Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 10 March 2022
Sec. Behavioral Endocrinology
Volume 16 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2022.809733

Loneliness and Health Indicators in Middle-Aged and Older Females and Males

  • 1Laboratory of Social Cognitive Neuroscience, Psychobiology-IDOCAL, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain
  • 2Department of Psychology and Sociology, Area of Psychobiology, IIS Aragón, University of Zaragoza, Teruel, Spain

Loneliness is a complex and uncomfortable feeling that results from the perception of a lack of desired personal and social ties. Loneliness is accentuated with aging. It has been related to a wide range of objective and subjective health indicators and is a risk factor for morbidity and mortality. One of the proposed underlying mechanisms through which loneliness affects health is the dysregulation of the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal (HPA) axis. However, the relationship between loneliness and cortisol, the main product of the HPA axis, is unclear and requires more research. The aims of this cross-sectional study were to investigate the relationships between loneliness, subjective health, and cortisol indexes, taking the sex into account, and investigate whether the HPA axis mediates the relationship between loneliness and subjective health. For this purpose, 79 participants (between 55 and 75 years old) completed several scales on loneliness, depression, perceived stress, psychological and physical health, and social relationships. Various salivary cortisol measurements were obtained on two consecutive days. The initial results showed that loneliness was related to psychological and physical health in the mixed-sex sample. However, when covariates were introduced, loneliness was only associated with psychological health in males. In addition, the cortisol indexes employed were not related to loneliness and did not mediate the relationship between loneliness and subjective health. Hence, we did not find a relevant role of the HPA axis in the association between loneliness and subjective health. More severe perceptions of loneliness would probably be necessary to detect this role. Overall, these results also show that the expected negative outcomes of loneliness associated with aging can be countered by an active life that can compensate for the natural losses experienced with age or at least delay these negative outcomes. Finally, some sex differences were found, in line with other studies, which warrants further examination of social variables and dimensions related to gender in future research.

Introduction

Humans are essentially social beings. From birth, we need an attachment figure in order to develop, survive, and understand how the social universe and personal ties work (Bowlby, 1973, 1980; Fonagy and Luyten, 2018). During the rest of life, we have the need to be integrated into a social network (Baumeister and Leary, 1995). When this need is not fulfilled, loneliness appears, the painful feeling that accompanies the perception of a lack of desired personal and social relationships (Peplau and Perlman, 1982). Hence, loneliness can be understood as a potent psychosocial stressor (Cacioppo et al., 2003; Hawkley and Cacioppo, 2003). In addition, loneliness increases with age and is usually associated with adverse health outcomes (Luo et al., 2012; Victor and Yang, 2012; Holt-Lunstad et al., 2015; Cohen-Mansfield et al., 2016; Richard et al., 2017).

Thus, loneliness has been related to self-reported psychological and physical health issues, such as poor health and low life satisfaction (Losada et al., 2012; Tomstad et al., 2017), subjective memory complaints (Montejo et al., 2019), lower self-esteem (Wagner et al., 2015), depression (Aylaz et al., 2012; Ge et al., 2017), mobility characteristics (Van Den Berg et al., 2016), sleep disorders (Shankar, 2020), or more doctor visits (Beutel et al., 2017; Richard et al., 2017). In addition, loneliness plays a mediating role between early life stress and perceived stress in adulthood (Crespo-Sanmiguel et al., 2021). These health issues can be explained by the fact that loneliness acts as a stressor, activating the stress response via hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis functioning through the action of cortisol (Hawkley and Cacioppo, 2003; Steptoe et al., 2004; O’Connor et al., 2021). In this regard, a dysregulation of this axis has been associated with different harmful health issues (Fries et al., 2009; Adam et al., 2017).

Several studies have investigated the relationship between loneliness, the cortisol awakening response (CAR), and the diurnal cortisol slope (DCS) in aging, but with inconsistent results. In middle-aged adults, loneliness was found to be associated with CAR (Steptoe et al., 2004; Adam et al., 2006; Okamura et al., 2011), but no differences were found in CAR or DCS based on loneliness in older males and females (Schutter et al., 2017, 2020). However, in older people, a flatter DCS was found in lonely people in comparison with non-lonely individuals (Cole et al., 2007). In a previous study, we found that loneliness was positively associated with bedtime cortisol levels, but not with awakening cortisol or the DCS (Montoliu et al., 2019). As far as we know, our previous study was the first one to study the possible association between loneliness and bedtime cortisol levels. However, we could not test subjective health in relation to loneliness. Thus, the association between loneliness, cortisol indexes, and subjective health requires further research.

Sex differences have been found in some research on loneliness and its effects on subjective health and HPA axis functioning. Specifically, Zebhauser et al. (2014) reported that loneliness had a higher impact on subjective psychological health in males than in females. Furthermore, a dysregulation of the HPA axis, with a diminished CAR and flatter DCS, has been found in lonely married males, but not in their female counterparts (Johar et al., 2021). However, other studies did not find sex differences in the relationship between loneliness and subjective health (Richard et al., 2017), the DCS, or bedtime cortisol (Montoliu et al., 2019).

