoesthesia, and visuospatial neglect) cause marked alterations in 180° turning, including increased number of external steps, decreased turn velocity, and altered axial segment coordination (2022). Right hemispheric stroke presents specific cognitive deficits (anosognosia, somatagnosia, and neglect), which could be involved in fall occurrence (10). These deficits likely affect the choice of turning strategy. To date, turn duration was found the only significant variable differentiating faller from non-faller paretic patients (22). However, turning strategies have not been studied in poststroke survivors. To the best of our knowledge, no study has focused on differences in 180° turns between left paretic (LP) and right paretic (RP) patients. Such an analysis is important to evaluate the risk of falls and to guide rehabilitation.

Full-body reflective markers and walking mates are the reference tool for studying turning kinematics and stepping patterns in humans (15, 18, 2025), but these tools are expensive and difficult to use in the clinics. Therefore, we used inertial measurement units (IMUs) for measurement, which are valid measuring tools for walking (2628) and turning (29, 30) and are much cheaper and more compatible with routine use in clinical departments, retirement homes, and the patient’s home. Here, we studied the kinematics with IMUs of spontaneous 180° turns in poststroke left and right hemiparetic patients and controls and their relationship with risk of falls.

Materials and Methods

Participants

We included all stroke patients from the same physical and rehabilitation medicine department between July 2015 and October 2015; inclusion criteria were (a) only one stroke event leading to hemiparesis, (b) could perform a timed up-and-go test (TUG) and walk 10 m without human nor assistive aids, (c) giving informed consent, and (d) right-handed and right-footed for mobilization tasks according to the lateral preference questionnaire (Table A1 in Appendix) (31). Even if it has never been reported to our knowledge, “footedness” may affect the spontaneous turning side and the turning strategy (32). In fact, turning requires mobilization of an initiator limb contralateral to the first stance limb, which may be affected by footedness (3335). We established a validated 8-item lateralization preference score ranging from −8 (consistent left) to +8 (consistent right); a score <4 and an answer “left” to the question “foot to kick a ball” excluded the participant.

We excluded patients with (a) multiple stroke, (b) a cerebellar syndrome, (c) a vestibular or musculoskeletal disorder that could affect walking or balance, and (d) an inability to understand the instructions. Patients were divided into RP and LP. The stroke type (ischemic or hemorrhagic) and location (hemispheric or pontine; see Table 1 for locations) were diagnosed by MRI. The etiology of the stroke had been established by adapted medical explorations. A poststroke period of ≤6 months at the time of the measurement was used to define chronic and recent strokes.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of healthy controls (HCs) and poststroke patients who were left paretic (LP) or right paretic (RP).

Functional ability was assessed in patients by the new functional ambulation categories (NFAC) test (36) (total score 8) and the TUG in seconds. Lower-limb motor control was assessed by the lower-limb Fugl-Meyer (FM) subscale (total score 34). We evaluated sensitivity via proprioceptive, thermal, and superficial deficits, graded 2 for no deficit, 1 for hypoesthesia, and 0 for anesthesia (superficial sensitivity deficit was graded 0 or 1); visuospatial neglect by the GEREN battery (37); visual field at the bedside; and dysexecutive syndrome and aphasia (graded present or absent).

The occurrence of falls within the 6 months after the measurement was assessed by a declarative fall questionnaire administered by phone call to patients or relatives. Patients were asked about the frequency, location, activity, and consequences of a fall, and whether they had injured themselves or experienced a fear of falling. Near-falls were not considered falls. Participants were classified as non-fallers (non-fallers with or without near-falls) or fallers (one-time fallers and repeat fallers) (9).

Healthy controls volunteered freely and had comparable age to patients. HC participants were right-handed and right-footed for mobilization tasks and had no neurological, vestibular, rheumatologic, or orthopedic disorders that could affect walking or balance.

This observational study was approved by the local ethics committee (Comité de Protection des Personnes Ile de France II, no. CPP 2014-10-04 RNI). Patients and controls gave their written informed consent to participate.

Instrumentation and Data Acquisition

We measured linear accelerations and angular velocities of the lower back (L4–L5 vertebra) and the feet (dorsal face) by using three IMUs: triaxial accelerometers, gyroscopes, and magnetometers (Technoconcept, Mane, France; I4 Motion, autonomy 4 h Li-Ion battery, device dimensions 4.9 cm × 3.8 cm × 1.9 cm, acceleration range ±6 g, angular velocity range ±500°/s, sampling frequency 100 Hz, and angular velocity measurement error <1°/s). IMUs were attached to the body with manufacturer-designed adhesive straps and connected to a computer via Bluetooth (29). A linear drift correction was applied to the foot angular velocities by assuming null velocity of the foot during foot flat (FF) periods (38). A linear drift correction was applied to the axial angular position during the 180° turn by assuming 0° at the beginning of the turn and 180° when the turn was completed.

