Skip to content
Licensed Unlicensed Requires Authentication Published by De Gruyter May 4, 2018

Estimating the Use of Higher-Order Theory of Mind Using Computational Agents

  • Harmen de Weerd EMAIL logo , Denny Diepgrond and Rineke Verbrugge

Abstract

When people make decisions in a social context, they often make use of theory of mind, by reasoning about unobservable mental content of others. For example, the behavior of a pedestrian who wants to cross the street depends on whether or not he believes that the driver of an oncoming car has seen him or not. People can also reason about the theory of mind abilities of others, leading to recursive thinking of the sort ‘I think that you think that I think…’. Previous research suggests that this ability may be especially effective in simple competitive settings. In this paper, we use a combination of computational agents and Bayesian model selection to determine to what extent people make use of higher-order theory of mind reasoning in a particular competitive game known as matching pennies. We find that while many children and adults appear to make use of theory of mind, participants are also often classified as using a simpler reactive strategy based only on the actions of the directly preceding round. This may indicate that human reasoners do not primarily use their theory of mind abilities to compete with others.

References

Ayala, Arad, and Ariel Rubinstein. 2012. “The 11–20 Money Request Game: A Level-k Reasoning Study.” The American Economic Review 102 (7): 3561–73.10.1257/aer.102.7.3561Search in Google Scholar

Bacharach, M., and D. O. Stahl. 2000. “Variable-Frame Level-n Theory.” Games and Economic Behavior 32 (2): 220–46.10.1006/game.2000.0796Search in Google Scholar

Bergwerff, Gerben, Ben Meijering, Jakub Szymanik, Rineke Verbrugge, and Stefan M. Wierda. 2014. “Computational and Algorithmic Models of Strategies in Turn-Based Games.” In Proceedings of the 36th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, edited by Paul Bello, Marcello Guarini, Marjorie McShane, and Brian Scassellati, 1778–83. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Search in Google Scholar

Blokpoel, Mark, Marlieke van Kesteren, Arjen Stolk, Pim Haselager, Ivan Toni, and Iris van Rooij. 2012. “Recipient Design in Human Communication: Simple Heuristics or Perspective Taking?” Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6 (253). doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00253.Search in Google Scholar

Camerer, C.F., T.H. Ho, and J.K. Chong. 2004. “A Cognitive Hierarchy Model of Games.” Quarterly Journal of Economics 119 (3): 861–98.10.1162/0033553041502225Search in Google Scholar

Camerer, Colin, and Teck Hua Ho. 1999. “Experience-Weighted Attraction Learning in Normal form Games.” Econometrica 67 (4): 827–74.10.1111/1468-0262.00054Search in Google Scholar

Costa-Gomes, Miguel, Vincent P. Crawford, and Bruno Broseta. 2001. “Cognition and Behavior in Normal-form Games: An Experimental Study.” Econometrica 69 (5): 1193–235.10.1111/1468-0262.00239Search in Google Scholar

De Ruiter, Jan Peter, M. Noordzij, Sarah Newman-Norlund, Peter Hagoort, and Ivan Toni. 2007. “On the Origin of Intentions.” Attention & Performance XXII: 593–610.Search in Google Scholar

de Weerd, Harmen, Rineke Verbrugge, and Bart Verheij. 2013. “How Much Does it Help to Know What She Knows You Know? An Agent-Based Simulation Study.” Artificial Intelligence 199-200: 67–92. doi:10.1016/j.artint.2013.05.004.Search in Google Scholar

de Weerd, Harmen, Rineke Verbrugge, and Bart Verheij. 2014. “Theory of Mind in the Mod Game: An Agent-Based Model of Strategic Reasoning.” In Proceedings of the European Conference on Social Intelligence (ECSI-2014) volume 1283 of CEUR Workshop Proceedings, edited by A. Herzig and E. Lorini, Barcelona, November 3–5, 128–36.Search in Google Scholar

de Weerd Harmen, Eveline Broers, and Rineke Verbrugge. 2015a. “Savvy Software Agents Can Encourage the Use of Second-Order Theory of Mind by Negotiators. In Proceedings of the 37th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, edited by D. Noelle, R. Dale, A. Warlaumont, J. Yoshimi, T. Matlock, C. Jennings, and P. Maglio, 542–47.Search in Google Scholar

de Weerd, Harmen, Rineke Verbrugge, and Bart Verheij. 2015b. “Higher-Order Theory of Mind in the Tacit Communication Game.” Biologically Inspired Cognitive Architectures 11: 10–21. doi:10.1016/j.bica.2014.11.010.Search in Google Scholar

de Weerd, Harmen, Rineke Verbrugge, and Bart Verheij. 2017. “Negotiating with Other Minds: The Role of Recursive Theory of Mind in Negotiation With Incomplete Information.” Autonomous Agents and Multi-Agent Systems 31: 250–87.10.1007/s10458-015-9317-1Search in Google Scholar