Given that loneliness can be understood as a stressor, we first aimed to study whether loneliness is associated with subjective psychological and physical health and HPA axis functioning in late middle-aged and early older people (older people henceforth). Second, we also aimed to explore whether these results differ depending on sex. Finally, we investigated whether HPA axis functioning is a mechanism underlying the relationship between loneliness and subjective health. We hypothesized that there would be a negative association between loneliness and subjective psychological and physical health (Beutel et al., 2017; Richard et al., 2017), and we expected to confirm the association between loneliness and cortisol indexes found previously (Montoliu et al., 2019). Finally, despite the heterogeneity in the findings, we expected to find clearer relationships in males than in females.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited from a study program for people over 55 years old at the University of Valencia (Spain). This program, called “La Nau Gran,” is a 3-year education program with basic modules from one of the different official degrees. The students are not issued an official degree, and they do not share the subjects with the regulated degree students. However, this program allows middle-aged and older people who want to learn and continue to grow to access the university as students. Two researchers from the laboratory came to these lessons to offer students the chance to participate in the research. Information on the type of data collected, the duration of the session, and the location of the laboratory was provided. Interested students provided their contact information. Participants who were not excluded, based on the exclusion criteria in a telephone interview, were given an appointment to attend an individual session at the Laboratory of Social Cognitive Neuroscience, of the University of Valencia.

Exclusion criteria were: being outside the age range from 55 to 75 years old; having diseases and disorders that interfere with daily wellbeing, such as endocrine (e.g., Type II diabetes), neurological (e.g., epilepsy), psychiatric (e.g., personality and psychotic disorders or depression), or other diseases (cancer, cerebrovascular diseases or chronic pain); using a medication such as glucocorticoids, anxiolytics, antidepressants, or other medications that can interfere directly with emotional, endocrinological, or cognitive functioning; having been under general anesthesia in the past 12 months; smoking more than 10 cigarettes a day, alcohol or other drug abuse; and the presence of a stressful life event during the past year, such as the death of the spouse, the appearance of a major disease, or any other event that had affected them in a significant way.

Procedure

The current study follows an observational and cross-sectional design. Experimental sessions, which lasted approximately 1 h, had three different schedules (at 10 a.m, 12 p.m, and 4 p.m). Both the schedule and the sex of the participants were counterbalanced, so that the number of participants and the number of males and females who attended each session were similar. During the session, a general questionnaire about sociodemographic information and questionnaires about loneliness, subjective health, social relationships, depressive symptomatology, and stress perception were filled out. Furthermore, weight and height measurements were taken to calculate the body mass index (BMI). The experimenter explained verbally to the participants how and when they had to collect salivary cortisol samples at home. In addition, written instructions were given to participants, attached to a diary where they could provide the collection times of the salivary samples. Within 3 days after the session, participants returned to the laboratory to bring the samples. The protocol was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the University of Valencia (Code: 1034878) and written according to the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants read and signed the informed consent. The period of the data collection was between January 2018 and January 2020.

Questionnaires

Socioeconomic Status

We assessed the socioeconomic status with the nine-rung social ladder (Adler and Stewart, 2007). A ladder with nine rungs is presented by explaining that the highest rungs would contain the people in our country with the highest standing, that is, with a lot of money, a good education, and the best jobs. In contrast, the lowest rungs would contain the poorest people with less education and worse jobs or no job. Participants are asked the following question: Where would you place yourself on this ladder?

Loneliness

We used the Spanish adaptation (Vázquez Morejón and Jiménez García-Bóveda, 1994) of the revised UCLA loneliness scale (R-UCLA) (Russell et al., 1980) to assess loneliness. This scale contains 20 items rated on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 4 (often), obtaining a total score ranging from 20 (low) to 80 (high). The Cronbach’s α in our study was 0.887.

Subjective Psychological and Physical Health and Social Relationships

We used the three domains of the Spanish version of the World Health Organization Quality of Life Short Form Survey (Carrasco, 1998), created by the WHOQOL Group (1998), to assess the perception of: (i) psychological health (e.g., self-esteem, positive and negative feelings, or capacity for concentration); (ii) physical health (e.g., mobility, activities of daily living, sleep and rest, or pain and discomfort); and (iii) social relationships (e.g., social support or sexual activity). The subscales have 6, 7, and 3 items, respectively, with a 5-point Likert response scale ranging from 1 to 5. The scores were transformed to a scale from 0 to 100, where higher scores represent perceptions of better health. The internal consistency (Cronbach’s α) for the psychological health scale was 0.763, for the physical health scale 0.623, and for the social relationships scale 0.708.

Depressive Symptomatology

We used the Spanish version (Sanz et al., 2003) of the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II; Beck et al., 1996) to assess depressive symptomatology. This test consists of 21 items, with a response scale ranging from 0 to 3, that evaluate the symptoms of depression (emotional, cognitive, somatic, and motivational) in the past month. Scores range from 0 to 63, where higher scores are interpreted as higher symptomatology. The internal consistency (Cronbach’s α) of this scale was 0.868.

Perceived Stress

The degree to which people perceived their lives as stressful and overloaded was evaluated using the Spanish version (Remor, 2006) of the 14-item Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-14; Cohen et al., 1983). Participants had to answer using a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (never) to 4 (very often), and total scores range from 0 to 56, with higher scores indicating higher stress perception. The evaluation refers to the past month. The internal consistency (Cronbach’s α) of this scale in this study was 0.801.