After sensor fixation, participants were instructed to execute the following task with their shoes on and without walking aids: stand quietly for 6 s at the starting point, walk at a comfortable walking speed to a previously shown turn point that was 10 m away (without previous specification of a turning side), and walk back to the starting point.

180° Turning Representation

Foot flat during walking and turning was manually annotated on the feet angular velocities in the sagittal plane (Figure 1) as previously validated by Mariani et al. (26). The rotation of the trunk in the axial plane during each FF period [i.e., axial rotation of the trunk during FF (ARTF)] was computed by integrating the angular velocity of the trunk in the axial plane during each FF period (Figure 1). The resulting data representation is shown in Figure 2A. Because ARTF never exceeded 10° during straight-ahead walking, a FF was considered part of a 180° turn when it exceeded a 10° empirical threshold, which allowed us to define both the 180° turn start and end.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Typical raw data for one healthy participant. From the bottom to the top: right foot mediolateral angular velocity (A). Right foot flat annotated from panel (A) is (B). Left foot mediolateral angular velocity (C). Left foot flat annotated from panel (C) is (D). Lower back vertical angular velocity (E). Lower back vertical angular position during each right (blue) and left (red) foot-flat obtained by integration of panel (E) during the colored time periods (F). yes and yes represent foot-flat and swing phases, respectively.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Typical vertical angle covered by the trunk during each stance phase during walking and turning (see Figure 1 for details) for three participants presenting pattern I (1 external step), pattern II (2–4 external steps), and pattern III (≥5 external steps). Blue corresponds to the right side, and red background corresponds to the left side. Full lines at the left or at the right represent right (blue) and left (red) stance phases. Thin, dashed black lines indicate the start (time = 0 s) and the end of the turn. The thin continuous black curve corresponds to the cumulative angle during the 180° turn. (B) Spontaneous 180° turning patterns for healthy controls (HCs), right paretic (RP), and left paretic (LP) patients. Patterns I–III are represented by a gradient of green (from light to dark green). (C) Percentage of fallers among all paretic patients as a function of the spontaneous turning pattern.

Turning Patterns

For each participant, the external number of steps, duration of the turn, and mean angular velocity of the trunk in the axial plane were computed. We defined three turning patterns (Figure 2A) according to the number of external steps taken. Pattern I involved 1 external step, pattern II from 2 to 4 steps and pattern III ≥5 steps.

Turning Strategies

For each participant, we observed the spontaneous turn side and determined the stance limb during the first step of the turn (i.e., first stance limb), given by the 180° turning representation. According to an extension of previous literature that defined turning strategies for 90° turns in healthy populations (13, 19), we defined four turning strategies in our patients: strategy 1, turn toward the healthy side with a healthy first stance limb; strategy 2, turn toward the healthy side with a paretic first stance limb; strategy 3, turn toward the paretic side with a healthy first stance limb; and strategy 4, turn toward the paretic side with a paretic first stance limb.

Statistical Analysis

The data were processed by using MATLAB® (2013 version). Categorical variables were compared by chi-square test. Quantitative variables were compared by one-way ANOVA. Significance was defined as p < 0.05.

Results

Participants

We included 17 patients with RP [mean (SD) age 57.5 (9.5) years (range 43–73)], 20 with LP [mean age 60.7 (8.8) years (range 43–63)], and 15 HC [mean age 56.7 (16.1) years (range 36–83)]. RP and LP groups did not differ in age or clinical deficiencies and disabilities scored by FM, NFAC, TUG, and GEREN (Table 1). Five RP and four LP patients were recent strokes. All participants were right-handed, with a median score of 4 (range 4–8) on the lateralization scale and were right-footed for mobilization tasks. All participants performed the protocol.

