Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Sex differences in intrusive memories following trauma

  • Chia-Ming K. Hsu ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    cmkhsu@utas.edu.au

    Affiliation Division of Psychology, School of Medicine, University of Tasmania, Hobart, Tasmania, Australia

  • Birgit Kleim,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, University Hospital Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland

  • Emma L. Nicholson,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Division of Psychology, School of Medicine, University of Tasmania, Hobart, Tasmania, Australia

  • Daniel V. Zuj,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Division of Psychology, School of Medicine, University of Tasmania, Hobart, Tasmania, Australia, Department of Psychology, Swansea University, Singleton Park, Wales, United Kingdom

  • Pippa J. Cushing,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Division of Psychology, School of Medicine, University of Tasmania, Hobart, Tasmania, Australia

  • Kate E. Gray,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Division of Psychology, School of Medicine, University of Tasmania, Hobart, Tasmania, Australia

  • Latifa Clark,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Division of Psychology, School of Medicine, University of Tasmania, Hobart, Tasmania, Australia

  • Kim L. Felmingham

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australia

Abstract

Background

A key mechanism thought to underlie Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) is enhanced emotional memory consolidation. Recent evidence in healthy controls revealed that women have greater negative memory consolidation following stress relative to men. This study examined emotional memory consolidation in women and men with PTSD, and in trauma-exposed and non-trauma controls to test the hypothesis that emotionally negative memory consolidation would be greater in women with PTSD.

Method

One hundred and forty-seven men and women (47 with PTSD, 49 trauma-exposed controls, and 51 non-trauma controls) completed an emotional memory task where they looked at negative, neutral and positive images from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS). Delayed recall and an intrusive memory diary were completed two days later.

Results

Women displayed greater recall, and reported more negative intrusive memories than men. A gender x group interaction effect showed that both women with PTSD and trauma-exposed women reported more intrusive memories than women without trauma exposure or men.

Conclusion

This study provided preliminary evidence of sex differences in intrusive memories in those with PTSD as well as those with a history of trauma exposure. Future research should include measures of sex hormones to further examine sex differences on memory consolidation in the context of trauma exposure and PTSD.

Introduction

Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is characterised by intrusive memories that are involuntary, distressing and difficult to control [1, 2]. Epidemiological studies reveal that women develop PTSD at higher rates than men, even after controlling for type of trauma exposure [36], rate of exposure and history of traumatisation [7]. Less consistent sex differences are found in military samples, however, military populations are subject to selection biases and methodological inconsistencies are common [8]. A meta-analysis found a moderately higher risk of PTSD in women exposed to combat trauma [8] and recently, a large study with more than 34,000 participants revealed a significant increase in PTSD rates in women across a broad range of trauma types, including combat [6].

The mechanisms underlying this higher risk in women remain unclear and are likely to be multifactorial, including social factors such as social and gender roles, cognitive factors, and biological factors. Some researchers have suggested that the sex difference in the prevalence of PTSD is due to the gendered nature of trauma exposure [9], as women are exposed to more sexual and interpersonal violence than men [10], and interpersonal trauma is associated with higher rates of PTSD [11]. However, previous studies have examined risk for PTSD in women and men whilst controlling for exposure to sexual violence and concluded that the increased exposure of females to sexual violence cannot fully account for sex differences [4, 5]. The large epidemiological study noted above found that increased PTSD rates were observed in women across most trauma types (17 out of 19) and that the greater rates of PTSD were not attributable to differences in trauma type, but were likely explained by sex differences in reactivity to the trauma [6]. These latter researchers suggested there may be a particular influence of biological factors, such as stress reactivity and endocrine response, influencing this differential risk for PTSD [6].

A key biological model proposes that PTSD is a result of an over-consolidation of emotional memories following trauma. During the traumatic event, heightened arousal and the release of stress hormones (noradrenaline and cortisol) are thought to lead to an over-consolidation of the trauma memory, resulting in a stronger memory trace that is readily primed by trauma reminders [12]. The subsequent retrieval of the trauma memory accompanied by heightened arousal creates a positive feedback loop that leads to more negative and frequent intrusive memories [13]. Brain imaging and pharmacological studies in PTSD suggested that the reduced activity in medial prefrontal cortex alongside increased amygdala activation and elevated noradrenergic activity might result in enhanced emotional memories [13, 14]. A recent study examined the role of stress hormones during the encoding of emotional images and found, in line with these models, that the interaction of noradrenaline and cortisol during encoding was associated with increased negative intrusive memories in people with PTSD [15].