Cortisol Measurements

Ten salivary samples were collected to assess participants’ diurnal cortisol levels using Salivettes (Sarstedt, Rommelsdorf, Germany). The saliva samples were collected by participants immediately, + 15, + 30 and + 45 min after awakening and before sleep on two consecutive days in their natural environment and without disturbing their usual daily activities. Participants were instructed to keep the cotton in their mouth for exactly 2 min and then store it in the refrigerator until they took it to the laboratory. They returned the samples as soon as possible, with 3 days after their collection being the maximum time. Once in the laboratory, the saliva samples were centrifuged for 15 min at 4,000 rpm, resulting in a clear supernatant of low viscosity that was stored at −80°C until analyses were performed. The ELISA kit from Salimetrics (Newmarket, United Kingdom) was used to determine the cortisol concentrations.

For each participant, all samples were measured in duplicate and analyzed in the same trial. The assay sensitivity and the inter- and intra- assay variation coefficients of raw densities were below 10%. Salivary cortisol levels were determined in the Laboratory of Social Cognitive Neuroscience (Valencia, Spain).

Statistical Analyses

Because the cortisol levels did not follow a normal distribution, they were log transformed. We used three cortisol indexes: (i) the CAR, a dynamic measure of post-awakening cortisol secretion, calculated from cortisol samples taken 0, + 15, + 30, and + 45 min after awakening following the trapezoidal formula for the area under the curve with respect to the increase (AUCi; see Pruessner et al., 2003); (ii) the DCS, which is calculated as awakening cortisol minus bedtime cortisol and reflects the diurnal decline in cortisol levels; and (iii) bedtime cortisol, which reflects cortisol levels immediately before going to sleep. To calculate each cortisol index, the mean for both days was used, and for participants who had missing cortisol data from one of the 2 days (CAR: n = 24, DCS: n = 7, bedtime: n = 6), we used the data from the day they were available. To study the effect of CAR compliance, we reran the analysis, excluding 12 participants (15.19%) who were outside the recommended strict time window, that is, with a 5-min delay (Stalder et al., 2016). These results are reported as Supplementary Materials.

To evaluate sex differences, Student’s t-tests for independent samples were performed for age, socioeconomic status, BMI, loneliness, psychological and physical health, cortisol indexes, social relationships, depressive symptomatology, perceived stress, and hours spent with their children. In addition, X2 were performed for educational level, marital status, and number of children, and Z analysis was performed for the statistically significant results of X2. To investigate whether loneliness was related to subjective psychological and physical health and HPA axis functioning, linear regression analyses were performed. In these analyses, loneliness was the independent variable, and subjective psychological and physical health and the cortisol indexes were the dependent variables. Additionally, we tested whether these relationships varied depending on sex by performing moderation analyses. Thus, loneliness was the independent variable, sex was the moderator variable, and subjective health (psychological and physical) and endocrine indicators (cortisol indexes) were the dependent variables. Both linear regression and moderation analyses were performed separately for each dependent variable. Finally, we tested whether the HPA axis mediates the relationship between loneliness and subjective psychological and physical health by performing mediation analyses. Thus, loneliness was the independent variable, the cortisol indexes were the mediators, and the two types of subjective health (psychological and physical) were the dependent variables.

Because loneliness has been widely related to depressive symptomatology (Erzen and Çikrikci, 2018) and most of the studies on loneliness and cortisol control depression in the analyses (Schutter et al., 2017; Montoliu et al., 2019; Johar et al., 2021), we included depressive symptomatology as covariate in the regression, moderation, and mediation analyses. Moreover, for the regression, moderation, and mediation analyses that include the CAR, we used time of awakening and cortisol levels immediately after awakening as covariates, as in Stalder et al. (2016). Furthermore, Pearson’s correlations were performed between the sociodemographic variables and loneliness, subjective psychological and physical health, and the cortisol indexes. The sociodemographic variables that were significantly related to these factors were used as covariates in the main analyses (regression, moderation, and mediation analyses). Thus, in all the main analyses, in addition to depressive symptomatology, age was controlled because it was positively related to loneliness in our sample. Likewise, socioeconomic status was a covariate in the main analyses that included subjective physical health, due to their relationship. BMI was a covariate in the main analyses that included bedtime cortisol, due to their relationship. In addition, due to the sex differences in marital status and in BMI, these variables were included as covariates in all the moderation analyses.

Multivariate outliers were considered those that deviated from the mean (± 3 SD), and standardized residuals were used to detect them. Specifically, there was an outlier in the main analyses that included the CAR. No collinearity issues were detected for the variables included in the main analyses, indicated by tolerance values > 0.1. One piece of data was missing for BMI and for level of studies, three for CAR and DCS, two for bedtime cortisol, thirteen for the time of awakening, and five for the hours per week spent with their children. Consequently, the number of participants in the different analyses varies.

We estimated a sample size of N = 55 to obtain a medium effect size (f2 = 0.15, α = 0.05 and power = 0.80), calculated using the G*Power (Faul et al., 2007). Thus, our sample size (N = 79) is adequate because in the recruitment we anticipated possible missing data. The bootstrap technique used for moderation and mediation analysis uses the original sample size as a miniature representation that is randomly replaced and resampled, which increases the statistical power and solves the problem of having a relatively small sample (Hayes, 2017).

To carry out all the statistical analyses, version 25.0 of SPSS was used. All p-values were two-tailed, and the level of significance was taken as p < 0.05. To test moderated and mediated regression effects, we used PROCESS 3.4 for SPSS (Model 1) and Z scores. PROCESS uses bootstrapped bias-corrected 95% confidence intervals with 5,000 bootstrapped samples in order to determine the significance of the interaction effect in the moderation analysis and the significance of the indirect effect in the mediation analysis. When the confidence interval for the interaction effect (moderation analysis) or the indirect effect (mediation analysis) did not include zero, it was interpreted that there was a significant interaction/indirect effect (Hayes, 2017).

