Editorial

in

Cognition and Psychopathology: Overview

Advertisement
Focus
  PreviousNext  

Abstract

The bidirectional relation of cognition and psychopathology is discussed in historical context as an introduction to the special issue of Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology devoted to contemporary research on this topic. Cognition refers to the processes by which information is transformed, elaborated, stored, recovered, and used. Psychopathology refers to disorders in behavior, cognition, and/or perception. Although rooted in studies of major psychiatric disorders and general intelligence, the field has moved to include developmental disabilities, neurological impairment, and less severe psychological disturbance on the one hand and finer grained cognitive processes on the other as well as a constant concern with language issues. Special methods of investigation reveal subtle effects but important facets of the cognition–psychopathology relation.

Cognition refers to the processes by which sensory input is transformed, elaborated, stored, recovered, and used. Psychopathology is generally understood to refer to disorders in behavior, cognition, and/or perception. An extreme example of disordered cognition is delusional states, often observed in persons with schizophrenia. An extreme example of disordered perception is hallucinatory states, less often but sometimes observed in persons with schizophrenia. Disorders of behavior may be associated with either or both conditions, varying along a wide continuum of severity. Cognitive psychopathology is a recent approach that uses the concepts and methods of cognitive psychology to study how cognitive dysfunctions (cognitive impairments, cognitive biases, and dysfunctional beliefs) contribute to the development, recurrence, and maintenance of psychopathological states. Adherents of this approach postulate that cognitive dysfunctions, which are closely related to emotional and relational processes, contribute to the development, maintenance, and recurrence of psychopathological disorders or symptoms. According to Bentall (1996), the main research goals of cognitive psychopathology are as follows:

  • To provide better understanding of mental disorders

  • To establish direct relationships between clinical symptoms and cognitive abnormalities

  • To provide better understanding of the organization, functioning, and cerebral substrates of normal psychological processes, thereby emphasizing the interaction between cognition and emotion

From a clinical point of view, the principal goals of cognitive psychopathology are as follows:

  • To develop new methods for assessing psychopathological symptoms

  • To develop new treatments

  • To contribute to new and improved classifications in psychiatry and abnormal psychology

We have assembled this group of papers as a sample, not necessarily representative, of contemporary psychological research on ways in which cognition and psychopathology are related and reflected in behavior, intellectual performance, social interaction, and nervous system phenomena. In doing so, we have defined psychopathology rather broadly to include psychotic, emotional, and behavior disorders as well as disorders primarily of development (e.g., intellectual disability, autism spectrum disorders, learning disabilities, and developmental language disorders). We have deliberately included language disorders because of the intimate relation of language and other cognitive processes.

Although cognition and psychopathology have traditionally been studied as relatively independent domains, there has been an observable trend over the last century to view and study them as interrelated in important and interesting ways (see, e.g., Beck, 1976; Beck & Bredemeier, 2016; Bleuler, 1911/1950; Dobson & Kendall, 1993; Haywood & Lidz, 2007; Hunt & Cofer, 1944; Kraepelin, 1919; Raffard et al., 2016; Roder & Müller, 2015; Shahar, 2013). Maher (1999) observed that “the mentally ill are generally defined in practice, if not in theory, by their manifest behavior. Abberations of their conscious experience, their sensations, feelings, thoughts and inner language are not open to direct observation” (p. 547). In spite of the “hidden” nature of these phenomena that are not directly observable, one can discern, from the papers in the present modest collection, unmistakable progress in quantifying and studying them.

Learning more about the interrelations of cognition and psychopathology is a scientifically important enterprise for both basic science and clinical applications. From a scientific standpoint, understanding how cognition and psychopathology are interrelated, and how each of these states and processes might possibly affect the other, will advance the understanding of both. From a clinical perspective, the importance of understanding their interrelations will surely lead to advances in diagnosis and treatment, with potential treatment entry points in both domains of psychopathology and cognitive processes.