Empirical evidence in healthy controls suggested that emotional memory consolidation differs in men and women. A study of autobiographical memory revealed that women displayed more accurate and detailed memories of both personal and non-personal experience, especially when the memories involved emotional content, whether positive or negative [16, 17]. Further studies revealed greater memory enhancements in women than men, and this occurs more rapidly and vividly in women [18, 19]. Healthy young women were also found to have greater memory enhancement for neutral stimuli that preceded emotional images compared to men [20].

Many emotional memory studies have employed deliberate recall methods that reflect strategic memory retrieval processes associated with hippocampal activation [21]. When examining memory consolidation in individuals with PTSD or a history of trauma, intrusive memories are also of particular interest and relevance. Intrusive memories are triggered by the environment or occur spontaneously. They are typically sensory-perceptual memories that are vivid [22], fragmented [23] and poorly contextualized into autobiographical memory [24]. The frequency of intrusive memories might be associated with how memories were processed, as healthy individuals reported more intrusions after they attempted to suppress distressing stimuli [25]. Similarly, women with PTSD who attempted to suppress their thoughts experienced a rebound effect in trauma-related thoughts, while the non-PTSD group did not display the same effect [26].

Despite the greater negative memory consolidation and prevalence of PTSD in women, no studies to our knowledge have examined sex differences in intrusive memories among the PTSD population. Accordingly, this study examined recall and intrusive memories of emotional stimuli in women and men with PTSD, in trauma-exposed and non-trauma controls. It was hypothesised that women with PTSD would display particularly greater negative memory recall and negative intrusive memories than men in all groups and women in the control groups.

Method

Participants

Participants were 147 individuals (90 females, 57 males) of Caucasian background recruited from university students and community centres. This included a subset of 58 individuals from a previous memory study examining the role of PTSD and stress hormones [15]. An a-priori power analysis for mixed-factorial ANOVA was conducted in G*Power (Version 3.1.9.3) to determine a sufficient sample size using an alpha value of 0.05, a power of 0.95 and a small to moderate effect size (ηp2 = 0.05). The analysis estimated a required total sample size of 120.

DSM-IV-TR criteria were used in this study, as data collection commenced before the DSM-5 tools were available. The experience of trauma was assessed using Traumatic Events Questionnaire (TEQ; [27]) and PTSD was assessed using the PTSD checklist (PCL; [28]), a standardized and well-validated instrument that enables both ordinal symptom severity and diagnosis of PTSD.

Participants were as PTSD if they had experienced a Criterion A trauma and met DSM-IV-TR diagnostic criteria for PTSD (n = 47, 28 females, 19 males, age M = 31.94, SD = 14.55) according to the PCL or if they reported a subclinical status for PTSD (defined as having a total PCL score >40). Trauma-exposed controls (TC) were defined as those who had experienced a criterion A trauma, but did not meet the diagnostic criteria for PTSD (n = 49, 30 females, 19 males, age M = 27.37, SD = 10.32), and non-trauma controls (NC) had never experienced a criterion A trauma (n = 51, 32 females, 19 males, age M = 22.47, SD = 7.38).

The inclusion of the TC group is to control for effects of trauma exposure versus effects of PTSD. This study excluded those who were aged under 18 or over 65 years, pregnant, reported a history of neurological damage or traumatic brain injury, history of psychosis or mania, and heavy substance abuse (Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test [AUDIT] total score > 15 or were using illicit drugs more regularly than once a week). The research protocol was approved by the Tasmania Social Sciences Human Research Ethics Committee.

Experimental memory task procedure

Each participant completed the study by attending two sessions separated by a two-day interval. To prevent rehearsal or priming effects, participants were informed that the study examined the impact of arousal on perception of emotional images. They were fully debriefed at the end of the second testing session.