Results

Descriptive Analyses

The sessions contained 82 participants, but three participants were eliminated due to missing data on the loneliness, depression, or health questionnaires. The final sample was composed of 79 participants (39 males, 40 females) with ages ranging from 55 to 75 years old (M = 64.481, SD = 5.563). Participants reported a medium-high socioeconomic status, medium-high levels of satisfaction with their social relationships, and low levels of depressive symptomatology and perceived stress. More than half the participants (57.7%) had university studies, and 52 (65.8%) were married. All the females were post-menopausal. There were no sex differences in loneliness [t(77) = 1.273, p = 0.207], subjective psychological [t(77) = 1.042, p = 0.301] or physical health [t(77) = −0.898, p = 0.372], or the cortisol indexes [CAR: t(64) = −0.041, p = 0.968; DCS: t(74) = 0.094, p = 926; bedtime: t(75) = 0.600, p = 0.551]. Significant differences were only found in BMI [t(76) = 3.999 p < 0.001] and marital status [X2(4) = 9.691, p = 0.046], with a higher proportion of married males compared to married females and of widowed females compared to widowed males. Sample characteristics are described in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of the total sample and for males and females.

Pearson’s Correlation Analyses

In our sample, loneliness increased with age (p = 0.001) and was negatively related to subjective psychological health (p = 0.011) and physical health (p = 0.038), but it was not significantly associated with the CAR (p = 0.710), DCS (p = 0.433), and bedtime (p = 0.950) cortisol indexes. Moreover, loneliness was negatively related to satisfaction with their relationships (p < 0.001) and to the hours per week they spent with their children (p = 0.006). However, loneliness was not significantly related to depressive symptomatology (p = 0.096) (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Pearson’s correlations.

Adjusted Regression Analyses

Results of adjusted regression analyses confirmed the significant negative relationship between loneliness and subjective psychological health (p = 0.034). However, when covariates were included, the relationship between loneliness and subjective physical health became non-significant (p = 0.167).1 The relationship between loneliness and the cortisol indexes was not significant either (all p > 0.291) (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Adjusted regression analyses with loneliness as a predictor and the psychological and physical health and cortisol indexes as dependent variables.

Moderation Analyses

Results of the moderation analyses showed a significant interaction effect of sex in the relationship between loneliness and subjective psychological health (p < 0.001). Thus, loneliness was negatively related to subjective psychological health in males (p < 0.001), but not in females (p = 0.263). No interaction effect of sex was found in the relationships between loneliness and subjective physical health (p = 0.697). Additionally, sex did not moderate the relationship between loneliness and the cortisol indexes (all p > 0.274) (Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Adjusted moderation analyses with loneliness as a predictor and psychological and physical health and cortisol indexes as dependent variables in males and females.

Mediation Analyses

Mediation analyses revealed that the cortisol indexes did not mediate the relationship between loneliness and subjective health. Regarding the analyses with subjective psychological health as dependent variable, the indirect effect (i.e., effect of loneliness on subjective psychological health via cortisol indexes) was not significant for any cortisol indexes: CAR (IC 95% [−0.025, 0.076]), DCS (IC 95% [−0.026, 0.058]), and bedtime (IC 95% [−0.047, 0.030]) (Table 5). In the analysis with subjective physical health as dependent variable, the indirect effect (i.e., effect of loneliness on subjective physical health via cortisol indexes) was not significant for any of the cortisol indexes: CAR (IC 95% [−0.082, 0.016]), DCS (IC 95% [−0.038, 0.038]), and bedtime (IC 95% [−0.023, 0.050]) (Table 6).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Adjusted mediation models of the relationship between loneliness as predictor and psychological health as dependent variables via cortisol indexes (CAR, DCS, and bedtime).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Adjusted mediation models of the relationship between loneliness as predictor and physical health as dependent variables via cortisol indexes (CAR, DCS, and bedtime).

Discussion

The aims of this study were, first, to test whether loneliness was related to subjective psychological and physical health indicators and HPA axis functioning (CAR, DCS, and bedtime cortisol) and, second, to analyze the role of sex in these relationships. Finally, we investigated whether HPA axis functioning was a mediator in the relationship between loneliness and subjective health. In the total sample, loneliness was correlated with psychological and physical health, but when these relationships were analyzed in more detail (including the pertinent covariates), loneliness did not appear to be associated with subjective physical health or the cortisol indexes, and the only relationship that remained was between loneliness and psychological health in males, but not in females.

Males with higher loneliness scores showed lower subjective psychological health. This finding is in line with a previous study showing that males tend to experience greater effects of loneliness on mental health (Zebhauser et al., 2014). These authors, based on Stevens (1995), proposed that the sex differences could be due to the fact that females have more settings where they can obtain social support, whereas males seek more social contact in the public spheres of organizations, where it is more difficult to find close personal contacts. However, this explanation is not supported by our results because there are no sex differences in satisfaction with social relationships. Although there are no sex differences in loneliness in previous literature (Maes et al., 2019) or in our sample, the association between loneliness and subjective psychological health could be explained by sex differences in experiences in interdependent relationships. It has been observed that females focus more on intimate and dyadic attachments (Baumeister and Sommer, 1997; Gardner and Gabriel, 2004) in which they can share their most personal and intimate experiences and, thus, strengthen their psychological wellbeing. In contrast, males focus more on the group (Hoza et al., 2000), where there is no space to share concerns. These differences are often related to masculinity (ideals and rules about what it means to be a man), manifested as difficulty in expressing their needs (Wide et al., 2011), a tendency to solve problems independently (Roy et al., 2017), and being less likely to request psychological support through services (Ogrodniczuk et al., 2016). These traits could influence the way loneliness affects older people differently depending on their sex. Therefore, males would benefit less from their relationships in terms of mental health. However, more research is needed to understand the underlying mechanisms that associate loneliness with psychological health in males but not in females.

