Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

The applicability of social cognitive career theory in predicting life satisfaction of university students: A meta-analytic path analysis

Abstract

Derived from the social cognitive career theory (SCCT), the present study developed a model for the empirical examination of factors affecting the life satisfaction of university students. A random-effects meta-analysis of zero-order correlations observed the results of 16 studies (20 samples, n = 7,967), and associations among the SCCT variables were examined by using a meta-analytic structural equation modeling (MASEM) according to a pooled correlation matrix. An alternative model was offered and then assessed. The findings showed a satisfactory fit of the new model as compared to the original SCCT. The results demonstrated support for the alternative model of SCCT in predicting life satisfaction. The present study suggested that researchers should embrace this alternative model when synthesizing SCCT factors. Limitations and avenues for future research were put forward for further consideration.

Introduction

The university-to-work transition is a vital step in the creation of a job identity for graduates. During this period, some can face this transition confidently, while others experience hesitancy, insecurity, and hopelessness [1]. Pierceall and Keim [2] discovered that 87% of university students experience moderate to high levels of stress during this stage of life. This stress leads to many undesirable results such as anxiety, isolation, and low self-confidence [3], which might influence their mental health. Therefore, Lange [4] stressed that career uncertainty, as an apparent risk and fear of prospect joblessness, has a negative consequence on people’s well-being.

In this regard, previous research has emphasized that career development experience is a vital source to facilitate university students from their unclear future, tackle unfavorable working situations, and therefore increase their life satisfaction [5]. These studies also proposed that university students who are self-assured in their career orientation are satisfied with life [6]. From this standpoint, it is important to examine how students experience concomitant successes and failures at the university-to-work shift. Therefore, the present study investigates how these can affect students’ life satisfaction. For this purpose, the study draws on the concept of life satisfaction from Diener and colleagues [7], who described it as the subjective evaluation and overall cognitive judgment of life.

Life satisfaction is a main factor in the work domain, a supreme goal in human existence after basic needs, and has numerous further positive components such as being an active social player and being in good health [8]. Work and organizational psychologists are recognizing life satisfaction as an important issue as it may both cause career associated consequences and be affected by career-related aspects [9]. Results of previous research have led researchers to assume that career development is potentially linked to general views about life [10]. Therefore, life satisfaction (LS) is the desired aim for both an individual and an organization.

To date, LS of individuals, particularly university students, continues to be diminished in the career-related literature. Consequently, there is a lack of theoretical base around university students’ LS through the university-to-work process and in facilitating their career growth since some fundamental theories [11, 12] may not effectively comprise the environmental and contextual elements that contribute to the effects of career development exposure on LS [13]. As a multistage and leading career model, the social cognitive career theory (SCCT) [14] has revealed to be comprehensive in explaining the distinctive career development practices of different people [1518]. The findings of these studies have constantly shown meaningful associations between predictor constructs in the SCCT and life satisfaction (see Fig 1). Blustein [19] affirmed SCCT as “the most important theory in career development which offers a powerful clarifying notion for scholars” (p. 350). It is proposed that human traits, social-cognitive factors, along with measurable success in other domains of life, should be expected for LS [20].

thumbnail
Fig 1. SCCT model of normative well-being of life satisfaction [20].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237838.g001

Nevertheless, the extent of the theoretical constructs of SCCT in predicting life satisfaction among university students is inadequate. Findings regarding socio-cognitive factors in predicting LS are conflicting. For instance, some have declared that personality features toward life satisfaction are more important than domain satisfaction [21], while other researchers disagreed [22]. These diverse results regarding SCCT necessitate a meta-analytic review.

A meta-analysis offers an organized tactic for studying empirical literature to synthesize results through studies. The structural equation modeling (SEM) is a technique that is normally employed for confirming whether the theory of hypothetical models fits the data [23]. The approach of merging meta-analysis and SEM could help create theory-based interferences to foster life satisfaction. Several past research has studied SCCT by using meta-analysis. Many of them have investigated a particular outcome, such as academic performance [24], choice goals [25], and career functioning [26]. Nevertheless, they examined the efficiency of SCCT variables in predicting how individuals perform actions for others’ benefits instead of individual benefits, such as life satisfaction. Thus, this study aims to test SCCT and how it can predict the life satisfaction of university students by using meta-analytic path analysis.

Literature review

SCCT is a model proposed by Lent [14] to clarify the conditions in which: (a) career hobbies are established; (b) academic and vocational options are chosen; (c) career determination is performed. It has received extensive consideration from researchers whereby many of them have placed the model on the career growth of school and university students [27]. The key notion has been that though career choice actions are performed through the lifetime, the concepts are most noticeable through the late teenage years and early adulthood when people are planning to join the workforce [14]. For this reason, not much attention has been paid to the value of this theory to predict outcomes for youths who are in the middle of the university-to-work transition. The university-to-work transition refers to on-the-job training, vocational training, service-learning agreements, or other programs designed to prepare university students to enter the workforce.

Lent and Brown [28] later extended SCCT to a satisfaction model. While the extended SCCT employs the term work satisfaction, this is meant to generally comprise other forms of satisfaction as well. Lent, Brown, and Hackett [29] suggested that SCCT, with its attention to both individual and background variables, can be a useful model to describe the university-to-work transition. The SCCT satisfaction model also includes self-efficacy (SE), outcome expectations (OEs), goal progress (GP), personality traits (PTs), domain satisfaction (DS), and environmental supports (ESs), which predict life satisfaction. Besides, this point needs to be reminded that, self-efficacy means a personal judgment of "how well one can execute courses of action required to deal with future situations [100]. Also, domain satisfaction reflects the extent to which objective conditions in a particular area of life match individuals’ respective needs or aspirations [79].

On the other hand, Lent and Brown [30] stated that general measures of SCCT variables may have limited utility, in that these variables require to be deliberated in domain-specific contexts. Thus, they warned academics to utilize domain-specific measures in any examinations of extended SCCT.

Previous studies found that life satisfaction was significantly associated with DS in the context of university-to-work transition, thus providing logic for embracing life satisfaction as an outcome construct [31]. Lent and Brown [28] mentioned that Heller, Watson, and Ilies’ [32] research is one of the bases for the satisfaction model. According to Heller and colleagues [32], the relative pros of top-down (person-based) or bottom-up (situation-based) approaches toward explaining subjective well-being in binary routes are as follows: (a) they studied associations between PTs, DS measures, and life satisfaction; and (b) they created and examined three rival hypothetical models. The initial model was named as “straight effects” top-down model, wherein PTs were each linked directly to DS and life satisfaction. The second model, named the personality top-down model, illustrated the PTs as having individual links directly to life satisfaction. Lastly, the third model, the combining model, offered direct associations between PTs with every satisfaction domain (marital, job, and life). Besides, DS linked directly to life satisfaction. This is the model as a blend of the top-down and bottom-up approaches toward describing subjective well-being [32]. Their study supported the notion that life satisfaction and DS are related constructs.