In the first session (encoding session), participants provided informed consent, and completed questionnaires to report their moods and symptoms, including the Depression Anxiety and Stress Scales (DASS; [29]), TEQ and PCL. Participants watched 60 images from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS; [30]) in three blocks according to the pictures’ level of valence (1 = extremely unpleasant, 9 = extremely pleasant) and arousal (1 = very calm, 9 = very aroused) ratings. These included 20 emotionally negative images (mean valence = 2.30, mean arousal = 6.18), 20 neutral images (mean valence = 4.99, mean arousal = 2.75), and 20 positive images (mean valence = 7.49, mean arousal = 4.42). The content of the negative images included assault, violence, accidents and natural disasters. The three blocks of images were shown on a 14-inch coloured computer screen in a randomized counterbalanced order. Images within the same block were shown in random order. Each image was shown for six seconds before the screen automatically switched to the next image slide. Participants were asked to direct their full attention to the images. Participants were informed that they would be asked to complete some similar tasks in the second session after two days.

In the second testing session (recall session), participants were given a surprise free recall task in which they were asked to write down descriptions of as many images as they could recall from the encoding session using a standardized procedure [15, 31]. Two independent raters matched these written descriptions to IAPS images. Descriptions that could not be linked to a specific image were recorded as non-responses. All responses were assessed by two independent raters to determine the accuracy of recall with the percentage of agreement 94%. To assess intrusive memories, participants completed an intrusive memory diary following previously published procedures [15]. The experimenter explained the definition and characteristics of intrusive memories–that is, memories of the images they were shown that have occurred involuntarily and spontaneously in the last two days; and it does not include memories that emerged when they were deliberately thinking about this experiment. Participants were asked to write down a description of any intrusive memories of the IAPS images. All descriptions of intrusive images provided in this study could be linked to a specific image. Participants were also asked, “Did you try to suppress or block any of the negative images?” Lastly, participants were asked to rate the valence and arousal of each image using the scales endorsed by IAPS [30].

Statistical analyses

Gender distribution across groups was analysed using a 2 (Sex [female, male]) x 3 (Group [PTSD, TC, NC]) Chi-square test of independence. Type of trauma distribution across groups and gender was analysed using separate 2 (Trauma type [interpersonal, non-interpersonal]) x 3 (Group [PTSD, TC, NC]) and 2 (Trauma type Trauma type [interpersonal, non-interpersonal]) x 2 (Sex [female, male]) Chi-square test of independence. Age and symptom data were analysed by separate 2 (Sex [female, male]) x 3 (Group [PTSD, TC, NC]) univariate analyses of variance (ANOVA). The numbers of recalled images, intrusive memories and picture rating data (subjective ratings for valence and arousal) were analysed by separate 2 (Sex [female, male]) x 3 (Group [PTSD, TC, NC]) x 3 (Valence [negative, neutral, positive]) mixed factorial ANOVAs.

All analyses were performed using SPSS (version 21.0, IBM Corp., Armonk, NY 2012). If there was significant sphericity in the data indicating a violation of the statistical assumption in ANOVA, Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were used when epsilon < .75 and Huynh-Feldt corrections were used when epsilon > .75 [32]. Sidak post-hoc pairwise comparisons were used to further assess specific group differences if there were significant main effects or interaction effects. An alpha value of p < .05 was used for all analyses.

Results

Demographic and clinical data

Table 1 presents the means and standard deviations for clinical data across the groups. In terms of age, there was a group main effect, F(2, 141) = 7.90, p = .001, such that age was significantly greater in the PTSD group (M = 31.94, SD = 14.55) than the NC group (M = 22.47, SD = 7.38). To control for this group difference, analyses were repeated taking age as a covariate (for ANCOVA analyses, see S1 File). There was no significant difference in age for men and women, F(1, 141) = 3.44, p = .066, and no significant interaction in age between group and sex, F(2, 141) = 0.51, p = .60. The distribution of males and females across groups was examined using Chi-square analysis, which revealed that the association between sex and group was non-significant, X2 (2, N = 147) = .10, p = .95.

thumbnail
Table 1. Mean scores, standard deviations and F-statistics of clinical scales across sex and groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208575.t001

Clinical data were assessed by separate 2 x 3 ANOVAs with sex (female, male) and group (PTSD, TC, NC) as between-subjects factors, which revealed a group main effect for each scale (see Table 1 for statistics). Sidak post-hoc analysis indicated that depression, anxiety and stress scales share a similar profile whereby the PTSD group scored significantly higher than both TC and NC groups (p < .001), but the difference between TC and NC groups were non-significant (p > .37). Sidak post-hoc analysis indicated that PCL total score was higher in the PTSD group than the TC group (p < .001, 95% CI [19.61, 27.98]), and higher in the TC group than the NC group (p = .015, 95% CI [0.74, 9.00]). There is a marginal sex main effect in stress (p = .053), but no significant sex main effect in these demographic scales. The group x sex interaction was non-significant for each scale (p > .05).