Previous studies found that lonely people were more likely to report factors related to poor physical health, such as visits to medical doctors, more chronic diseases, or poor subjective health, among others (Richard et al., 2017). In our sample, loneliness was associated with subjective physical health, although the inclusion of depressive symptomatology as a covariate made the relationship non-significant. This could be explained by the fact that depressive symptomatology can aggravate the way people perceive their health (Wells et al., 1989; Gaynes et al., 2002). In line with this, depressive symptomatology is associated with more difficulty sleeping (Koffel and Watson, 2009) and a wide variety of somatic complaints, which also share biological pathways and neurotransmitters with depression (Bair et al., 2003). Our results are not consistent with Richard et al. (2017), who found an association between loneliness and self-reported physical health. However, Richard et al. (2017) included participants with non-specified chronic illnesses, and this worse state of health may have corresponded to lower subjective physical health (Idler and Benyamini, 1997; Benyamini and Idler, 1999; Benyamini et al., 2014; Elran-Barak et al., 2019). In the current study, due to the restrictive exclusion criteria followed, participants did not have any important diseases that seriously interfered with their wellbeing, and they did not take any medications that could indicate an initial phase of illness. These characteristics could explain the non-relationship found between loneliness and subjective physical health.

Regarding the lack of association between loneliness and the HPA axis indexes, this result agrees with a previous study that did not find a relationship between loneliness and CAR (Schutter et al., 2017), although Johar et al. (2021) found a diminished CAR in lonely married males. However, in this latter study, the relationship between CAR and loneliness in males was no longer significant when adjusting for sociodemographic covariates, depression, or awakening time. Therefore, our results are in line with those of Johar et al. (2021), suggesting that loneliness and CAR are not associated. Furthermore, although in the regressions separated by sex, the relationship was significant for males, the sex interaction of the relationship between loneliness and CAR was not significant.

We also failed to find a relationship between loneliness and DCS. This result agrees with other previous studies (Schutter et al., 2017, 2020; Montoliu et al., 2019), although it contrasts with studies that found a flattened DCS in married participants (Johar et al., 2021) and in a selected sample with extremely high loneliness scores (Cole et al., 2007). In both of these studies, the sample was composed of people who could suffer from alcohol abuse, smoking, or diabetes, which can influence the DCS (Adam et al., 2017). These health issues alone could contribute to the dysregulation of the HPA axis functioning and skew the association between loneliness and cortisol patterns. In addition, in Cole et al. (2007), although the saliva samples were collected reliably (on three consecutive days and at three different time points due to the longitudinal study design), the procedure used to classify the groups and the small number of participants in each group could explain the disparity in the results. Specifically, the groups were composed of 14 participants who consistently scored in the top 15% of the loneliness distribution throughout the study (high-lonely group; N = 6) and in the bottom 15% (low-lonely; N = 8). Thus, the analyses of group differences were performed with extreme scores and small samples.

Regarding the lack of relationship between loneliness and bedtime cortisol, the current results did not confirm our previous study (Montoliu et al., 2019), which found a positive association between them. Although it is true that participants in both studies showed similar loneliness scores, there were protective factors that we added in the current study and had not tested before. The characteristics of our participants represent optimal aging: no chronic diseases, a high socioeconomic level, low depressive symptomatology, low perceived stress, children they see frequently, and high satisfaction with social relationships. These circumstances could be acting as a buffer against stressors (Hawkley et al., 2008) such as loneliness and its endocrine effects, or they could even keep feelings of loneliness from appearing (Teater et al., 2021).

Finally, the results of the mediation analyses showed that the direct relationship between loneliness and psychological health was negative, confirming the regression analyses. However, the indirect effect that indicates whether HPA axis functioning is an underlying mechanism between loneliness and health was not found, contrary to what other authors suggested (Hawkley and Cacioppo, 2003; Steptoe et al., 2004). Considering all this, it is worth noting that loneliness is an experience with potentially adverse effects on psychological health, although more research is needed on what factors could influence the way loneliness affects subjective health in older people without severe perceptions of loneliness.

Some limitations should be considered when interpreting the results of this study. First, the cross-sectional design of the study makes it impossible to draw conclusions about causal relationships. Second, the internal consistency of the physical health scale shows low values, and so the results that include this scale should be confirmed in further studies. Moreover, to control possible confounders, this study had restrictive exclusion criteria, and only participants who were in good general health were included. Our participants have healthy characteristics and behaviors, and perhaps due to this, in our study loneliness was not related to the perception of physical health or to HPA axis indicators. Therefore, in future studies, these associations could be tested using longitudinal designs, other middle and older age ranges, and people with greater loneliness, for example, due to social circumstances or chronic diseases, such as older people with diabetes who report higher feelings of loneliness (Hackett et al., 2020).