Goal progress, domain satisfaction, personality traits, and life satisfaction

Discussions on GP and DS on life satisfaction of university students have been directly or indirectly debated since the original work of Lent and Brown [30]. GP represents a third type of cognitive variable. According to Latham, Mawritz, and Locke [33], having goals and experiencing growth toward one’s goals directs to bigger stages of satisfaction. Sheldon and Kasser’s [34] study supported the relationship between perceived GP and satisfaction. For university students, academic GP was predictive of enrolment and persistence in academic majors [35]. Lent [20] stated that goals may encourage an individual’s feeling of satisfaction by activating optimistic responses in reaction to his/her perceived progress on an appreciated goal. On the contrary, the lack of such support, or the existence of background barriers, is possible to block GP and decrease life satisfaction [30]. Besides, Lent [36] showed that DS (e.g., academic satisfaction) was the most consistent predictor of life satisfaction. Previous literature has commonly believed that an additive association among DS and life satisfaction does exist [37]. Lent and Brown [28] characterized life satisfaction as having “trait-like features” (p. 243). Thus, they conceptualized PTs as inputs to the satisfaction model. PTs can influence people’s life satisfaction [38]. Therefore:

  1. Hypothesis 1a: GP is positively linked to life satisfaction.
  2. Hypothesis 1b: DS is positively linked to life satisfaction.
  3. Hypothesis 1c: PTs are positively linked to life satisfaction.

Environmental supports, self-efficacy, outcome expectations, goal progress, personality traits, and domain satisfaction

ESs as contextual affordances can facilitate an individual’s vocational option and growth [35]. SCCT is concerned with ESs as a predictor of DS. Gibbons and Shoffner [39] acclaimed SCCT as most promising as it embraces constructs that explain disparities in environmental opportunities along with individuals’ beliefs about the environment. Researchers have found support for including ESs in the social cognitive model of the domain in university students’ sample [40]. According to Lent and Brown [30], ESs as reinforcement, modeling, and positive feedback provide assets to individuals that can increase the sense of satisfaction [20]. Besides, persons differ in their SE regarding the behaviors needed in several academic and career domains. SE beliefs are pretty dynamic and are specific to specific activity domains. SE as a fundamental part of one’s life provides a vital advantage in terms of social and career development [41]. Lent’s [20] research supported the relationship between OEs and DS. The SCCT model forecasts satisfaction in the social domain through straight routes from the combined effects of social cognitive constructs [36]. Further study is required to define the association between OEs and DS. Lent [36] discovered that GP was predictive of DS. According to Wiese and Freund [42], career development depends partly on external factors but is also determined by personal goals. The meta-analysis of Klug and Maier [43] revealed a strong connection between GP and satisfaction in a particular domain. Moreover, Lent [44] proposed that factors like PTs influence DS. Lent’s normative model of well-being (see Fig 1) offers cognitive, behavioral, environmental, and personality elements that regulate individuals’ DS [36, 45]. Hence:

  1. Hypothesis 2a: ESs are positively linked to DS.
  2. Hypothesis 2b: SE is positively linked to DS.
  3. Hypothesis 2c: OEs are positively linked to DS.
  4. Hypothesis 2d: GP is positively linked to DS.
  5. Hypothesis 2e: PTs are positively linked to DS.

Environmental supports, self-efficacy, outcome expectations, and goal progress

Lent [46] uncovered that ESs, which comprise goal-relevant assets, modeling, and inspiration, is associated with affecting GP (r = .23). Furthermore, Lent [14] found that SE will directly affect the kinds of interests that a person will grow. Later, Lent [47] stated that SE is a predictor of GP for undergraduates because they anticipate to obtain favorable results when pursuing actions at which they think are effective. Higher SE appraisals are also associated with GP, regardless of barriers. Moreover, OEs also play a key role in motivating individuals toward their goals. According to the SCCT model, OEs and SE together shape motivation [14]. Satisfaction can be viewed as somewhat a function of individuals’ positive OEs about that possible result from pursuing an esteemed goal [48]. Therefore:

  1. Hypothesis 3a: ESs are positively linked to GP.
  2. Hypothesis 3b: SE is positively linked to GP.
  3. Hypothesis 3c: OEs are positively linked to GP.

Personality traits, environmental supports, and self-efficacy

In the career development context, SE refers to persons’ confidence in their skill to succeed in creating academic or vocational choices. Scholars highlighted the vitality of examining the initial phase of career development because it is an important stage when PTs and career SE are founded [49]. Thus, they have started to call for the combination of PTs into any model assessing the backgrounds of academic and career options. PTs may influence vocational confidence in a parallel way to interests [14]. Although limited studies have examined the relationships between PTs and the vocational and career constructs [50], it is still difficult to illustrate strong deductions regarding the effects of PTs due to the use of varying scales. More recently, scholars have pursued to study if and how PTs influence the cognitive constructs involved in the career development progressions of SE [22]. In this context, PTs are also considered as direct antecedents of SE in which both PTs and SE have proven to be significant antecedents of academic and career success [51]. Studies showed that ESs are indirectly linked to life satisfaction through SE and OEs [28]. In their longitudinal study, Hou, Wu, and Liu [52] found that environmental and social supports predict SE among Chinese students. Therefore:

  1. Hypothesis 4a: PTs are positively linked to SE.
  2. Hypothesis 4b: ESs are positively linked to SE.

Environmental supports, self-efficacy, and outcome expectations

OEs are individuals’ attitudes about the effects of their behaviors and can be influenced by previous experiences, skills, and performance in addition to social supports [14]. According to Franco [53], OEs do not happen in a vacuum and socio-cultural environments may affect these expectations. In the SCCT model, OEs are informed by vicarious environmental factors (e.g., social support) and they later play a central role in predicting career options [54]. Kelly [27] found that ESs are unique contributors to OEs. Hui, Yuen, and Chen’s study [55] also validated the link between ESs and OEs. According to Garriott [56], ES predicted college OEs in first-and non-first-generation students. Bandura [57] posited that SE is another significant source of OEs, which means higher confidence in a person’s skill of completing certain tasks would result in more positive perceptions of the outcomes. According to Ali, McWhirter, and Chronister [58], vocational/educational SE beliefs are accounted for a meaningful total of variance in vocational and educational OEs. Therefore:

  1. Hypothesis 5a: ESs are positively related to OEs.
  2. Hypothesis 5b: SE is positively related to OEs.

Personality traits and environmental supports

PTs are unique qualities that are the epitome of an individual. They are individuals’ habitual patterns of behavior, temperament, and emotion [59]. According to the social cognitive theory and symbolic interactionism, humans are not passive agents; however, as these traditions also suggest, individuals’ PTs are actively shaped by the environment [60]. Previous studies similarly addressed the roles of PTs and ESs, such as background experiences [61]. PTs in the SCCT model may comprise characteristics like trait positive/negative effects and are hypothesized to predict ESs [56]. Furthermore, PTs have been shown to predict ESs [44]. Longitudinal investigations have provided support for this relationship [62]. It means that PTs can provide the motivational impulses or the motivational blocks to use or not to use environmental and social supports and thus to improve or reduce performance [63]. Therefore:

  1. Hypothesis 6a: PTs are positively related to ESs.

Materials and methods

Search strategy for identifying studies

The authors of the present study perused PsycNet, ProQuest, and ScienceDirect, to find related research published between January 1, 2004, and December 31, 2018. The combination of words employed to search these records were life, academic, and domain satisfaction, outcome expectations, self-efficacy, environmental and social supports, and social cognitive career theory. Studies met the inclusion criteria if they: (a) employed the SCCT as a theoretical basis, and (b) supplied quantitative data allowing for the calculation of correlation (r) and/or effect sizes among the SCCT variables.

As well as exploring journal databases, additional search tactics were used to enlarge the number of studies. The authors investigated the websites of well-known journals with a history of releasing higher education and career studies. They also inspected article references and searched for authors by name. Abstracts were assessed according to the inclusion criteria. First, they excluded duplicated research, studies that involved school students (i.e., young people before entering higher education) as participants, and qualitative studies. Second, studies were excluded if they did not meet all of the inclusion standards, or if inadequate data were available for calculation. Sixteen studies (20 samples) were chosen for the meta-analytic path analysis (see Fig 2). Finally, Cooper’s [64] method of the unstructured search was employed by using Google Scholar and Google for identifying more relevant studies (see Table 1).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Flow chart of the study selection process in this meta-analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237838.g002

Data extraction

Data were obtained by a predesigned protocol that was based on the ‘Joanna Briggs Institute’ [65] and ‘Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analysis’ (PRISMA) regulations [66]. It was re-examined and filtered by an expert group including a higher education expert and a career development university lecturer. The obtained outcome data comprised a correlation matrix of SCCT variables, which were ESs, SE, OEs, GP, DS, PTs, and life satisfaction. The sample size (n) of every study and the observed association of SCCT variables were gathered to execute the meta-analytic path analysis.

Quality assessment

The sixteen studies that achieved the inclusion criteria were evaluated individually by two researchers for methodology characteristic by the ‘Quality Assessment and Validity Tool for Correlational Studies’ (QAVTCS) [67]. The quality assessment tool was employed to check four key parts of the study: design, sample selection, instrument, and data analysis. Thirteen standards were assessed, with a total of fourteen possible scores. Build on the designated score, studies were categorized as low (0–4), moderate (5–9), or high (10–14) quality. Inconsistencies in the scores were settled after the debate between the two researchers. This resulted in sixteen studies rated as high. The rest did not achieve most of the criteria listed in the QAVTCS, so it was scored as 3 which was of low-level quality and was omitted from additional review. Therefore, all sixteen studies were kept for analysis (see Table 2).

Data abstraction

The present study utilized a strategy for data abstraction. The subsequent data were obtained: authors, publication year, aim, country, variables, sample size, and mean age (see Table 3 for further detail).

thumbnail
Table 3. A short review of included studies for the meta-analysis processes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237838.t003

Data analysis

Meta-analytic method

In a meta-analysis, a summary effect is provided that explains the overall trend. An important subject is then the selection among fixed or random-effects models. As found by Field and Gillett [82], scholars must decide on the proper model earlier based on the included studies and the preferred deductions. According to Borenstein [83], the random-effects model permits that the true effect size may differ from study to study (normally distributed). Particularly, random-effect models are more suitable when studies are carried out by diverse academics in different contexts so that effect sizes can vary randomly [8486]. By contrast, the fixed-effect model depends on the notion that all researches in the meta-analysis have a shared effect size. In this study, a random-effects model was then used since most of the included studies were executed separately, with several samples drawn from various respondents (see Table 4). Furthermore, the researchers managed to conduct heterogeneity tests of effect sizes through their studies, such as 95% confidence interval (CI), Cochran's Q statistic, and I2 statistic [87]. The 95% CI of every estimate was made around the true score correlation. In particular, this study used the Q statistic and the I2 statistic, as I2 was more appropriate for meta-analyses with fewer studies [88]. The I2 statistic was employed to determine the degree of heterogeneity. The estimated heterogeneity variances explained in Table 5. The range of I2 is from 79.297 to 94.891. This proposes that the correlations are fairly heterogeneous.

thumbnail
Table 4. Raw correlation coefficient and two-tailed p-value included for the meta-analytic processes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237838.t004

thumbnail
Table 5. Random effects average correlation and heterogeneity statistics for SCCT constructs and life satisfaction.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237838.t005

Path analysis

In running a path analysis by maximum-likelihood estimate, the authors used the created correlation matrix. Consequently, means and standard deviations for each construct were set to 0 and 1, respectively. According to the revealed corrected meta-analyzed correlations, the meta-analytic path analyses by AMOS 23.0 software were conducted to assess SCCT and the antecedents of life satisfaction. As in past meta-analyses that have also employed additional path analyses [89], the harmonic mean of the sample sizes underpinning each effect size described in the path models was used as the input sample size. Model fit can be evaluated by goodness-of-fit indices; like chi-square/degree of freedom ratio (CMIN/DF), comparative fit index (CFI), goodness-of-fit index (GFI), normed fit index (NFI), and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA).

Results

Study characteristics

Built on this study’s criteria, sixteen studies (20 samples) in the datasets that met the inclusion criteria provided a total of 7,967 respondents. Altogether, four dissertations, one conference paper, and eleven journal articles met the study’s criteria. The oldest study included in this research was published in 2005, while the latest study was printed in 2018. The included research were from eleven countries, where the majority of them were conducted in the United States (US) (k = 8). The sample sizes ranged from 111 [73] and 1,187 [71].

Total relationship between SCCT constructs

The table below shows the correlation between the six constructs of SCCT. Every average weighted correlation (r+) was meaningfully varied from zero (p < .001). Through the studies, life satisfaction had a low correlation with GP (r+ = .291). All 16 correlations revealed significant heterogeneity between studies, with all Q statistics being significant. The relationship strength between SCCT constructs was low to medium, with an effect size ranging from .150 to .647. All dimensions of SCCT, though, were significantly linked (i.e., 95% CI excluded 0). The I2 statistics proposed that the correlations between all SCCT constructs proposed high heterogeneity, as stated by Higgins [90].