Intentional recall

A mixed-design ANOVA with valence (negative, neutral, positive) as a within-subjects factor and sex (female, male) and group (PTSD, TC, NC) as between-subjects factors revealed effected reported in Table 2. There was a significant valence main effect such that participants recalled more negative images (M = 4.76, SE = 0.17) than both positive images (M = 2.43, SE = 0.14; p < .001, 95% CI [1.86, 2.80]) and neutral images (M = 1.82, SE = 0.13; p < .001, 95% CI [2.55, 3.33]); and more positive images than neutral images (p = .001, 95% CI [0.20, 1.02]). There was also a significant sex main effect, where images were recalled more by women (M = 3.43, SE = 0.13) than by men (M = 2.57, SE = 0.17). The group main effect was non-significant. The predicted interaction between sex, group and valence was also non-significant. All other effects were not significant.

thumbnail
Table 2. Statistics of effects on intentional recall and intrusive memory.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208575.t002

Intrusive memory

A mixed-design ANOVA with valence (negative, neutral, positive) as a within-subjects factor and sex (female, male) and group (PTSD, TC, NC) as between-subjects factors revealed effects reported in Table 2. The was a significant valence main effect such that participants reported more intrusions to negative images (M = 0.55, SE = 0.07) than both positive images (M = 0.09, SE = 0.03; p < .001, 95% CI [0.28, 0.64]) and neutral images (M = 0.04, SE = 0.02; p < .001, 95% CI [0.32, 0.70]); but there was no significant difference between the positive and neutral images (p = .32). The analysis revealed a sex main effect, where more intrusive images were reported by women (M = 0.31, SE = 0.04) than men (M = 0.14, SE = 0.05). This sex main effect was qualified by a significant interaction between sex and valence, such that more negative intrusive images were reported by women (M = 0.73, SE = 0.09) than men (M = 0.37, SE = 0.12; p = .016, 95% CI [0.07, 0.66]). The number of positive and neutral intrusive images reported by women and men were not significantly different.

There was also a group main effect, such that more intrusive images were reported by the PTSD group (M = 0.44, SE = 0.05) than the TC (M = 0.17, SE = 0.05; p = .001, 95% CI [0.10, 0.44]) and the NC (M = 0.06, SE = 0.05; p < .001, 95% CI [0.21, 0.56]) groups; and there was no significant difference between the TC and the NC groups (p = .300). This was qualified by interactions between group and valence, which revealed that more negative intrusions specifically were reported by the PTSD group (M = 1.15, SE = 0.13) compared to the TC group (M = 0.35, SE = 0.13; p < .001, 95% CI [0.39, 1.28]) and the NC group (M = 0.11, SE = 0.13; p < .001, 95% CI [0.64, 1.52]); and there was no significant difference between TC and NC groups (p = .454). The frequency of intrusions to the positive or neutral images did not differ among groups (p > .05).

Importantly, there was a significant interaction effect between sex and group (Fig 1). Women reported more intrusive memories than men in the PTSD group (women: M = 0.57, SE = .07; men: M = 0.32, SE = 0.08; p = .014, 95% CI [0.05, 0.46]) and the TC group (women: M = 0.31, SE = .06; men: M = 0.04, SE = 0.08; p = .007, 95% CI [0.08, 0.48]), but there were no sex differences in the NC group (p = .572). The three-way interaction among sex, group and valence was not significant.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Mean numbers of intrusive images reported two days post-learning by males and females among the PTSD, trauma-exposed control (TC) and non-trauma-exposed control (NC) groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208575.g001

Memory suppression

A Chi-square test of independence indicated a significant association between group and memory suppression, X2 (2, N = 137) = 27.78, p < .001. The PTSD group reported higher rates of memory suppression than both TE and NC groups. The association between sex and memory suppression was also significant, X2 (1, N = 137) = 5.23, p = .022, where women reported higher rates of memory suppression than men. The frequencies for suppression across groups and sex are displayed in Table 3. The analysis for individual-group subsamples revealed that more women reported memory suppression than men in the TC group, X2 (1, N = 47) = 4.31, p = .038, but such an association was not significant in the PTSD and the NC groups.