MODERN (BUT NOT CONTEMPORARY) HISTORY OF INQUIRY INTO COGNITION AND PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

Both Bleuler (1911/1950) and Kraepelin (1919) were concerned with the relation of psychopathology, especially the schizophrenias, and intellective functions, primarily as effects of severe psychiatric disorders on intelligence. To be sure, intelligence had not yet been analyzed into its subcomponents, so these pioneers in psychiatry tended to regard intelligence as a gross, undifferentiated phenomenon, in spite of the efforts of such investigators as Galton (1883) and Spearman (1927) to discover the components of intelligence. Both Bleuler and Kraepelin, as well as many of their contemporaries, observed that the acute onset of dementia praecox brought with it a significant decline in general intelligence, a decline that most psychiatrists of the time thought to be irreversible. Hunt and Cofer (1944) reported and challenged those observations. A series of studies done primarily in U. S. Veterans Administration psychiatric hospitals demonstrated a corelation of psychiatric symptoms and scores on intelligence tests, showing that IQ was at least partially recovered with diminution of psychiatric symptoms (see, e.g., Blaufarb, 1962; Hamlin, Haywood, & Folsom, 1965; Haywood & Moelis, 1963; Heinrich, 1991; Hunt & Cofer, 1944; Sclan, 1986; see also Haywood & Lidz, 2007, pp. 49–63). These studies revealed that special interactive techniques of assessment, now known as dynamic assessment, were instrumental in uncovering intellective abilities that had been thought to be lost permanently.

It is important to note that most of the foregoing studies were focused on linguistic functions, such as vocabulary, verbal abstracting, and interpretation of proverbs, although some relied on general intellectual functioning (full-scale IQ). The series of studies eventually included individuals with intellectual and developmental disabilities (e.g., Gordon & Haywood, 1969) and traumatic brain injuries (Haywood & Miller, 2003), with strikingly similar results. Whereas, in cases of major psychiatric disorders, abilities that had once been developed appeared to be lost with the appearance of symptoms of the disorder, then to be at least partially recovered as psychiatric symptoms diminished, in cases of developmental disabilities, it has been possible, with the use of interactive/dynamic assessment strategies, to reveal intellective and cognitive process abilities that had not been previously manifest as well as an unsuspected “teachability,” that is, ability to learn given teaching of fundamental cognitive processes, principles, and operations. Such findings lead us to conclude that special and more sensitive techniques of investigation and assessment are required to discover the masking of intellective and cognitive processes that appears to accompany an astonishing variety of psychopathological conditions, reminding us of Sigmund Freud’s and Otto Fenichel’s references to “neurotic stupidity” (e.g., Fenichel, 1946).

Such special and sensitive methods of research certainly include brain imaging techniques that have become available in the last quarter century. Although recent analyses have questioned the validity of several fMRI studies (e.g., Eklund, Nichols, & Knutson, 2016), we acknowledge that neurophysiological and neuroanatomical methods such as electroencephalography, magnetoencephalography, fMRI, and transcranial magnetic stimulation have provided an unprecedented opportunity to examine the neurocognitive components of mental disorders. For example, there is now evidence that auditory hallucinations in both clinical and nonclinical populations can be considered as erroneous perceptions or sensory deceptions without the presence of external stimuli and have been attributed to erroneous integration of sensory and cognitive processes that may influence conscious perception (Jardri, Pouchet, Pins, & Thomas, 2011).

From a neuroimaging perspective, structural brain imaging has allowed researchers to identify subtle but robust reductions in the gray matter volume in patients with auditory verbal hallucinations, particularly in areas involved in speech and language (Allen, Larøi, McGuire, Aleman, 2008; see also Caprihan et al., 2013), even if underlying mechanisms of auditory hallucinations extend beyond those processes (Waters & Jardri, 2014). Despite these important scientific advances, we must acknowledge that for the most part, brain imaging studies reproduce what has already been demonstrated via experimental and abnormal psychology, such as difficulty in identifying self- generated information, specific externalization bias, or failure to inhibit irrelevant memories (for a review, see Waters, 2012).