Meta-analysis correlation matrix

The results were organized into a correlation matrix (see Table 6), which facilitated to create a base for subsequent path analyses. The associations between life satisfaction and three SCCT constructs were positive (ranging from 0.291 to 0.436), while the correlations of all SCCT constructs were positive. Thus, life satisfaction revealed a low to medium association with three SCCT constructs, i.e., DS, PT, and GP.

thumbnail
Table 6. Meta-analysis correlation matrix among SCCT constructs.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237838.t006

Path analysis

The study examined the SCCT model by using MASEM. Confidence intervals from the meta-analytic correlation matrix are exhibited in Table 5. For model fit, Kline [91] inspired the use of model fit indices, with chi-square/degree of freedom ratio (CMIN/DF), comparative-fit index (CFI), goodness-of-fit index (GFI), and normed fit index (NFI). A rule of thumb for the fit indices is that values at 0.90 or above show acceptable fit [92]. Moreover, the model may be considered as satisfactory if the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) is between 0.03 and 0.08.

Structural model

The fit indices of the original SCCT structural model (Model I) were not satisfactory and did not fit the data across most fit indices, CMIN/DF = 4718.698, p < 0.01, IFI = .668, CFI = .666, NFI = .654, GFI = .841, and RMSEA = .473. As specified by Kline [92], the model suggested an unsuitable fit for the model. After removing GP and OEs, the second model (Model II) includes ESs, SE, DS, PTs, and life satisfaction. As revealed in Table 7, the model was satisfactory and considered as acceptable. This model offered better fit indices: CMIN/DF = 3102.582, p < 0.01, IFI = 0.903, CFI = 0.903, NFI = .904, GFI = .907, and RMSEA = 0.078. Following Kline [92], the model suggested an acceptable and appropriate fit (see Fig 3).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Path analysis for Model I.

All paths are significant at p < .01.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237838.g003

As revealed in Fig 4, DS is positively associated to life satisfaction (β = .296, p-value = 0.000). Therefore, hypothesis 1b is supported. This finding is in line with Loewe’s [93] study, which proved DS as an antecedent of life satisfaction. The structural model showed that PTs positively predicted life satisfaction (β = .310, p-value = 0.000). Thus, hypothesis 1c is supported. This is in line with Ali [94] that PTs may also be key indicators of other facets of individuals’ life, including satisfaction with life. ESs also have a positive correlation with DS (β = .328, p-value = 0.000). Therefore, hypothesis 2a is supported. Thus, the results are consistent with Lent’s [68] study, which indicated social cognitive variables predicted DS. The data in Fig 4 showed that SE (β = .338, p-value = 0.000) was positively linked to DS. Hence, hypothesis 2b is supported. It seems that SE could lead to greater DS among the university students. The present study’s findings can confirm Lent’s [68] finding a positive association between SE and DS (r = .43). This study revealed that PTs were predictors of DS (β = .182, p-value = 0.000). Therefore, hypothesis 2e is accepted. This finding supports Watson and Clark’s [95] conclusion that PTs and DS are related. The structural model showed that PTs (β = .333, p-value = 0.000) were positively associated with SE. Hence, hypothesis 4a is accepted. The findings confirm Navarro’s [96] results, indicating that PTs of engineering students have a direct effect on their SE. As revealed in Fig 4, ESs were significant in justifying the proportion of SE (β = .226, p-value = 0.000). Thus, hypothesis 4b is accepted. This is in line with Lent and Brown [28], whereby ESs help SE to shape one’s adaptive career behavior (e.g., by helping to regulate skill use and help persistence). The current results showed that PTs could significantly influence ESs (β = .331, p-value = 0.000). Thus, hypothesis 6a is accepted. This study found similar results with Lent’s [45] study that showed PTs as affective dispositions and dynamic traits, which continually interacted with ESs. Moreover, ESs, SE, and PTs jointly explained 41.9% variance in DS; and PTs and DS explained 26% of the variance in life satisfaction.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Path analysis for Model II.

All paths are significant at p < .01.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237838.g004

Discussion and conclusions

The study assessed the applicability of SCCT in predicting life satisfaction and also the inter-correlations of SCCT factors by employing a meta-analysis with path analysis. The results did not entirely support the initial SCCT satisfaction model [20]. Thus, GP and OEs are not a factor in the context of higher education students. This confirmed Lent’s [44] study of the non-significant association between OEs and DS. This inconsistency over OEs as explained by Lent [44] is one of the potential issues related to OE measure and/or receiving direct rewards is opposite to the rewards that are expected by students in the long-term, as a greater predictor of DS. The present study excluded OEs (due to model unfit) and this decision is in agreement with Lent [44] and Singley [62] when eliminating OEs. Moreover, the current study also reached the model fit (Model II) by excluding GP. The present results are consistent with Lent’s [68] study, which confirmed the association between GP and DS, but not between GP and ESs. University student samples, which comprised the participants of the present study, might be less likely to testify their engagement in effective pursuit of their goals or unable to be responsive to the types of support (e.g., social, physical, fiscal aspects) that they obtained from the environment, for instance, due to insufficient savings, access to an incomplete variety of career role models, or lack of instrumental support with university applications.

However, with a modified version of the model by removing two constructs, the model was fit for the data. All eight remaining paths created meaningful coefficients, varying in magnitude from small to moderate. The results also yielded theoretically unexpected findings; PTs’ role and their importance in explaining all remaining constructs in the modified SCCT model. The findings proposed that PTs and socio-cognitive factors might not signify distinct, separate causes of satisfaction [97]. Although it is not yet clear that PTs are flexible to modification by psychological intervention, the findings are in line with results from different contexts such as the employees' selection literature that PTs augment overall cognitive ability as main antecedents of life satisfaction [98]. The findings demonstrated the need to incorporate dispositional and socio-cognitive factors and this will be one of the key missions of novel career development theories. Besides, flexible SE proposes a possible objective for intervention attempts since it is realized that a necessary level of ability is also needed to succeed in life satisfaction [99]. SE beliefs to some extent, but not critically, are seen as inspiring students to take on gradually challenging tasks for which success is possible [100]. Findings of the alternative SCCT (Model II) offered support for the view that ESs direct paths to SE as well as DS.