Our findings support the view that loneliness is not associated with HPA dysregulation, but it is associated with subjective health, specifically psychological health in males. Subjective health is an important health measure because it predicts the evolution of health and life expectancy as well as or even better than objective health examinations (Miilunpalo et al., 1997; Helmer et al., 1999). Finally, males appeared to be more vulnerable to loneliness because loneliness was related to their subjective health, and so they could benefit from prevention procedures and follow-ups to avoid more severe psychological difficulties.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the University of Valencia (Code: 1034878). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

IC-S participated in the data acquisition, managed the literature search, the statistical analyses, and with AS and VH interpreted the results, revised the literature, and wrote the manuscript. MZ-F and RG-C contributed to the data acquisition and management. All the authors contributed to and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by the Spanish Science, Innovation and Universities Ministry (PID2020-119406GB- I00/AEI/10.13039/501100011033, the FPU 17/03428), and Grant BES-2017-082148 funded by MCIN/AEI/10.13039/501100011033 and by “ESF Investing in your future.” The contribution of VH has been supported by the Government of Aragón (S31_20D) and the Universities Ministry and European Union (European Union-NextGenerationEU, grant for the requalification of the Spanish University System).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ To study in more detail which covariate is influencing the change in the significance of the relationship between loneliness and physical health, we performed a stepwise regression including the covariates (age, socioeconomic status, and depressive symptomatology). Depressive symptomatology was the only covariate that remained in the model, and so it is the one that is modifying the statistical conclusion.

References

Adam, E. K., Hawkley, L. C., Kudielka, B. M., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2006). Day-to-day dynamics of experience–cortisol associations in a population-based sample of older adults. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 103, 17058–17063. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605053103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adam, E. K., Quinn, M. E., Tavernier, R., McQuillan, M. T., Dahlke, K. A., and Gilbert, K. E. (2017). Diurnal cortisol slopes and mental and physical health outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 83, 25–41. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adler, N., and Stewart, J. (2007). The MacArthur Scale of Subjective Social Status. San Francisco, CA: MacArthur Research Network on SES & Health.

Google Scholar

Aylaz, R., Aktürk, Ü., Erci, B., Öztürk, H., and Aslan, H. (2012). Relationship between depression and loneliness in elderly and examination of influential factors. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 55, 548–554. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2012.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bair, M. J., Robinson, R. L., Katon, W., and Kroenke, K. (2003). Depression and pain comorbidity: a literature review. Arch. Intern. Med. 163, 2433–2445. doi: 10.1001/archinte.163.20.2433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., and Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol. Bull. 117, 497–529.

Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., and Sommer, K. L. (1997). What do men want? Gender differences and two spheres of belongingness: comment on Cross and Madson. Psychol. Bull. 122, 38–55. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.122.1.38

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., and Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Association.

Google Scholar

Benyamini, Y., and Idler, E. L. (1999). Community studies reporting association between self-rated health and mortality: additional studies, 1995 to 1998. Res. Aging 21, 392–401.

Google Scholar

Benyamini, Y., Boyko, V., Blumstein, T., and Lerner-Geva, L. (2014). Health, cultural and socioeconomic factors related to self-rated health of long-term Jewish residents, immigrants, and Arab women in midlife in Israel. Women Health 54,, 402–424. doi: 10.1080/03630242.2014.897679

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beutel, M. E., Klein, E. M., Brähler, E., Reiner, I., Jünger, C., Michal, M., and Tibubos, A. N. (2017). Loneliness in the general population: prevalence, determinants and relations to mental health. BMC Psychiatry 17:97. doi: 10.1186/s12888-017-1262-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and Loss: Separation, Anxiety and Anger. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and Loss: Vol. III. Loss: Sadness and depression. London:Hogarth.

Google Scholar

Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., and Berntson, G. G. (2003). The anatomy of loneliness. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 12, 71–74. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.01232

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrasco, R. L. (1998). Versión Española del WHOQOL. Jackson, MS: Ergón.

Google Scholar

Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., and Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. J. Health Soc. Behav. 24, 385–396.

Google Scholar

Cohen-Mansfield, J., Hazan, H., Lerman, Y., and Shalom, V. (2016). Correlates and predictors of loneliness in older-adults: a review of quantitative results informed by qualitative insights. Int. Psychogeriatr. 28, 557–576. doi: 10.1017/S1041610215001532

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, S. W., Hawkley, L. C., Arevalo, J. M., Sung, C. Y., Rose, R. M., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). Social regulation of gene expression in human leukocytes. Genome Biol. 8:R189. doi: 10.1186/gb-2007-8-9-r189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crespo-Sanmiguel, I., Zapater-Fajarí, M., Pulopulos, M. M., Hidalgo, V., and Salvador, A. (2021). Loneliness mediates the relationship between early life stress and perceived stress but not hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis functioning. Front. Psychol. 12: 647265. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.647265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elran-Barak, R., Weinstein, G., Beeri, M. S., and Ravona-Springer, R. (2019). The associations between objective and subjective health among older adults with type 2 diabetes: the moderating role of personality. J. Psychosom. Res. 117, 41–47. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2018.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erzen, E., and Çikrikci, Ö. (2018). The effect of loneliness on depression: a meta-analysis. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 64, 427–435. doi: 10.1177/0020764018776349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., and Luyten, P. (2018). Handbook of personality disorders: theory, research, and treatment. In W. J. Livesley and R. Larstone (Eds.), Attachment, Mentalizing, and The Self (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 123–140. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2005.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fries, E., Dettenborn, L., and Kirschbaum, C. (2009). The cortisol awakening response (CAR): facts and future directions. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 72, 67–73. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardner, W. L., and Gabriel, S. (2004). “Gender differences in relational and collective interdependence: implications for self-views, social behavior, and subjective well-being” in A. H. Eagly, A. E. Beall, and R. J. Sternberg eds The Psychology of Gender (2nd Edn). New York, NY: Guilford Press, 169–191.