There is now clear evidence that cognitive psychopathology has allowed fundamental advances for our understanding of psychopathological disorders and brought significant advances for the treatment of individuals with mental disorders. Cognitive behavioral therapy and cognitive remediation have produced clear evidence of their efficacy (see, e.g., the review by Hofmann, Asnaani, Vonk, Sawyer, & Fang, 2012). It is important to note here that psychopathology cannot be explained solely by cognition. Biological, environmental, and societal variables, as well as emotional, motoric, and motivational processes, are important for our comprehension of mental states. As proposed by Kendler, Zachar, and Craver (2011), psychiatric disorders are defined not in terms of essences but in terms of complex, mutually reinforcing networks of causal mechanisms. Thus, we also would like to point out, following the work proposed by Belayashi and Van der Linden (in this issue), that the relationships between cognition and psychopathological states or psychiatric symptoms are complex, dynamic, and multidimensional and that cognitive interventions and cognitive theories must take account of this complexity.

REFERENCES

  1. P. Allen, F. Larøi, P. K. McGuire, & A. Aleman (2008). The hallucinating brain: A review of structural and functional neuroimaging studies of hallucinations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 175–191.
  2. A. T. Beck (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders New York, NY: International Universities Press.
  3. A. T. Beck, & K. Bredemeier (2016). A unified model of depression: Integrating clinical, cognitive, biological, and evolutionary perspectives. Clinical Psychological Science, 4(4), 596–619.
  4. H. A. Blaufarb (1962). A demonstration of verbal abstracting ability in chronic schizophrenics under enriched stimulus and instructional conditions. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 26, 471–475.
  5. R. P. Bentall (1996). At the centre of a science of psychopathology? Characteristics and limitations of cognitive research. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 1(4), 265–273.
  6. M. Bleuler (1950). Dementia praecox or the group of schizophrenias (H. Zenkin, Trans.). New York, NY: International Universities Press. (Original work published 1911)
  7. A. Caprihan, T. Jones, H. Chen, N. Lemke, C. Abbott, C. Qualls, . . . J. Bustillo (2013). The paradoxical relationship between white matter, psychopathology and cognition in schizophrenia: A diffusion tensor and proton spectroscopic imaging study. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40, 2248–2257.
  8. K. S. Dobson, & P. C. Kendall (Eds.). (1993). Psychopathology and cognition. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  9. A. Eklund, T. E. Nichols, & H. Knutsson (2016). Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 113, 7900–7905.
  10. O. Fenichel (1946). The psychoanalytic theory of neurosis. London, United Kingdom: Kegan Paul.
  11. F. Galton (1883). Inquiries into human faculty and its development. London, United Kingdom: Macmillan.
  12. J. E. Gordon, & H. C. Haywood (1969). Input deficit in cultural-familial retardates: Effect of stimulus enrichment. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 73, 604–610.
  13. R. M. Hamlin, H. C. Haywood, & A. T. Folsom (1965). Effects of enriched input on schizophrenic abstraction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 70, 390–394.
  14. H. C. Haywood, & C. S. Lidz (2007). Dynamic assessment in clinical settings. In Dynamic assessment in practice: Clinical and educational applications. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  15. H. C. Haywood, & M. B. Miller (2003). Dynamic assessment of adults with traumatic brain injuries. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 3, 137–163.
  16. H. C. Haywood, & I. Moelis (1963). Effect of symptom change on intellectual function in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 76–78.
  17. J. J. Heinrich (1991). Responsiveness of adults with severe head injury to mediated learning (Unpublished doctoral dissertation), Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN.