Limitations and avenues for future studies

The present study provided an organized theory-driven analysis of the studies on SCCT as a path to those embarking on future studies and developing theoretical descriptions. Some limitations must be considered and their possible implications on the findings are deliberated. It is vital to mention that this study like other meta-analyses is limited due to the availability of obtainable findings [101]. When authors do not report adequate statistics in primary studies, meta-analysts cannot include these studies in a MASEM; thus, the information from those primary studies is essentially lost. Meta-analytic results may, though, be anticipated to deliver more strong discoveries than those of single studies. Another limitation of meta-analyses is the bias of publications, i.e., nonpublished studies (e.g., unpublished research studies excluded from the study). Although many factors were incorporated in this study, the number of studies inspected and/or the sample size (n) of these studies was not high because various studies inspected the effect sizes for more than one outcome, and as a result of analyzing the data by using a multi-level modeling approach [102]. Besides, due to the significant heterogeneity of the findings, the accurateness of random effect size was reduced and the small datasets added to this study limited the study’s ability to test moderators that may support the analysis. In the case of insufficient data for conducting a moderator inspection, the particular association was omitted [103]. On the other hand, it is admitted that this was not inevitably the case for concept mapping. However, it is vital to say that in these cases, the studies examined were experimental, gave themselves better recognizing effects, should these exist [104]. Besides, meta-analyses are insensitive to causal directions and not sufficient to infer causality [105]; consequently, longitudinal or experimental and quasi-experimental studies are essential to determine causal associations and make more assured generalizations about the strength of SCCT associations [106]. This is due to the reason that longitudinal studies can capture the relationship among antecedents of LS over time–instead of only as simultaneous predictors of LS at a single time. Along with longitudinal research [107], experimental studies could offer a welcome addition to the literature. Furthermore, intervention research can use the present findings to increase the level of LS among students and other cases. These interventions can be designed to help university students to secure new supports or utilize existing ones that may enable them by strengthening SE, DS, and life satisfaction. For example, it may be likely to help students to assert in part of agency over their affecting regulation. Such a tactic might explain life domains/roles that are of specific relevance to students and evaluate their life satisfaction in these domains/roles. However, these interventions must be offered very cautiously because particular features of well-being (e.g., life satisfaction) may be more changeable and vulnerable to nonpersonality effects than the present study and other studies assumed [108]. Meta-analysis cannot replace focused empirical research in addition to it could not adopt the full complexity of interrelationships between constructs [106, 109]. These interrelationships require to be addressed in prospective studies. They have to study other factors that account for variance in life satisfaction beyond that explained by the SCCT antecedents. The constructs included in this meta-analysis are limited to constructs for which appropriate data are accessible. Therefore, the meta-analysis has to be considered as a summary of the most commonly studied elements of life satisfaction. Future studies may inspect other theories/models and the effects of those variables not comprised in the present study (i.e., positive and negative expected emotions). The present study proposes visions into the pros and cons of theory-driven meta-analysis and meta-analytic structural equation modeling in the area of life satisfaction [110]. A vital area for further meta-analytic research is the potential mediating role of SCCT variables in the relationship between life satisfaction and more distant constructs, like SE and DS. The theory-driven meta-analysis offers a method to address unanswered research questions and reach a sense of theoretical transparency of the relationships that career development researchers strive to understand [111]. Study findings showed that students’ personality traits can predict life satisfaction among university students. Previous studies also showed different results, as some studies found a negative influence of some aspects of personality traits on life satisfaction [112], other studies discovered personality traits as significant predictors of life satisfaction [113]. This inconsistency in previous findings reflects that personality traits are different in every culture and country. Future studies need to consider different cultural contexts when applying the SCCT model. It is also possible for prospective studies to test this model by adding cultural related constructs to see whether the view of LS is different in various countries. This will be helpful to redefine LS and also create novel scales of LS for different cultures.

The population sampled in this meta-analysis was university students. Generalization of the findings to other samples (e.g., high school students, employees, etc.) must be performed cautiously. Regardless of these limitations, this study advises that the modified form of SCCT can offer a usable pattern for comprehending and predicting life satisfaction and designing interventions to satisfy students in their university-to-work transition. Last but not least, it would be useful for prospective studies to test SCCT constructs in predicting life satisfaction of university students, which can be moderated by cultural [114], field-specific, measurement, or sampling considerations.

Supporting information

S1 Checklist. PRISMA 2009 checklist (Adapted for KIN 4400).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237838.s001

(DOC)

Acknowledgments

We thank Professor Thomas N. Garavan (Business School, Edinburgh Napier University) for assistance with comments that greatly improved the manuscript.