Devaine, Marie, Guillaume Hollard, and Jean Daunizeau. 2014a. “The Social Bayesian Brain: Does Mentalizing Make a Difference When we Learn?” PLoS Computational Biology 10 (12): e1003992. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003992.Search in Google Scholar

Devaine, Marie, Guillaume Hollard, and Jean Daunizeau. 2014b. “Theory of Mind: Did Evolution Fool us?” PloS ONE 9 (2): e87619. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0087619.Search in Google Scholar

Doshi, Prashant, Qu Xia, Adam Goodie, and Diana Young. 2010. “Modeling Recursive Reasoning by Humans Using Empirically Informed Interactive POMDPs. In Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Autonomous Agents and Multiagent Systems: volume 1-Volume 1, 1223–30. IFAAMAS, 2010.Search in Google Scholar

Franke, Michael, and Paolo Galeazzi. 2014. “On the Evolution of Choice Principles.” In Proceedings of the Second Workshop Reasoning About Other Minds: Logical and Cognitive Perspectives, co-located with Advances in Modal Logic, Groningen, The Netherlands volume 1208 of CEUR Workshop Proceedings, edited by Jakub Szymanik and Rineke Verbrugge, 11–15.Search in Google Scholar

Frey, Seth, and Robert L. Goldstone. Cyclic Game Dynamics Driven by Iterated Reasoning.” PloS ONE 8 (2): e56416, 2013. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0056416.Search in Google Scholar

Ghosh, Sujata, and Rineke Verbrugge. 2017. “Studying Strategies and Types of Players: Experiments, Logics and Cognitive Models.” Synthese 2017. doi:10.1007/s11229-017-1338-7. in press.Search in Google Scholar

Ghosh, Sujata, Ben Meijering, and Rineke Verbrugge. 2014. “Strategic Reasoning: Building Cognitive Models from Logical Formulas.” Journal of Logic, Language and Information 23 (1): 1–29. doi:10.1007/s10849-014-9196-x.Search in Google Scholar

Ghosh, Sujata, Aviad Heifetz, and Rineke Verbrugge 2015. “Do Players Reason by Forward Induction in Dynamic Perfect Information Games?” In Proceedings of the 15th Conference on Theoretical Aspects of Rationality and Knowledge (TARK 2015), edited by R. Ramanujam, 121–30. Chennai: Institute of Mathematical Sciences.Search in Google Scholar

Gmytrasiewicz, P., and E. Durfee. 1995. “A Rigorous, Operational Formalization of Recursive Modeling.” In Proceedings of the First International Conference on Autonomous Agents and Multiagent Systems, 125–32.Search in Google Scholar

Gmytrasiewicz, Piotr J., and Prashant Doshi. 2005. “A Framework for Sequential Planning in Multi-agent Settings.” Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research 24: 49–79.10.1613/jair.1579Search in Google Scholar

Gmytrasiewicz, Piotr J., Sanguk Noh, and Tad Kellogg. 1998. “Bayesian Update of Recursive Agent Models.” User Modeling and User-Adapted Interaction 8 (1-2): 49–69.10.1023/A:1008269427670Search in Google Scholar

Goeree, Jacob K., and Charles A. Holt. 2004. “A Model of Noisy Introspection.” Games and Economic Behavior 46 (2): 365–82.10.1016/S0899-8256(03)00145-3Search in Google Scholar

Goodie, Adam S., Prashant Doshi, and Diana L. Young. 2012. “Levels of Theory-of-Mind Reasoning in Competitive Games.” Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 25 (1): 95–108. doi:10.1002/bdm.717.Search in Google Scholar

Halder, Tamoghna, Khyati Sharma, Sujata Ghosh, and Rineke Verbrugge. 2015. “How do Adults Reason About Their Opponent? Typologies of Players in Turn-Taking Games.” In Proceedings of the 37th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, edited by D. Noelle, R. Dale, A. Warlaumont, J. Yoshimi, T. Matlock, C. Jennings, and P. Maglio, 854–59. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Search in Google Scholar

Hedden, Trey, and Jun Zhang. 2002. “What do you Think I Think you Think?: Strategic Reasoning in Matrix Games.” Cognition 85 (1): 1–36, ISSN 0010-0277. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00054-9.Search in Google Scholar

Heifetz, Aviad. 2012. Game Theory: Interactive Strategies in Economics and Management. Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781139049344Search in Google Scholar

Hu, Junling, and Michael P. Wellman. 1998. “Online Learning About Other Agents in a Dynamic Multiagent System.” In Proceedings of the Second International Conference on Autonomous Agents, edited by Katia P. Sycara and Michael Wooldridge, 239–46. doi:10.1145/280765.280839.Search in Google Scholar

Johnson, E. J., C. F. Camerer, S. Sen, and T. Rymon. 2002. “Detecting Failures of Backward Induction: Monitoring Information Search in Sequential Bargaining.” Journal of Economic Theory 104: 16–47. doi:10.1006/jeth.2001.2850.Search in Google Scholar