thumbnail
Table 3. Number of participants reporting attempts to suppress emotional images across gender and groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208575.t003

Picture rating

The mixed design ANOVA revealed a significant valence main effect on subjective ratings of emotional valence, F(1.13, 58.82) = 209.50, p < .001, ηp2 = .801. Negative images (M = 2.87, SE = 0.13) were rated lower than neutral images (M = 5.00, SE = 0.03; p < .001, 95% CI [1.81, 2.45]), and neutral images were rated lower than positive images (M = 6.43, SE = 0.13; p < .001, 95% CI [1.11, 1.76]). The mixed design ANOVA also revealed a significant valence main effect on subjective ratings of emotional arousal, F(1.90, 252.00) = 251.63, p < .001, ηp2 = .654. Negative images (M = 5.57, SE = 0.18) were rated higher than positive images (M = 3.96, SE = 0.16; p < .001, 95% CI [1.24, 1.98]), and positive images were rated higher than neutral images (M = 1.77, SE = 0.10; p < .001, 95% CI [1.81, 2.56]).

Discussion

This study examined whether there was greater negative memory consolidation in women with PTSD and/or with a history of trauma compared to men, and specifically examined if this was the case in two distinct forms of emotional memory: intentional recall (indexing strategic retrieval processes) and intrusive memory. Replicating previous research, negative images resulted in greater intentional recall and intrusive memories than neutral or positive images. Individuals with PTSD revealed significantly greater negative intrusive memories than the trauma-exposed or non-trauma controls, and women displayed significantly more intentional recall generally and reported significantly more intrusive memories of negative images than men (regardless of group). Key findings revealed sex differences in intrusive memories following trauma exposure as women with PTSD reported more intrusive images compared to men with PTSD, and this difference was not apparent in non-trauma-exposed individuals. Interestingly, not only did women with PTSD report more intrusions than men with PTSD, these sex differences were also apparent in the trauma-exposed control group. These findings support the hypothesis that women with PTSD will display significantly greater negative intrusive memories than men, but no significant sex x group effects were found for intentional recall.

The valence main effects for intentional recall and intrusive memories replicate many previous emotional memory studies which reveal that negative emotional images are associated with greater memory consolidation, leading to greater memory recall [33]. The interaction between group and valence for intrusive memories revealed that the PTSD group had significantly greater negative intrusive memories than both control groups, a finding that confirms previous research [15, 34]. This pattern is consistent with theoretical models highlighting that negative intrusive memories are key mechanisms involved in PTSD [12, 35].

There were sex main effects for intentional recall and intrusive memory revealing that women recalled more IAPS images and reported more intrusive memories than men. This main effect for intentional recall was found across valence, and a sex x valence interaction for intrusive memories showed that women had more intrusive memories specifically of negative images than men, but not positive or neutral images. These sex effects accord with an analogue emotional film study of healthy controls which found that women reported more intrusive memories than men [36]. This is also in line with the finding that women reported more involuntary memories than men when the task involved autobiographical memory [37, 38]. Given evidence that thought suppression increases the likelihood of intrusions [39, 40], it is possible that the greater suppression reported by women in the trauma-exposed group might contribute to the increased number of intrusive memories. This requires future research to specifically examine this question.

The key finding of the current study was the sex differences in intrusive memory observed in individuals with a history of trauma. The sex x group interaction revealed that women reported more intrusive memories than men, but only in those with PTSD or with a history of trauma, as this was not evident in the non-trauma controls. The fact that this gender effect was observed in both the trauma-exposed and PTSD groups is interesting to note, and suggests that trauma exposure per se may influence memory consolidation more in women than in men. Neuroimaging evidence also revealed that trauma exposure affects neural activity (in limbic regions involved in emotional memory) independent of PTSD symptoms [41]. Supporting evidence is found in a previous fMRI study in which women with PTSD or following trauma exposure were found to have significantly greater blood oxygen level in brainstem and midbrain arousal networks relative to men [42]. Therefore, trauma exposure may play a role in intrusion development.