References

  1. 1. Karavdic S, Baumann M. Positive career attitudes effect on happiness and life satisfaction by master students and graduates. Open J Soc Sci. 2014; 2:15–23.
  2. 2. Pierceall EA, Keim MC. Stress and coping strategies among community college students. Community Coll J Res Pract. 2007; 31(9):703–712.
  3. 3. Eisenbarth C. Does self-esteem moderate the relations among perceived stress, coping, and depression? Coll Stud J. 2012; 46(1):149–57.
  4. 4. Lange T. Scarred from the past or afraid of the future? Unemployment and job satisfaction across European labour markets. Int J Hum Resour Manag. 2013; 24(6):1096–1112.
  5. 5. Santilli S, Marcionetti J, Rochat S, Rossier J, Nota L. Career adaptability, hope, optimism, and life satisfaction in Italian and Swiss adolescents. J Career Dev. 2017; 44(1):62–76.
  6. 6. Cabras C, Mondo M. Coping strategies, optimism, and life satisfaction among first-year university students in Italy: Gender and age differences. High Educ. 2018; 1–12.
  7. 7. Diener E, Suh EM, Lucas RE, Smith HL. Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychol Bull. 1999; 125(2):276–302.
  8. 8. Pavot W, Diener E. Review of the satisfaction with life scale. In: Diener E, editor. Assessing well-being. Dordrecht: Springer; 2009. p. 101–117. (Social Indicators Research; vol. 39).
  9. 9. Abele AE, Hagmaier T, Spurk D. Does career success make you happy? The mediating role of multiple subjective success evaluations. J Happiness Stud. 2016; 17(4):1615–1633.
  10. 10. Rode JC. Job satisfaction and life satisfaction revisited: A longitudinal test of an integrated model. Hum Relat. 2004; 57(9):1205–1230.
  11. 11. Holland JL. Making vocational choices: A theory of vocational personalities and work environments. 3rd ed. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1997.
  12. 12. Super DE. A life-span, life-space approach to career development. J Vocat Behav. 1980; 16(3):282–298.
  13. 13. Dickinson J, Abrams MD, Tokar DM. An examination of the applicability of social cognitive career theory for African American college students. J Career Assess. 2017; 25(1):75–92.
  14. 14. Lent RW, Brown SD, Hackett G. Toward a unifying social cognitive theory of career and academic interest, choice, and performance. J Vocat Behav. 1994; 45(1):79–122.
  15. 15. Byars-Winston A, Estrada Y, Howard C, Davis D, Zalapa J. Influence of social cognitive and ethnic variables on academic goals of underrepresented students in science and engineering: A multiple-groups analysis. J Couns Psychol. 2010; 57(2):1–27.
  16. 16. Lent RW, Lopez FG, Sheu H-B, Lopez AM Jr. Social cognitive predictors of the interests and choices of computing majors: Applicability to underrepresented students. J Vocat Behav. 2011; 78(2):184–192.
  17. 17. Lent RW, Sheu H-B, Gloster CS, Wilkins G. Longitudinal test of the social cognitive model of choice in engineering students at historically Black universities. J Vocat Behav. 2010; 76(3):387–394.
  18. 18. Vondracek FW, Lerner RM, Schulenberg JE. Career development: A life-span developmental approach. New York, NY: Routledge; 2019.
  19. 19. Blustein DL. A match made in heaven? Career development theories and the school-to-work transition. Career Dev Q. 1999; 47:348–52.
  20. 20. Lent RW. Toward a unifying theoretical and practical perspective on well-being and psychosocial adjustment. J Couns Psychol. 2004; 51(4):482–509.
  21. 21. Rottinghaus PJ, Lindley LD, Green MA, Borgen FH. Educational aspirations: The contribution of personality, self-efficacy, and interests. J Vocat Behav. 2002; 61(1):1–19.
  22. 22. Bullock-Yowell E, Andrews L, Buzzetta ME. Explaining career decision-making self-efficacy: Personality, cognitions, and cultural mistrust. Career Dev Q. 2011; 59(5):400–411.
  23. 23. Farooq R. Role of structural equation modeling in scale development. J Adv Manag Res. 2016;13(1):75–91.
  24. 24. Brown SD, Tramayne S, Hoxha D, Telander K, Fan X, Lent RW. Social cognitive predictors of college students’ academic performance and persistence: A meta-analytic path analysis. J Vocat Behav. 2008;72(3):298–308.
  25. 25. Sheu H-B, Lent RW, Brown SD, Miller MJ, Hennessy KD, Duffy RD. Testing the choice model of social cognitive career theory across Holland themes: A meta-analytic path analysis. J Vocat Behav. 2010;76(2):252–264.
  26. 26. Brown SD, Lent RW, Telander K, Tramayne S. Social cognitive career theory, conscientiousness, and work performance: A meta-analytic path analysis. J Vocat Behav. 2011;79(1):81–90.
  27. 27. Kelly ME. Social cognitive career theory as applied to the school-to-work transition [PhD Thesis]. [South Orange, NJ]: Seton Hall University; 2009.
  28. 28. Lent RW, Brown SD. On conceptualizing and assessing social cognitive constructs in career research: A measurement guide. J Career Assess. 2006;14(1):12–35.
  29. 29. Lent RW, Brown SD, Hackett G. Career development from a social cognitive perspective. In: Brown D, Brooks L, Associates, editors. Career choice and development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1996. p. 373–421.
  30. 30. Lent RW, Brown SD. Integrating person and situation perspectives on work satisfaction: A social-cognitive view. J Vocat Behav. 2006; 69(2):236–247.
  31. 31. Blustein DL. The role of work in psychological health and well-being: a conceptual, historical, and public policy perspective. Am Psychol. 2008; 63(4):228. pmid:18473608
  32. 32. Heller D, Watson D, Ilies R. The role of person versus situation in life satisfaction: A critical examination. Psychol Bull. 2004; 130(4):574–600. pmid:15250814
  33. 33. Latham GP, Mawritz MB, Locke EA. Goal setting and control theory: Implications for job search. In: Klehe U-C, Van Hooft EAJ, editors. The Oxford Handbook of Job Loss and Job Search. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2018. p. 8.
  34. 34. Sheldon KM, Kasser T. Goals, congruence, and positive well-being: New empirical support for humanistic theories. J Humanist Psychol. 2001; 41(1):30–50.
  35. 35. Lent RW, Brown SD, Hackett G. Contextual supports and barriers to career choice: A social cognitive analysis. J Couns Psychol. 2000; 47(1):36–49.
  36. 36. Lent RW, Brown SD, Sheu H-B, Schmidt J, Brenner BR, Gloster CS, et al. Social cognitive predictors of academic interests and goals in engineering: Utility for women and students at historically black universities. J Couns Psychol. 2005; 52(1):84–92.
  37. 37. de Coning JA, Rothmann S, Stander MW. Do wage and wage satisfaction compensate for the effects of a dissatisfying job on life satisfaction? SA J Ind Psychol. 2019; 45(1):1–11.
  38. 38. Malouff JM, Thorsteinsson EB, Schutte NS, Bhullar N, Rooke SE. The five-factor model of personality and relationship satisfaction of intimate partners: A meta-analysis. J Res Personal. 2010; 44(1):124–127.
  39. 39. Gibbons MM, Shoffner MF. Prospective first-generation college students: Meeting their needs through social cognitive career theory. Prof Sch Couns. 2004; 8(1):91–97.
  40. 40. Ojeda L. Social cognitive predictors of Mexican American college students’ academic and life satisfaction [PhD Thesis]. [Columbia, MO]: University of Missouri; 2009.