Kinderman, Peter, Robin I. M. Dunbar, and Richard P. Bentall. 1998. “Theory-of-Mind Deficits and Causal Attributions.” British Journal of Psychology 89 (2): 191–204. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1998.tb02680.x.Search in Google Scholar

McKelvey, R. D., and T. R. Palfrey. 1995. “Quantal Response Equilibria for Normal form Games.” Games and Economic Behavior 10 (1): 6–38.10.1006/game.1995.1023Search in Google Scholar

Meijering, Ben, Leendert van Maanen, Hedderik van Rijn, and Rineke Verbrugge. 2010. “The Facilitative Effect of Context on Second-Order Social Reasoning.” In Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, edited by Stellan Ohlsson and Richard Catrambone, 1423–28. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Search in Google Scholar

Meijering, Ben, Hedderik van Rijn, Niels A. Taatgen, and Rineke Verbrugge. 2011. “I do Know what you Think I Think: Second-Order Theory of Mind in Strategic Games is not that Difficult.” In Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, edited by Laura Carlson, Christoph Hoelscher, and Thomas F. Shipley, 2486–91. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Search in Google Scholar

Meijering, Ben, Hedderik van Rijn, Niels A Taatgen, and Rineke Verbrugge. 2012. “What Eye Movements can Tell About Theory of Mind in a Strategic Game.” PLOS ONE 7: e45961. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0045961.Search in Google Scholar

Meijering, Ben, Niels A. Taatgen, Hedderik van Rijn, and Rineke Verbrugge. 2014. “Modeling Inference of Mental States: As Simple as Possible, as Complex as Necessary.” Interaction Studies 15: 455–77. doi: 10.1075/is.15.3.05mei.10.1075/is.15.3.05meiSearch in Google Scholar

Nagel, Rosemarie. 1995. “Unraveling in Guessing Games: An Experimental Study.” The American Economic Review 85 (5): 1313–26.Search in Google Scholar

Premack, David, and Guy Woodruff. 1978. “Does the Chimpanzee have a Theory of Mind?” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1 (04): 515–26, ISSN 0140-525X. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00076512.Search in Google Scholar

Raijmakers, Maartje E. J, Dorothy J. Mandell, Sara E. van Es, and Marian Counihan. 2014. “Children’s Strategy Use when Playing Strategic Games.” Synthese 191 (3): 355–70. doi:10.1007/s11229-012-0212-x.Search in Google Scholar

Rapoport, Amnon, and David V. Budescu. 1997. “Randomization in Individual Choice Behavior.” Psychological Review 104 (3): 603–17. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.104.3.603.Search in Google Scholar

Rubinstein, Ariel. 2016. “A Typology of Players: Between Instinctive and Contemplative.” The Quarterly Journal of Economics 131 (2): 859–90.10.1093/qje/qjw008Search in Google Scholar

Sher, Itai, Melissa Koenig, and Aldo Rustichini. 2014. “Children’s Strategic Theory of Mind.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111 (37): 13307–12.10.1073/pnas.1403283111Search in Google Scholar

Stahl, D. O., and P. W. Wilson. 1995. “On Players’ Models of Other Players: Theory and Experimental Evidence.” Games and Economic Behavior 10 (1): 218–54.10.1006/game.1995.1031Search in Google Scholar

Stephan, Klaas E., Will D. Penny, Jean Daunizeau, Rosalyn J. Moran, and Karl J. Friston. 2009. “Bayesian Model Selection for Group Studies.” Neuroimage 46 (4): 1004–17.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.025Search in Google Scholar

Stiller, James, and Robin I. M. Dunbar. 2007. “Perspective-Taking and Memory Capacity Predict Social Network Size.” Social Networks 29 (1): 93–104. doi:10.1016/j.socnet.2006.04.001.Search in Google Scholar

van de Pol, Iris, Iris van Rooij, and Jakub Szymanik. 2016. “Parameterized Complexity Results for a Model of Theory of Mind Based on Dynamic Epistemic Logic.” In Proceedings of the 15th Conference on Theoretical Aspects of Rationality and Knowledge, edited by R. Ramanujam, 239–48. Chennai: Institute of Mathematical Sciences.Search in Google Scholar

Wagenaar, Willem A. 1972. “Generation of Random Sequences by Human Subjects: A Critical Survey of Literature.” Psychological Bulletin 77 (1): 65–72. doi:10.1037/h0032060.Search in Google Scholar

Wright, James R., and Kevin Leyton-Brown. 2010. “Beyond Equilibrium: Predicting Human Behavior in Normal-Form Games.” In Proceedings of the 24th Conference on Artificial Intelligence, 901–07.10.1145/1807406.1807449Search in Google Scholar

Yoshida, Wako, Ray J. Dolan, and Karl J. Friston. 2008. “Game Theory of Mind.” PLoS Computational Biology 4 (12): e1000254.10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000254Search in Google Scholar

Published Online: 2018-05-04

© 2018 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 17.5.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/bejte-2016-0184/html
Scroll to top button