Google Scholar

Gaynes, B. N., Burns, B. J., Tweed, D. L., and Erickson, P. (2002). Depression and health-related quality of life. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 190, 799–806. doi: 10.1097/00005053-200212000-00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ge, L., Yap, C. W., Ong, R., and Heng, B. H. (2017). Social isolation, loneliness and their relationships with depressive symptoms: a population-based study. PLoS One 12:e0182145. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0182145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hackett, R. A., Hudson, J. L., and Chilcot, J. (2020). Loneliness and type 2 diabetes incidence: findings from the English longitudinal study of ageing. Diabetologia 63, 2329–2338. doi: 10.1007/s00125-020-05258-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawkley, L. C., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2003). Loneliness and pathways to disease. Brain Behav. Immun. 17, 98–105. doi: 10.1016/s0889-1591(02)00073-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawkley, L. C., Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Masi, C. M., Thisted, R. A., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). From social structural factors to perceptions of relationship quality and loneliness: the Chicago health, aging, and social relations study. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 63, S375–S384. doi: 10.1093/geronb/63.6.s375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. New York, NY: Guilford publications.

Google Scholar

Helmer, C., Barberger-Gateau, P., Letenneur, L., and Dartigues, J. F. (1999). Subjective health and mortality in French elderly women and men. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 54, S84–S92. doi: 10.1093/geronb/54b.2.s84

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., Baker, M., Harris, T., and Stephenson, D. (2015). Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10, 227–237. doi: 10.1177/1745691614568352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoza, B., Bukowski, W. M., and Beery, S. (2000). Assessing peer network and dyadic loneliness. J. Clin. Child Psychol. 29, 119–128. doi: 10.1207/S15374424jccp2901_12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Idler, E. L., and Benyamini, Y. (1997). Self-rated health and mortality: a review of twenty-seven community studies. J. Health Soc. Behav. 38, 21–37. doi: 10.2307/2955359

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johar, H., Atasoy, S., Bidlingmaier, M., Henningsen, P., and Ladwig, K. H. (2021). Married but lonely. Impact of poor marital quality on diurnal cortisol patterns in older people: findings from the cross-sectional KORA-Age study. Stress 24, 36–43. doi: 10.1080/10253890.2020.1741544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koffel, E., and Watson, D. (2009). The two-factor structure of sleep complaints and its relation to depression and anxiety. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 118, 183–194. doi: 10.1037/a0013945

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Losada, A., Márquez-González, M., García-Ortiz, L., Gómez-Marcos, M. A., Fernández-Fernández, V., and Rodríguez-Sánchez, E. (2012). Loneliness and mental health in a representative sample of community-dwelling Spanish older adults. J. Psychol. 146, 277–292. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2011.582523

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, Y., Hawkley, L. C., Waite, L. J., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2012). Loneliness, health, and mortality in old age: a national longitudinal study. Soc. Sci. Med. 74, 907–914. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.11.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maes, M., Qualter, P., Vanhalst, J., Van den Noortgate, W., and Goossens, L. (2019). Gender differences in loneliness across the lifespan: a meta–analysis. Eur. J. Pers. 33, 642–654. doi: 10.1002/per.2220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miilunpalo, S., Vuori, I., Oja, P., Pasanen, M., and Urponen, H. (1997). Self-rated health status as a health measure: the predictive value of self-reported health status on the use of physician services and on mortality in the working-age population. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 50, 517–528. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(97)00045-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montejo, P, Montenegro-Pe, M, Prada, D, García-Mulero, E, García-Marín, A, and Pedrero-Pérez, EJ. (2019). Quejas de memoria: salud mental, enfermedades, dolor y soledad. Estudio poblacional en la Ciudad de Madrid. Rev. Neurol. 69, 481–491. doi: 10.33588/rn.6912.2019252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montoliu, T., Hidalgo, V., and Salvador, A. (2019). The relationship between loneliness and cognition in healthy older men and women: the role of cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology 107, 270–279. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.05.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connor, D. B., Thayer, J. F., and Vedhara, K. (2021). Stress and health: a review of psychobiological processes. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 72, 663–688. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-062520-122331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogrodniczuk, J., Oliffe, J., Kuhl, D., and Gross, P. A. (2016). Men’s mental health: spaces and places that work for men. Can. Fam. Physician 62, 463–464.

Google Scholar

Okamura, H., Tsuda, A., and Matsuishi, T. (2011). The relationship between perceived loneliness and cortisol awakening responses on work days and weekends. Japanese Psychol. Res. 53, 113–120. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-5884.2011.00459.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peplau, L. A., and Perlman, D. (1982). “Theoretical approaches to loneliness,” in Loneliness: A Sourcebook of Current Theory, Research and Therapy, ed. L. A. Peplau (New York, NY: Wiley-Interscience), 123–134.