  41. 41. Madiha M, Akhouri D. Self efficacy and life satisfaction among young adults. Int J Appl Sci Eng Manag. 2018; 4(2):19–29.
  42. 42. Wiese BS, Freund AM. Goal progress makes one happy, or does it? Longitudinal findings from the work domain. J Occup Organ Psychol. 2005; 78(2):287–304.
  43. 43. Klug HJP, Maier GW. Linking goal progress and subjective well-being: A meta-analysis. J Happiness Stud. 2015; 16(1):37–65.
  44. 44. Lent RW, Singley D, Sheu H-B, Schmidt JA, Schmidt LC. Relation of social-cognitive factors to academic satisfaction in engineering students. J Career Assess. 2007;15(1):87–97.
  45. 45. Lent RW, do Céu Taveira M, Sheu H-B, Singley D. Social cognitive predictors of academic adjustment and life satisfaction in Portuguese college students: A longitudinal analysis. J Vocat Behav. 2009; 74(2):190–198.
  46. 46. Lent RW, Taveira M do C, Figuera P, Dorio I, Faria S, Gonçalves AM. Test of the social cognitive model of well-being in Spanish college students. J Career Assess. 2017; 25(1):135–143.
  47. 47. Lent RW, Sheu H-B, Singley D, Schmidt JA, Schmidt LC, Gloster CS. Longitudinal relations of self-efficacy to outcome expectations, interests, and major choice goals in engineering students. J Vocat Behav. 2008; 73(2):328–335.
  48. 48. Lent RW, Brown SD. Social cognitive model of career self-management: Toward a unifying view of adaptive career behavior across the life span. J Couns Psychol. 2013; 60(4):557. pmid:23815631
  49. 49. Brown D, Cinamon RG. Personality traits’ effects on self-efficacy and outcome expectations for high school major choice. Int J Educ Vocat Guid. 2016; 16(3):343–61.
  50. 50. Jin L, Watkins D, Yuen M. Personality, career decision self-efficacy and commitment to the career choices process among Chinese graduate students. J Vocat Behav. 2009; 74(1):47–52.
  51. 51. Caprara GV, Vecchione M, Alessandri G, Gerbino M, Barbaranelli C. The contribution of personality traits and self-efficacy beliefs to academic achievement: A longitudinal study. Br J Educ Psychol. 2011; 81(1):78–96.
  52. 52. Hou C, Wu Y, Liu Z. Career decision-making self-efficacy mediates the effect of social support on career adaptability: A longitudinal study. Soc Behav Personal Int J. 2019; 47(5):1–13.
  53. 53. Franco M, Hsiao Y-S, Gnilka PB, Ashby JS. Acculturative stress, social support, and career outcome expectations among international students. Int J Educ Vocat Guid. 2019; 19(2):275–291.
  54. 54. Lent RW. Career-life preparedness: Revisiting career planning and adjustment in the new workplace. Career Dev Q. 2013; 61(1):2–14.
  55. 55. Hui T, Yuen M, Chen G. Career adaptability, self-esteem, and social support among Hong Kong university students. Career Dev Q. 2018; 66(2):94–106.
  56. 56. Garriott PO, Hudyma A, Keene C, Santiago D. Social cognitive predictors of first-and non-first-generation college students’ academic and life satisfaction. J Couns Psychol. 2015; 62(2):253–63. pmid:25730170
  57. 57. Bandura A. Social cognitive theory of personality. In: Pervin LA, John OP, editors. Handbook of personality. 2nd ed. New York & London: The Guilford Press; 1999. p. 154–196.
  58. 58. Ali SR, McWhirter EH, Chronister KM. Self-efficacy and vocational outcome expectations for adolescents of lower socioeconomic status: A pilot study. J Career Assess. 2005; 13(1):40–58.
  59. 59. Parks-Leduc L, Feldman G, Bardi A. Personality traits and personal values: A meta-analysis. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. 2015; 19(1):3–29.
  60. 60. Sutin AR, Costa PT Jr. Reciprocal influences of personality and job characteristics across middle adulthood. J Pers. 2010; 78(1):257–88. pmid:20433619
  61. 61. Schultz DP, Schultz SE. Theories of personality. 11th ed. Boston, MA: Cengage Learning; 2016.
  62. 62. Singley DB, Lent RW, Sheu H-B. Longitudinal test of a social cognitive model of academic and life satisfaction. J Career Assess. 2010; 18(2):133–146.
  63. 63. Meyer J, Fleckenstein J, Retelsdorf J, Köller O. The relationship of personality traits and different measures of domain-specific achievement in upper secondary education. Learn Individ Differ. 2019; 69:45–59.
  64. 64. Cooper H. Research synthesis and meta-analysis: A step-by-step approach. 5th ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage publications; 2015. (Applied social research methods series; vol. 2).
  65. 65. Joanna Briggs Institute. Joanna Briggs Institute Reviewers Manual [Internet]. Adelaide, Australia: Royal Adelaide Hospital; 2011 [cited 2019 Jan 25]. Available from: http://joannabriggs.org/assets/docs/sumari/reviewersmanual-2011.pdf
  66. 66. Moher D, Shamseer L, Clarke M, Ghersi D, Liberati A, Petticrew M, et al. Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Syst Rev. 2015; 4(1):1–9.
  67. 67. Cicolini G, Comparcini D, Simonetti V. Workplace empowerment and nurses’ job satisfaction: a systematic literature review. J Nurs Manag. 2014; 22(7):855–871. pmid:25298049
  68. 68. Lent RW, Singley D, Sheu H-B, Gainor KA, Brenner BR, Treistman D, et al. Social cognitive predictors of domain and life satisfaction: Exploring the theoretical precursors of subjective well-being. J Couns Psychol. 2005; 52(3):429–42.
  69. 69. Silva AD, Lobo CC, Taveira M do C, Bernardo E, Bucuto M. Academic and life satisfaction in Portuguese and Mozambican college students: a comparative study. In: 3rd International Conference of Education, Research and Innovation. Madrid: IATED; 2010. p. 3757–3764.
  70. 70. Işık E, Ulubey E, Kozan S. An examination of the social cognitive model of well-being in Turkish college students. J Vocat Behav. 2018; 106:11–21.
  71. 71. Lee H-S, Flores LY, Navarro RL, Suh HN. Development and validation of the negative outcome expectations scale in engineering (NOES-E). J Career Assess. 2018; 26(1):52–67.
  72. 72. Zalazar-Jaime MF, Moretti LS, Losano MC, Medrano LA. Evaluation of an academic satisfaction model for first-year university students. J Psychol Educ Res. 2017; 25(2):115–140.
  73. 73. Truong NN, Miller MJ. Family and social cognitive predictors of Southeast Asian American college students’ academic satisfaction. J Career Assess. 2018; 26(3):488–502.
  74. 74. Antl SM. Is two always better than one? A moderation analysis of self-concordance and self-efficacy on well-being and goal progress [PhD Thesis]. [Ottawa]: University of Ottawa; 2011.
  75. 75. Ezeofor I. Social cognitive and self-construal predictors of academic satisfaction among African students attending US universities [PhD Thesis]. [College Park, MD]: University of Maryland; 2013.
  76. 76. Sheu H-B, Chong SS, Chen H-F, Lin W-C. Well-being of Taiwanese and Singaporean college students: Cross-cultural validity of a modified social cognitive model. J Couns Psychol. 2014; 61(3):447–60. pmid:25019547
  77. 77. Sheu H-B, Liu Y, Li Y. Well-being of college students in China: Testing a modified social cognitive model. J Career Assess. 2017; 25(1):144–158.