Turning Patterns

All 52 spontaneous 180° turns but one, which was erratic, were classified by turning patterns. Figure 2B shows the turning patterns in percentages for the HC, LP, and RP groups. The LP and RP groups behaved similarly in terms of turning patterns. Pattern III was absent in the HC group, and pattern I was unusual in the paretic groups (RP 12% and LP 15%). The mean (SD) turn duration significantly differed by turning patterns I, II, and III—2.3 (0.6), 3.8 (0.9), and 7.0 (3.3) s, respectively—as did the mean (SD) angular velocity of the trunk in the axial plane—80.0 (20.0), 52.3 (11.1), and 32.0 (0.7)°s, respectively. The mean (SD) TUG time differed by patterns II and III: 15.4 (3.8) and 25.7 (7.8) s. The NFAC and FM differed by turning patterns II and III and I and III (Table A2 in Appendix).

Turning Strategies

The spontaneous 180° turn side for the groups is shown in Figure 3A: 54% of HC, 95% of LP, and 41% of RP patients chose the left side. The first stance limb by side of turn is in Figure 3B. Among HC subjects, for 62% of the participants who turned to the left and all participants who turned to the right, the first stance limb was the internal limb. Among LP and RP patients, for turns to the healthy side, the first stance limb was always the healthy and internal limb (strategy 1). For 90% of LP patients and 50% of RP patients who turned to the paretic side, the first stance limb was the paretic and internal limb (strategy 4). Globally, LP patients showed a marked predominance of strategy 4 (85%) as compared to RP patients (29%). No patient performed strategy 2.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. (A) Spontaneous 180° turn side (STS) for healthy controls, RP, and LP patients. The exercise is observed from the top. Hatched areas yes represent hemiparesis. (B) First stance limb (gray circle yes) with respect to the spontaneous turn side (across the number N for each column).

The turning strategies by time since stroke are shown in Figure 4A. Among recent poststroke patients, none of the RP patients performed strategy 4 as compared with 75% of the LP patients. This percentage increased for chronic RP patients (40%) but remained constant for LP patients (88%).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (A) Proportion of turning strategies for recent (time since stroke <6 months) and chronic (>6 months) LP and RP patients. Turning strategies numbered from 1 to 4 are indicated in the top left corner of each cell. The turning strategies are defined by the spontaneous turn side with respect to the hemiparesis side (scheme of the exercise observed from the top at the top of each column) and the stance limb during the first step of the turn (indicated by a black circle yes around the footprint at the left of each row). The two tables on the left show the data for LP patients, the two tables on the right show the data for RP patients, the two top tables show the data for recent strokes, and the two bottom tables show the data for chronic strokes. The proportion of patients in a given subgroup who performed a given turning strategy is represented by a proportional sized black circle. Hatched areas yes represent hemiparesis. (B) Proportion of fallers among LP (B-1) and RP patients (B-2) as a function of turning strategy.

Turning strategies were not associated with turning patterns. However, recent RP patients who performed strategy 1 had better turning kinematics, TUG, NFAC, and FM scores, than those who performed strategy 3 (Table A3 in Appendix). Chronic RP patients who performed strategy 1 or 3 had better TUG, NFAC, and FM scores than those who performed strategy 4. The corresponding data for LP patients are not shown because all but three performed strategy 4.

Relation of Turning Strategy with Fall Incidence

During the 6 months after the turning measurement, 42 falls occurred in eight patients (four one-time fallers and four repeat fallers) (Table 2). The mean (SD) number of external steps required to perform the 180° turn was greater for fallers than non-fallers—4.0 (1.6) and 3.1 (0.7) (p = 0.026)—and mean (SD) TUG time was greater—23.1 (9.4) versus 16.3 (6.0) s (p = 0.018). The turn duration, mean angular velocity of the trunk in the axial plane, FM, and NFAC did not differ between non-fallers and fallers (Table A4 in Appendix).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Total number, circumstances, and consequences of falls.

Fallers represented 29% of the RP patients and 15% of the LP patients. No patients who presented pattern I, 18% of patients who presented pattern II, and 33% of patients who presented pattern III were fallers (Figure 2C). The patient who was unclassified regarding turning patterns was a faller. In the RP and LP groups, the highest proportion of fallers were patients who performed turning strategy 4 (40 and 17%; Figure 4B). In the RP group, the proportion of fallers was greater among patients who performed strategy 1 or 4 (28 and 40%) than strategy 3 (20%). In the LP group, no patient who performed strategy 1 or 3 was a faller.