A potential common mechanism thought to underlie intrusions which is also characteristic of both PTSD and trauma exposure, is arousal [14, 43, 44]. A key model of intrusions suggests that arousal during encoding is a key mechanism underlying intrusions [44] and arousal is also core to trauma exposure and a key characteristic of PTSD. Similar findings were revealed in healthy populations, as stress-induced cortisol during encoding was found to predict greater negative memory consolidation in healthy women [45]. It should be noted that this gender x group interaction only partially confirmed our hypotheses, as the increase of intrusive memories in women was not specific to negative images as predicted, but was a generalized effect across valence. Future studies should examine the impact of stress hormones to assess the potential role of arousal in intrusive memories.

Sex hormone is a potential influence on intrusive memories that were not examined in this study. Previous studies in women have found evidence that the luteal phase (characterised by high progesterone) and stress induction were associated with greater intentional recall [46] and intrusive memories [47, 48]. Similarly, another study found that intrusive memories of emotional films were reported more by women in the luteal phase than in the follicular phase, and those intrusions significantly correlated with progesterone [49]. In contrast, other studies found that estradiol levels influenced intrusive memories [50, 51], and one study found that estradiol-to-progesterone ratio predicted intrusions [48]. It is important to note that most of these studies recruited non-traumatized individuals. Although sex hormones may influence intrusive memories, the inconsistent finding highlight the need for future research to further delineate the relationship between specific hormones and intrusive memories. Given that both intrusive memories and estradiol are associated with limbic network activation [52, 53], and that estradiol modulates noradrenergic activity which is associated with intrusive memories [15, 54, 55], it is important to examine the potential interaction of sex and stress hormones on intrusive memories following trauma exposure.

In contrast to the intrusive memory data, the intentional recall measure did not reveal a significant group main effect or significant interaction with sex. Therefore, there was no evidence of greater recall of negative images in the PTSD group. This result is contrary to the common assumption that people with PTSD will recall more emotional than neutral stimuli, but the result is consistent with previous experimental memory studies [15, 56]. This may have resulted in part from the use of relatively generic emotional and negative stimuli (from the IAPS) which were not specifically tailored to each individual’s traumatic experience. The only significant effects for the intentional recall data were a valence main effect and a sex main effect. The sex main effect indicated that women recalled more images than men irrespective of valence, which is consistent with the previous finding that women had better semantic memory [57], included more accurate and detailed episodic memory and autobiographical memory [58, 59]. Similarly, the valence main effect revealed that negative and positive stimuli were better recalled than neutral images (with negative images recalled most of all), which is consistent with many previous emotional memory studies [36, 45, 60].

As noted previously, a limitation of this study is the lack of stress hormone and sex hormone data which should be included in future research studies. However, this study contributes preliminary findings of sex differences which provides a further rationale for examining sex hormone effects. A second limitation is that we examined sex differences (a biological distinction) without examining gender effects and the potential influence of gender and social roles. It is recognized there is increasing diversity in gender identification and there is a potentially powerful impact of social roles on emotional functioning, so future studies should include specific measures of gender and social roles. While it was speculated that women might recall more counts of traumatic experiences than men, it is a limitation that this study did not quantify the amount of trauma exposure. However, the conceptual and methodological issues in assessing trauma history highlight multidimensional challenges in obtaining reliable information [61], particularly when a person attempts to recall a full personal history. Further, to account for the significant age difference between the PTSD group and the non-trauma control group, the analyses of memory data were repeated with age as a covariate and revealed no change to the key factorial effects (see S1 File). Finally, the small number of intrusive memories in the data is challenging and may have led to floor effects in our data, however this is common in many intrusive memory studies. It is noted that this study measured the number of intrusive memories of the emotional stimuli presented to the participants, rather than intrusive memories of each individual’s personal trauma.

In conclusion, this study provides preliminary evidence of sex differences in intrusive memories of emotional images among individuals with a history of trauma. Specifically, women following trauma exposure, including those with PTSD, reported more intrusive memories of emotional images compared to trauma-exposed males and non-trauma controls. However, this effect was small, and it was not specific to negative stimuli. This preliminary finding highlights the need for future research to consider sex differences in memory, and to specifically investigate the impact of sex hormones and stress hormones on intrusive memories following trauma.

Supporting information

S1 File. A ZIP file containing the dataset and statistical analysis results reported in this manuscript.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208575.s001

(ZIP)

Acknowledgments

This project was conducted at the Cognitive Neuroscience Lab of University of Tasmania. The authors thank Samuel Sauvage for writing the computer program for the memory task.