  78. 78. Jezzi MM. The role of attachment in a social cognitive model of social domain satisfaction in college students [PhD Thesis]. [College Park, MD]: University of Maryland; 2016.
  79. 79. Lent RW, Ireland GW, Penn LT, Morris TR, Sappington R. Sources of self-efficacy and outcome expectations for career exploration and decision-making: A test of the social cognitive model of career self-management. J Vocat Behav. 2017; 99:107–117.
  80. 80. Lent RW, do Céu Taveira M, Cristiane V, Sheu H-B, Pinto JC. Test of the social cognitive model of well-being in Portuguese and Brazilian college students. J Vocat Behav. 2018; 109:78–86.
  81. 81. Bergin AJ, Jimmieson NL. The implications of university outcome expectations for student adjustment. J Coll Stud Dev. 2017; 58(5):752–770.
  82. 82. Field AP, Gillett R. How to do a meta-analysis. Br J Math Stat Psychol. 2010; 63(3):665–694.
  83. 83. Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JP, Rothstein HR. A basic introduction to fixed-effect and random-effects models for meta-analysis. Res Synth Methods. 2010; 1(2):97–111. pmid:26061376
  84. 84. Bamberg S, Möser G. Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera: A new meta-analysis of psycho-social determinants of pro-environmental behaviour. J Environ Psychol. 2007; 27(1):14–25.
  85. 85. Cheung MW-L. Meta-analysis: A structural equation modeling approach. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons; 2015.
  86. 86. McEachan RRC, Conner M, Taylor NJ, Lawton RJ. Prospective prediction of health-related behaviours with the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analysis. Health Psychol Rev. 2011; 5(2):97–144.
  87. 87. Borenstein M, Higgins JP, Hedges LV, Rothstein HR. Basics of meta-analysis: I2 is not an absolute measure of heterogeneity. Res Synth Methods. 2017; 8(1):5–18. pmid:28058794
  88. 88. Veroniki AA, Jackson D, Viechtbauer W, Bender R, Knapp G, Kuss O, et al. Recommendations for quantifying the uncertainty in the summary intervention effect and estimating the between-study heterogeneity variance in random-effects meta-analysis. Res Synth Methods. 2015; 7(1):25–27.
  89. 89. Viswesvaran C, Ones DS, Mesmer-Magnus J. Meta-analytic findings: updating research and supporting practice on workplace issues. Career Dev Int. 2017; 22(5):462–468.
  90. 90. Higgins JPT, Jackson D, Barrett JK, Lu G, Ades AE, White IR. Consistency and inconsistency in network meta-analysis: concepts and models for multi-arm studies. Res Synth Methods. 2012; 3(2):98–110. pmid:26062084
  91. 91. Kline RB. Promise and pitfalls of structural equation modeling in gifted research. In: Thompson B, Subotnik RF, editors. Methodologies for Conducting Research on Giftedness. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2010. p. 147–169.
  92. 92. Kline RB. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. 4rd ed. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2015.
  93. 93. Loewe N, Bagherzadeh M, Araya-Castillo L, Thieme C, Batista-Foguet JM. Life domain satisfactions as predictors of overall life satisfaction among workers: Evidence from Chile. Soc Indic Res. 2014; 118(1):71–86.
  94. 94. Ali I. Personality traits, individual innovativeness and satisfaction with life. J Innov Knowl. 2019; 4(1):38–46.
  95. 95. Watson D, Clark LA. On traits and temperament: General and specific factors of emotional experience and their relation to the five-factor model. J Pers. 1992; 60(2):441–476. pmid:1635050
  96. 96. Navarro RL, Flores LY, Lee H-S, Gonzalez R. Testing a longitudinal social cognitive model of intended persistence with engineering students across gender and race/ethnicity. J Vocat Behav. 2014;85(1):146–155.
  97. 97. Suldo SM, Minch DR, Hearon BV. Adolescent life satisfaction and personality characteristics: Investigating relationships using a five factor model. J Happiness Stud. 2015;16(4):965–983.
  98. 98. Roberts BW, Kuncel NR, Shiner R, Caspi A, Goldberg LR. The power of personality: The comparative validity of personality traits, socioeconomic status, and cognitive ability for predicting important life outcomes. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2007; 2(4):313–345. pmid:26151971
  99. 99. Lent RW. A Social Cognitive View of Career Development and Counseling. In: Brown SD, Lent RW, editors. Career development and counseling: Putting theory and research to work. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons Inc; 2005.
  100. 100. Bandura A. Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, NY: Macmillan; 1997.
  101. 101. Hattie J. Visible learning: A synthesis of over 800 meta-analyses relating to achievement. New York, NY: Routledge; 2008.
  102. 102. Snijders TA, Bosker RJ. Multilevel analysis: An introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. London: Sage Publications; 1999.
  103. 103. Card NA. Applied meta-analysis for social science research. New York, NY: Guilford Publications; 2012.
  104. 104. Moghimi D, Zacher H, Scheibe S, Van Yperen NW. The selection, optimization, and compensation model in the work context: A systematic review and meta-analysis of two decades of research. J Organ Behav. 2017; 38(2):247–275.
  105. 105. Bergh DD, Aguinis H, Heavey C, Ketchen DJ, Boyd BK, Su P, et al. Using meta-analytic structural equation modeling to advance strategic management research: Guidelines and an empirical illustration via the strategic leadership-performance relationship. Strateg Manag J. 2016; 37(3):477–497.
  106. 106. Cooper H, Hedges LV, Valentine JC. The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation; 2009.
  107. 107. Dobrow Riza S, Ganzach Y, Liu Y. Time and job satisfaction: A longitudinal study of the differential roles of age and tenure. J Manag. 2018; 44(7):2558–2579.
  108. 108. Diener E, Heintzelman SJ, Kushlev K, Tay L, Wirtz D, Lutes LD, et al. Findings all psychologists should know from the new science on subjective well-being. Can Psychol Can. 2017; 58(2):87–104.
  109. 109. Schlaegel C, and Koenig M. "Determinants of entrepreneurial intent: a meta‐analytic test and integration of competing models." Entr. Theo. Prac. 2014; 38(2): 291–332.
  110. 110. Zaremohzzabieh Z, Ahrari A, Krauss SE, Samah AA, Meng LK, Ariffin Z. Predicting social entrepreneurial intention: A meta-analytic path analysis based on the theory of planned behavior", J Bus Res. 2019; 264–276.
  111. 111. Gartner WB. Is there an elephant in entrepreneurship? Blind assumptions in theory development. In: Cuervo Á, Ribeiro D, Roig S, editors. Entrepreneurship. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer; 2007. p. 229–242.
  112. 112. Hosseinkhanzadeh AA, Taher M. The Relationship between Personality Traits with Life Satisfaction. Soc Mn. 2013; 3(1): 99–105.
  113. 113. Ojeda L, Flores LY, Navarro RL. Social Cognitive Predictors of Mexican American College Students’ Academic and Life Satisfaction. J Coun Psy. 2011; 58(1): 61.71.
  114. 114. Lent RW, Miller MJ, Smith PE, Watford BA, Lim RH, Hui K. Social cognitive predictors of academic persistence and performance in engineering: Applicability across gender and race/ethnicity. J Vocat Behav. 2016; 94:79–88.