Discussion

We investigated spontaneous 180° turns after 10 m of walking in a poststroke population with hemiparesis and controls by using three IMUs used for routine hospital practice. We described three turning patterns based on the number of external steps and extend the definitions of turning strategies to 180° turns in poststroke hemiparetic patients according to the spontaneous turn side and the first stance limb chosen. The LP and RP groups behaved similarly in turning patterns, but most LP patients chose to turn to the paretic side as compared with only about half of the RP patients. This difference increased with turning strategies: 85% of LP versus 29% of RP patients used strategy 4 (paretic turn side with paretic limb at first stance). The patients using strategy 4 had the highest rate of falls. Spontaneous turning strategies could be routinely assessed by using IMUs to identify risk of new falls among poststroke patients.

We found that IMUs are valid tools for locomotion measurements in stroke patients (27, 28). The positioning of three IMUs on the trunk and on the feet had been used for ambulatory monitoring of turns in Parkinson’s disease (30) and older-aged populations (39, 40), but this was the first time they were found appropriate to investigate poststroke turns in walking. IMUs appeared to perform adequately for defining the 180° turn start and end and for describing turning patterns and turning strategies in routine hospital practice. Actually, the stance limb can be recognized during the first turn step by only clinical observation. Usual turning factors (i.e., duration and mean axial angular velocity) could then be recorded also.

We considered participants who fell once as fallers based on previous results: for stroke patients, falls or near-falls while in hospital are strongly associated with falls when people return home (3, 4, 10) and older people in the general population who fall once are likely to become repeat fallers (3, 4, 4143).

The spin and step turning strategies are commonly defined for 90° turns (12, 13, 1719). Without the participant receiving specific instructions regarding turning, the reorientation toward the new direction of travel is often completed with more than one step (17). The step in the turn that should be chosen to define the turning strategy remains an open question (17, 19). In the Taylor et al. study, 20% of the turns were unclassifiable (13) and Fino et al. found variable results depending on the methods used to define turning strategies (19). For mechanical reasons, we hypothesized that the turn initiation constituted one critical instant for poststroke patients. Thus, we proposed the definition of 180° turning strategies considering the stance limb during the first step of the 180° turn. Strategies 1 and 4 are considered close to spin strategies and strategies 2 and 3 close to step strategies.

To our knowledge, spontaneous choice of turning has not been considered crucial information in previous studies imposing the side of turning (20, 21, 44, 45). Regarding falls in daily life, the side chosen for spontaneous turning may be important. In psychological studies of the spontaneous turn side in healthy subjects, 60% of right-handed participants turn to the left (46). With our conditions, 54% of right-handed HC participants turned spontaneously to the left. The consistency of our results with the literature validates our protocol and shows the absence of bias in the spontaneous choice of turning direction.

Turning patterns, as defined here, conveniently summarize the turning kinematics because they reflect the turning performances well (i.e., turn duration, number of external steps, and mean angular velocity) (11, 22, 4749). Pattern I (<2 steps) was absent in paretic populations, which confirms the loss of pivoting during turning, as underlined by others (11, 22). The use of more steps when turning is thought to signify instability (39, 40, 48, 49). Suggestions for thresholds of number of steps to turn that indicate the risk of falls in community-dwelling older adults vary from 1 to >12 steps to complete a 360° turn (47) to ≥5 steps to accomplish a 180° turn (11, 22). Likewise, our findings show a relation between the number of steps taken to turn and risk of fall. In the only study to our knowledge that analyzed turning kinematics and risk of fall, turn duration and not number of steps differed between fallers and non-fallers (22). This discrepancy could be due to our analysis of spontaneous turns in daily clinical conditions. Hollands et al. imposed the turn side and performed the measurements in a gait analysis laboratory. Both these factors likely affect turning kinematics.

Considering turning strategies, turning toward the side of the stance limb reduces the polygon of lift (12, 13, 1719). However, falls are more likely to occur during turns to the paretic side (9). Both the stance limb and the paretic turn direction could explain the highest rate of fallers among RP and LP patients (40 and 17%, respectively) among patients who performed strategy 4, a turn to the paretic side in the direction of the stance limb.

The LP and RP patients differed in turning strategies, with a marked predominance of strategy 4 among LP patients. This finding could suggest a differential role of both hemispheres in postural control (50). Anosognosia, neglect, and dysexecutive syndrome are predominant in right-hemispheric lesions of right-handed people (51) (i.e., among our LP patients, except for two who had a subcortical pontine stroke). We believe that turning strategy 4 is more natural because turns to the paretic side are simpler for mechanical reasons (15, 20, 21). Furthermore, our results among healthy subjects suggest that initiating the 180° by standing on the internal limb is more natural. Thus, mechanical easiness and a lack of consciousness of danger could explain why LP patients spontaneously adopt strategy 4.