  18. S. G. Hofmann, A. Asnaani, I. J. J. Vonk, A. T. Sawyer, & A. Fang (2012). The efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy: A review of meta-analyses. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36(5), 427–440.
  19. J. M. Hunt, & C. N. Cofer (1944). Psychological deficit. In J. M. Hunt (Ed.), Personality and the behavior disorders (pp. 971–1032). New York, NY: Ronald Press.
  20. R. Jardri, A. Pouchet, D. Pins, & P. Thomas (2011). Cortical activations during auditory verbal hallucinations in schizophrenia: A coordinate-based meta-analysis. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 73–81.
  21. K. S. Kendler, P. Zachar, & C. Craver (2011). What kinds of things are psychiatric disorders? Psychological Medicine, 41, 1143–1150.
  22. E. Kraepelin (1971). Dementia praecox (E. Barclay & S. Barclay, Trans.). New York, NY: Churchill Livingstone. (Originally published 1919)
  23. B. A. Maher (1999). Anomalous experience in everyday life: Its significance for psychopathology. The Monist, 82(4), 547–570.
  24. S. Raffard, C. Bortolon, M. Burca, C. Novara, M. C. Gely-Nargeot, D. Capdevielle, & M. Van der Linden (2016). Self-imagination can enhance memory in individuals with schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 21(2), 168–181.
  25. V. Roder, & D. R. Müller (Eds.) (2015). INT-Integrated neurocognitive therapy for schizophrenia patients. Cham, Switzerland: Springer Publishing.
  26. S. G. Sclan (1986). Dynamic assessment and thought disorder in paranoid and nonparanoid schizophrenic patients (Unpublished doctoral dissertation), Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN.
  27. G. Shahar (2013).Self, cognition, and psychopathology: Introduction to the special section (Part 1). International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 6(3), 203–207.
  28. C. Spearman (1927). The abilities of man: Their nature and measurement. London, United Kingdom: Macmillan.
  29. F. Waters (2012). Multidisciplinary approaches to understanding auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia and nonschizophrenia populations: The International Consortium on Hallucination Research. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38, 693–694.
  30. F. Waters, & R. Jardri (2014). Auditory hallucinations: Debunking the myth of language supremacy. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 41, 533–534.
Article usage
Article Usage
Period Abstract Full PDF Total
Apr 2024 199 110 3 312
Mar 2024 197 128 1 326
Feb 2024 161 156 5 322
Jan 2024 186 96 4 286
Dec 2023 175 65 3 243
Nov 2023 249 114 3 366
Oct 2023 230 92 3 325
Sep 2023 153 62 5 220
Aug 2023 154 54 1 209
Jul 2023 206 48 6 260
Jun 2023 532 70 13 615
May 2023 278 134 6 418
Apr 2023 249 112 8 369
Mar 2023 277 160 11 448
Feb 2023 285 177 5 467
Jan 2023 473 138 14 625
Dec 2022 222 155 8 385
Nov 2022 11654 148 9 11811
Oct 2022 12855 109 12 12976
Sep 2022 4402 107 7 4516
Aug 2022 231 150 7 388
Jul 2022 259 133 3 395
Jun 2022 258 72 7 337
May 2022 242 102 12 356
Apr 2022 252 149 5 406
Mar 2022 264 175 9 448
Feb 2022 237 200 5 442
Jan 2022 246 162 4 412
Dec 2021 270 170 3 443
Nov 2021 207 112 3 322
Oct 2021 258 102 9 369
Sep 2021 235 176 1 412
Aug 2021 235 58 1 294
Jul 2021 243 70 2 315
Jun 2021 291 119 7 417
May 2021 339 173 3 515
Apr 2021 413 187 5 605
Mar 2021 469 226 8 703
Feb 2021 525 216 9 750
Jan 2021 499 138 4 641
Dec 2020 312 160 9 481
Nov 2020 357 239 7 603
Oct 2020 375 128 3 506
Sep 2020 395 144 6 545
Aug 2020 670 68 1 739
Jul 2020 297 54 7 358
Jun 2020 402 80 6 488
May 2020 283 59 2 344
Apr 2020 308 85 12 405
Mar 2020 341 35 7 383
Feb 2020 358 47 8 413
Jan 2020 311 30 4 345
Dec 2019 343 23 5 371
Nov 2019 342 56 4 402
Oct 2019 305 69 10 384
Sep 2019 149 57 7 213
Aug 2019 174 65 12 251
Jul 2019 139 20 7 166
Jun 2019 176 21 17 214
May 2019 9 10 4 23
Apr 2019 29 0 0 29
Mar 2019 31 0 0 31
Feb 2019 42 0 0 42
Jan 2019 22 1 1 24
Dec 2018 10 0 0 10
Nov 2018 9 0 0 9
Oct 2018 4 1 1 6
Sep 2018 15 0 1 16
Aug 2018 2 0 0 2
Jul 2018 1 0 0 1
Focus
  PreviousNext