Google Scholar

Pruessner, J. C., Kirschbaum, C., Meinlschmid, G., and Hellhammer, D. H. (2003). Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology 28, 916–931. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(02)00108-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Remor, E. (2006). Psychometric properties of a European Spanish version of the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS). Span. J. Psychol. 9, 86–93. doi: 10.1017/s1138741600006004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richard, A., Rohrmann, S., Vandeleur, C. L., Schmid, M., Barth, J., and Eichholzer, M. (2017). Loneliness is adversely associated with physical and mental health and lifestyle factors: results from a Swiss national survey. PLoS One 12:e0181442. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0181442

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roy, P., Tremblay, G., Robertson, S., and Houle, J. (2017). “Do it All by Myself”: a salutogenic approach of masculine health practice among farming men coping with stress. Am. J. Mens Health 11, 1536–1546. doi: 10.1177/1557988315619677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., and Cutrona, C. E. (1980). The revised UCLA loneliness scale: concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 39, 472–480. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.39.3.472

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanz, J., Perdigón, A. L., and Vázquez, C. (2003). Adaptación española del Inventario para la Depresión de Beck-II (BDI-II): 2. Propiedades psicométricas en población general. Clín. Salud 14, 249–280.

Google Scholar

Schutter, N., Holwerda, T. J., Stek, M. L., Dekker, J. J. M., Rhebergen, D., and Comijs, H. C. (2017). Loneliness in older adults is associated with diminished cortisol output. J. Psychosom. Res. 95, 19–25. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2017.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schutter, N., Holwerda, T. J., Comijs, H. C., Naarding, P., Van, R., Dekker, J., et al. (2020). Loneliness, social network size, and mortality in older adults and the role of cortisol. Aging Ment. Health 25, 2246–2254. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2020.1843001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shankar, A. (2020). Loneliness and sleep in older adults. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 55, 269–272. doi: 10.1007/s00127-019-01805-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stalder, T., Kirschbaum, C., Kudielka, B. M., Adam, E. K., Pruessner, J. C., Wüst, S., et al. (2016). Assessment of the cortisol awakening response: expert consensus guidelines. Psychoneuroendocrinology 63, 414–432. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steptoe, A., Owen, N., Kunz-Ebrecht, S. R., and Brydon, L. (2004). Loneliness and neuroendocrine, cardiovascular, and inflammatory stress responses in middle-aged men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29, 593–611. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(03)00086-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, N. (1995). Gender and adaptation to widowhood in later life. Ageing Soc. 15, 37–58. doi: 10.1017/s0144686x00002117

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teater, B., Chonody, J. M., and Davis, N. (2021). Risk and protective factors of loneliness among older adults: the significance of social isolation and quality and type of contact. Soc. Work Public Health 36, 128–141. doi: 10.1080/19371918.2020.1866140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomstad, S., Dale, B., Sundsli, K., Sævareid, H. I., and Söderhamn, U. (2017). Who often feels lonely? A cross-sectional study about loneliness and its related factors among older home-dwelling people. Int. J. Older People Nurs. 12:e12162. doi: 10.1111/opn.12162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Den Berg, P., Kemperman, A., De Kleijn, B., and Borgers, A. (2016). Ageing and loneliness: the role of mobility and the built environment. Travel Behav. Soc. 5, 48–55. doi: 10.1016/j.tbs.2015.03.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vázquez Morejón, A.J., and Jiménez García-Bóveda, R. (1994). RULS: escala de soledad UCLA revisada. Fiabilidad y validez de una versión española. Rev. Psicol. Salud 6, 45–54.

Google Scholar

Victor, C. R., and Yang, K. (2012). The prevalence of loneliness among adults: a case study of the United Kingdom. J. psychol. 146, 85–104. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2011.613875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, J., Hoppmann, C., Ram, N., and Gerstorf, D. (2015). Self-esteem is relatively stable late in life: the role of resources in the health, self-regulation, and social domains. Dev. Psychol. 51, 136–149. doi: 10.1037/a0038338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wells, K. B., Stewart, A., Hays, R. D., Burnam, M. A., Rogers, W., Daniels, M., et al. (1989). The functioning and well-being of depressed patients. results from the medical outcomes study. JAMA 262, 914–919.

Google Scholar

WHOQOL Group (1998). Development of the World Health Organization WHOQOL-BREF quality of life assessment. Psychol. Med. 28, 551–558. doi: 10.1017/s0033291798006667

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wide, J., Mok, H., McKenna, M., and Ogrodniczuk, J. S. (2011). Effect of gender socialization on the presentation of depression among men: a pilot study. Can. Fam. Physician 57, e74–e78.

Google Scholar

Zebhauser, A., Hofmann-Xu, L., Baumert, J., Häfner, S., Lacruz, M. E., Emeny, R. T., et al. (2014). How much does it hurt to be lonely? Mental and physical differences between older men and women in the KORA-Age Study. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 29, 245–252. doi: 10.1002/gps.3998

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: loneliness, subjective health, stress, HPA axis functioning, cortisol, aging

Citation: Crespo-Sanmiguel I, Zapater-Fajarí M, Garrido-Chaves R, Hidalgo V and Salvador A (2022) Loneliness and Health Indicators in Middle-Aged and Older Females and Males. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16:809733. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.809733

Received: 05 November 2021; Accepted: 18 February 2022;
Published: 10 March 2022.

Edited by:

César Venero, National University of Distance Education (UNED), Spain

Reviewed by:

Hamimatunnisa Johar, Institute of Epidemiology, Germany
Maite Garolera, Terrassa Health Consortium, Spain

Copyright © 2022 Crespo-Sanmiguel, Zapater-Fajarí, Garrido-Chaves, Hidalgo and Salvador. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Vanesa Hidalgo, vhidalgo@unizar.es

Download