Among RP patients, a good consciousness of the danger may lead to a modulation of turning strategies. Among recent RP patients, those with good motor skills turned to the healthy side, following the natural tendency of healthy participants. However, recent RP patients with high motor impairment turned to the paretic side for mechanical easiness, but they modulated the turning strategy by standing on the healthy external limb at turn initiation. By doing so, they adopted a safe behavior. Of note, chronic RP patients with poor turning performance adopted strategy 4. They may have adopted that risky and natural behavior as they became accustomed to their disability.

Even if stroke location is debated as a risk factor of falling, falls are considered more frequent in LP than RP patients (9). In the present study, falls were more frequent in RP than LP patients (29 and 15%, respectively). This finding could be explained by the declarative fall questionnaire in that patients with right hemisphere lesions tend to minimize fall occurrence.

“Footedness for the mobilization tasks” designates the lower limb that is spontaneously used in asymmetric lower-limb tasks requiring active mobilization (such as kicking a ball) as opposed to stabilization tasks (standing on one foot) (3335). Turning requires mobilization of an initiator limb contralateral to the first stance limb (3335). Previous findings in right-footed subjects show consistency and strong preference for the right foot in mobilization tasks as compared with a weak-foot preference in stabilization tasks in right-lateralized populations (3335). Thus, footedness may affect the spontaneous turning strategy in the choice of the initiator limb (i.e., foot contralateral to the first stance limb) (32). In the present study, all control and poststroke subjects were right-footed for mobilization tasks. Only 33.5% of the control subjects spontaneously choose to turn left with the right limb as initiator. Thus, the coincidence between footedness for mobilization tasks and initiating turning is not found in 66.5% of the subjects turning right with the left limb as initiator, which suggests that other aspects are at stake (32). Recent and chronic LP and recent RP stroke survivors prefer to use the dominant right limb as the initiator limb (contralateral to the first stance limb) with strong consistency regardless of behavioral context (side of spontaneous turn and side of hemiparesis) (33). When the dominant right limb is paretic, then over time (chronically >6 months), the left leg may become dominant and increasingly used as the initiator limb (Figure 4). Bonifer et al showed that for stroke patients, the paretic arm (52, 53), even when dominant before stroke, only assisted the healthy arm after stroke. For the lower limb, we can only infer an analogous trend. However, the difference of proportion in turning strategies between HC and stroke patients implies that mechanical easiness and neglect may also play a determinant role in the choice of turning strategy.

Fall risk is due to multiple factors. Our study proposes considering turning strategies according to the side of the hemiparesis as new indicators to evaluate the risk of fall after stroke. The question of the importance of this aspect among other well-known risk factors [balance impairment (24, 43, 54) with sensitive deficits, motor deficits, and hemineglect (1, 5557)] is of interest for future study. The spontaneous choice of turning and the number of external steps taken do not require the use of IMUs in daily clinical practice. Yet, to identify the first stance limb of the 180° turn, IMUs can give better information with minimal need for space. In this approach, light IMU instrumentation could also become an interesting extension of clinical examination to teach patients secured turning strategies in a rehabilitation setting. Turning strategies evaluated by IMUs can be proposed to provide customized balance rehabilitation programs.

Ethics Statement

This observational study was approved by the local ethics committee (Comité de Protection des Personnes Ile de France II, no. CPP 2014-10-04 RNI). Patients and controls gave their written consent to participate.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization and methodology: AY, SB, P-PV, and DR. Software: RB, LO, and AV. Validation: RB, AV, LO, and DR. Formal analysis: RB, LO, AY, P-PV, AV, and DR. Investigation: RB, AY, DR, LT, and CP. Resources: AY, LT, and CP. Data curation: RB. Writing (original draft preparation): RB, LO, DR, and AY. Writing (review and editing): RB, LO, DR, AY, and P-PV. Visualization: RB and DR. Supervision: AY, P-PV, and DR. Project administration: RB, AY, P-PV, and DR. Funding acquisition: P-PV.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Jacqueline Fagard for assistance with data interpretation. The authors acknowledge Satt Innov Ile de France for financial support.

Abbreviations

IMU, inertial measurement unit; HC, healthy control; LP, left paretic; RP, right paretic; FF, foot flat; ARTF, axial rotation of the trunk during foot flat.

References

1. Nyberg L, Gustafson Y. Patient falls in stroke rehabilitation a challenge to rehabilitation strategies. Stroke (1995) 26(5):838–42. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.26.5.838

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Teasell R, McRae M, Foley N, Bhardwaj A. The incidence and consequences of falls in stroke patients during inpatient rehabilitation: factors associated with high risk. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (2002) 83(3):329–33. doi:10.1053/apmr.2002.29623

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Forster A, Young J. Incidence and consequences of falls due to stroke: a systematic inquiry. BMJ (1995) 311(6997):83–6. doi:10.1136/bmj.311.6997.83

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Mackintosh SF, Hill KD, Dodd KJ, Goldie PA, Culham EG. Balance score and a history of falls in hospital predict recurrent falls in the 6 months following stroke rehabilitation. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (2006) 87(12):1583–9. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2006.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Nyberg L, Gustafson Y. Fall prediction index for patients in stroke rehabilitation. Stroke (1997) 28(4):716–21. doi:10.1161/01.STR.28.4.716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Wada N, Sohmiya M, Shimizu T, Okamoto K, Shirakura K. Clinical analysis of risk factors for falls in home-living stroke patients using functional evaluation tools. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (2007) 88(12):1601–5. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2007.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Ashburn A, Hyndman D, Pickering R, Yardley L, Harris S. Predicting people with stroke at risk of falls. Age Ageing (2008) 37(3):270–6. doi:10.1093/ageing/afn066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Yates JS, Lai SM, Duncan PW, Studenski S. Falls in community-dwelling stroke survivors: an accumulated impairments model. J Rehabil Res Dev (2002) 39(3):385.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

9. Hyndman D, Ashburn A, Stack E. Fall events among people with stroke living in the community: circumstances of falls and characteristics of fallers. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (2002) 83(2):165–70. doi:10.1053/apmr.2002.28030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Batchelor FA, Mackintosh SF, Said CM, Hill KD. Falls after stroke. Int J Stroke (2012) 7(8):482–90. doi:10.1111/j.1747-4949.2012.00796.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Thigpen MT, Light KE, Creel GL, Flynn SM. Turning difficulty characteristics of adults aged 65 years or older. Phys Ther (2000) 80:1174–87. doi:10.1093/ptj/80.12.1174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Hase K, Stein RB. Turning strategies during human walking. J Neurophysiol (1999) 81(6):2914–22.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

13. Taylor MJD, Dabnichki P, Strike SC. A three-dimensional biomechanical comparison between turning strategies during the stance phase of walking. Hum Mov Sci (2005) 24(4):558–73. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2005.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Patla AE, Adkin AL, Ballard T. Online steering: coordination and control of body center of mass, head and body reorientation. Exp Brain Res (1999) 129(4):629–34. doi:10.1007/s002210050932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Hollands M, Sorensen K, Patla A. Effects of head immobilization on the coordination and control of head and body reorientation and translation during steering. Exp Brain Res (2001) 140(2):223–33. doi:10.1007/s002210100811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Hollands MA, Ziavra NV, Bronstein AM. A new paradigm to investigate the roles of head and eye movements in the coordination of whole-body movements. Exp Brain Res (2004) 154(2):261–6. doi:10.1007/s00221-003-1718-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Akram SB, Frank JS, Chenouri S. Turning behavior in healthy older adults: is there a preference for step versus spin turns? Gait Posture (2010) 31:23–6. doi:10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.08.238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Fino P, Lockhart TE. Required coefficient of friction during turning at self-selected slow, normal, and fast walking speeds. J Biomech (2014) 47(6):1395–400. doi:10.1016/j.jbiomech.2014.01.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Fino PC, Frames CW, Lockhart TE. Classifying step and spin turns using wireless gyroscopes and implications for fall risk assessments. Sensors (Basel) (2015) 15:10676–85. doi:10.3390/s150510676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Lamontagne A, Fung J. Gaze and postural reorientation in the control of locomotor steering after stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair (2009) 23(3):256–66. doi:10.1177/1545968308324549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Hollands KL, Van Vliet P, Zietz D, Wing A, Wright C, Hollands MA. Stroke-related differences in axial body segment coordination during preplanned and reactive changes in walking direction. Exp Brain Res (2010) 202(3):591–604. doi:10.1007/s00221-010-2162-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Hollands KL, Hollands MA, Zietz D, Wing AM, Wright C, van Vliet P. Kinematics of turning 180 degrees during the timed up and go in stroke survivors with and without falls history. Neurorehabil Neural Repair (2010) 24(4):358–67. doi:10.1177/1545968309348508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Bonnyaud C, Pradon D, Vuillerme N, Bensmail D, Roche N. Spatiotemporal and kinematic parameters relating to oriented gait and turn performance in patients with chronic stroke. PLoS One (2015) 10(6):e0129821. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0129821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Bonnyaud C, Pradon D, Bensmail D, Roche N. Dynamic stability and risk of tripping during the timed up and go test in hemiparetic and healthy subjects. PLoS One (2015) 10(10):e0140317. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0140317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Hollands KL, Agnihotri D, Tyson SF. Effects of dual task on turning ability in stroke survivors and older adults. Gait Posture (2014) 40(4):564–9. doi:10.1016/j.gaitpost.2014.06.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Mariani B, Rouhani H, Crevoisier X, Aminian K. Quantitative estimation of foot-flat and stance phase of gait using foot-worn inertial sensors. Gait Posture (2013) 37(2):229–34. doi:10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Yang S, Zhang JT, Novak AC, Brouwer B, Li Q. Estimation of spatio-temporal parameters for post-stroke hemiparetic gait using inertial sensors. Gait Posture (2013) 37(3):354–8. doi:10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.07.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Mizuike C, Ohgi S, Morita S. Analysis of stroke patient walking dynamics using a tri-axial accelerometer. Gait Posture (2009) 30(1):60–4. doi:10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.02.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. El-Gohary M, Pearson S, McNames J, Mancini M, Horak F, Mellone S, et al. Continuous monitoring of turning in patients with movement disability. Sensors (Basel) (2013) 14(1):356–69. doi:10.3390/s140100356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Mancini M, El-Gohary M, Pearsonb S, McNames J, Schlueter H. Continuous monitoring of turning in Parkinson’s disease: rehabilitation potential. NeuroRehabilitation (2015) 37(1):3–10. doi:10.3233/NRE-151236

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Coren S, Porac C. The validity and reliability of self-report items for the measurement of lateral preference. Br J Psychol (1978) 69(2):207–11. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1978.tb01649.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Lenoir M, Van Overschelde S, De Rycke M, Musch E. Intrinsic and extrinsic factors of turning preferences in humans. Neurosci Lett (2006) 393:179–83. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2005.09.061

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Hart S, Cabbard C. Examining the mobilizing feature of footedness. Percept Mot Skills (1998) 86:1339–42.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

34. Grouios G, Hatzitaki V, Kollias N, Koidou I. Investigating the stabilising and mobilising features of footedness. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition (2009) 14(4):362–80. doi:10.1080/13576500802434965

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Bacelar AM, Teixeira LA. Footedness across ages: distinction between mobilization and stabilization tasks. Laterality (2015) 20(2):141–53. doi:10.1080/1357650X.2014.924959

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Yelnik A, Bonan I. Clinical tools for assessing balance disorders Séméiologie et évaluation clinique des troubles de l’équilibre. Neurophysiol Clin (2008) 38:439–45. doi:10.1016/j.neucli.2008.09.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Azouvi P, Samuel C, Louis-Dreyfus A, Bernati T, Bartolomeo P, Beis JM, et al. Sensitivity of clinical and behavioural tests of spatial neglect after right hemisphere stroke. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2002) 73(2):160–6. doi:10.1136/jnnp.73.2.160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Sabatini AM. Quaternion-based extended Kalman filter for determining orientation by inertial and magnetic sensing. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng (2006) 53(7):1346–56. doi:10.1109/TBME.2006.875664

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Mancini M, Schlueter H, El-Gohary M, Mattek N, Duncan C, Kaye J, et al. Continuous monitoring of turning mobility and its association to falls and cognitive function: a pilot study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci (2016) 71(8):1–7. doi:10.1093/gerona/glw019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. El-Gohary M, Pearson S, McNames J, Mancini M, Horak F, Mellone S, et al. Continuous monitoring of turning in patients with movement disability. Sensors (Basel) (2014) 14:356–69. doi:10.3390/s140100356

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Watanabe Y. Fear of falling among stroke survivors after discharge from inpatient rehabilitation. Int J Rehabil Res (2004) 28(4):149–52. doi:10.1097/00004356-200506000-00008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Hyndman D, Ashburn A. Stops walking when talking as a predictor of falls in people with stroke living in the community. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2004) 75:994–7. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2003.016014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Belgen B, Beninato M, Sullivan PE, Narielwalla K. The association of balance capacity and falls self-efficacy with history of falling in community-dwelling people with chronic stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (2006) 87(4):554–61. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2005.12.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Lam T, Luttmann K. Turning capacity in ambulatory individuals poststroke. Am J Phys Med Rehabil (2009) 88(11):873–83; quiz 884–6, 946. doi:10.1097/PHM.0b013e3181bc0ddf

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Bovonsunthonchai S, Hiengkaew V, Vachalathiti R, Said CM, Batchelor F. Temporospatial analysis: gait characteristics of young adults and the elderly in turning while walking. Int J Ther Rehabil (2015) 22(3):129–34. doi:10.12968/ijtr.2015.22.3.129

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Toussaint Y, Fagard J. A counterclockwise bias in running. Neurosci Lett (2008) 442(1):59–62. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2008.06.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Lipsitz LA, Jonsson PV, Kelley MM, Koestner JS. Causes and correlates of recurrent falls in ambulatory frail elderly. J Gerontol (1991) 46(4):M114–22. doi:10.1093/geronj/46.4.M114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Fuller JR, Adkin AL, Vallis LA. Strategies used by older adults to change travel direction. Gait Posture (2007) 25(3):393–400. doi:10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Dite W, Temple VA. A clinical test of stepping and change of direction to identify multiple falling older adults. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (2002) 83(11):1566–71. doi:10.1053/apmr.2002.35469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Bolton DAE. The role of the cerebral cortex in postural responses to externally induced perturbations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2015) 57:142–55. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Kwasnica CM. Unilateral neglect syndrome after stroke: theories and management issues. Crit Rev Phys Rehabil Med (2002) 14(1):16. doi:10.1615/CritRevPhysRehabilMed.v14.i1.20

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Bonifer NM, Anderson KM, Arciniegas DB. Constraint-induced movement therapy after stroke: efficacy for patients with minimal upper-extremity motor ability. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (2005) 86:1867–73. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2005.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Bonifer NM, Anderson KM, Arciniegas DB. Constraint-induced therapy for moderate chronic upper extremity impairment after stroke. Brain Inj (2005) 19(5):323–30. doi:10.1080/02699050400004302

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Rabadi MH, Rabadi FM, Peterson M. An analysis of falls occurring in patients with stroke on an acute rehabilitation unit. Rehabil Nurs (2008) 33(3):104–9. doi:10.1002/j.2048-7940.2008.tb00213.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Mackintosh SFH, Hill K, Dodd KJ, Goldie P, Culham E. Falls and injury prevention should be part of every stroke rehabilitation plan. Clin Rehabil (2005) 19(4):441–51. doi:10.1191/0269215505cr796oa

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Olsson E, Löfgren B, Gustafson Y, Nyberg L. Validation of a fall risk index in stroke rehabilitation. J Stroke Cerebrovasc Dis (2005) 14(1):23–8. doi:10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2004.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Czernuszenko A. Risk factors for falls in post-stroke patients treated in a neurorehabilitation ward. Neurol Neurochir Pol (2006) 41(1):28–35.

Google Scholar

Appendix

TABLE A1
www.frontiersin.org

Table A1. Lateral preference questionnaire.

TABLE A2
www.frontiersin.org

Table A2. Characteristics of turning patterns (pattern I = 1; II = 2–4; III ≥ 5 external steps).

TABLE A3
www.frontiersin.org

Table A3. Characteristics of turning strategies in right paretic (RP) patients.

TABLE A4
www.frontiersin.org

Table A4. Characteristics of fallers.

Keywords: stroke, 180° turn, turning strategy, inertial measurement unit, fall

Citation: Barrois RP-M, Ricard D, Oudre L, Tlili L, Provost C, Vienne A, Vidal P-P, Buffat S and Yelnik AP (2017) Observational Study of 180° Turning Strategies Using Inertial Measurement Units and Fall Risk in Poststroke Hemiparetic Patients. Front. Neurol. 8:194. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00194

Received: 14 October 2016; Accepted: 21 April 2017;
Published: 15 May 2017

Edited by:

Ayrton R. Massaro, Hospital Sírio-Libanês, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Anirban Dutta, University at Buffalo, USA
Sheng Li, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, USA

Copyright: © 2017 Barrois, Ricard, Oudre, Tlili, Provost, Vienne, Vidal, Buffat and Yelnik. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alain P. Yelnik, alain.yelnik@aphp.fr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.