References

  1. 1. Clark DA, Rhyno S. Unwanted Intrusive Thoughts in Nonclinical Individuals: Implications for Clinical Disorders. 2005.
  2. 2. Brewin CR. Intrusive autobiographical memories in depression and post-traumatic stress disorder. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 1998;12(4):359–70.
  3. 3. Goldstein RB, Smith SM, Chou SP, Saha TD, Jung J, Zhang H, et al. The epidemiology of DSM-5 posttraumatic stress disorder in the United States: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions-III. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2016;51(8):1137–48. pmid:27106853; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4980174.
  4. 4. Olff M, Langeland W, Draijer N, Gersons BP. Gender differences in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychol Bull. 2007;133(2):183–204. Epub 2007/03/07. pmid:17338596.
  5. 5. Tolin DF, Foa EB. Sex differences in trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder: a quantitative review of 25 years of research. Psychol Bull. 2006;132(6):959–92. Epub 2006/11/01. pmid:17073529.
  6. 6. Blanco C, Hoertel N, Wall MM, Franco S, Peyre H, Neria Y, et al. Toward Understanding Sex Differences in the Prevalence of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Results From the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. J Clin Psychiatry. 2018;79(2). Epub 2018/04/17. pmid:29659210.
  7. 7. Breslau N, Davis GC, Andreski P, Peterson EL, Schultz LR. Sex differences in posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54(11):1044–8. Epub 1997/11/21. pmid:9366662.
  8. 8. Crum-Cianflone NF, Jacobson I. Gender differences of postdeployment post-traumatic stress disorder among service members and veterans of the Iraq and Afghanistan conflicts. Epidemiol Rev. 2014;36:5–18. Epub 2013/08/31. pmid:23988441.
  9. 9. Breslau N. Gender differences in trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder. The journal of gender-specific medicine: JGSM: the official journal of the Partnership for Women's Health at Columbia. 2002;5(1):34–40.
  10. 10. Kessler RC, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, Alonso J, Benjet C, Bromet EJ, Cardoso G, et al. Trauma and PTSD in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. Eur J Psychotraumatol. 2017;8(sup5):1353383. Epub 2017/10/28. pmid:29075426; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC5632781.
  11. 11. Forbes D, Lockwood E, Phelps A, Wade D, Creamer M, Bryant RA, et al. Trauma at the hands of another: distinguishing PTSD patterns following intimate and nonintimate interpersonal and noninterpersonal trauma in a nationally representative sample. The Journal of clinical psychiatry. 2014.
  12. 12. Pitman RK, Delahanty DL. Conceptually driven pharmacologic approaches to acute trauma. CNS Spectr. 2005;10(2):99–106. Epub 2005/02/03. pmid:15685120.
  13. 13. Elzinga BM, Bremner JD. Are the neural substrates of memory the final common pathway in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD)? J Affect Disord. 2002;70(1):1–17. Epub 2002/07/13. pmid:12113915.
  14. 14. Pitman RK, Rasmusson AM, Koenen KC, Shin LM, Orr SP, Gilbertson MW, et al. Biological studies of post-traumatic stress disorder. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13(11):769–87. pmid:23047775; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4951157.
  15. 15. Nicholson EL, Bryant RA, Felmingham KL. Interaction of noradrenaline and cortisol predicts negative intrusive memories in posttraumatic stress disorder. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014;112:204–11. Epub 2013/12/04. pmid:24296460.
  16. 16. Davis PJ. Gender differences in autobiographical memory for childhood emotional experiences. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1999;76(3):498–510. Epub 1999/04/02. pmid:10101879.
  17. 17. Seidlitz L, Diener E. Sex differences in the recall of affective experiences. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;74(1):262–71. Epub 1998/02/11. pmid:9457787.
  18. 18. Hamann S. Sex differences in the responses of the human amygdala. Neuroscientist. 2005;11(4):288–93. Epub 2005/08/03. pmid:16061516.
  19. 19. Hamann S, Canli T. Individual differences in emotion processing. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004;14(2):233–8. Epub 2004/04/15. pmid:15082330.
  20. 20. Clewett D, Sakaki M, Huang R, Nielsen SE, Mather M. Arousal amplifies biased competition between high and low priority memories more in women than in men: The role of elevated noradrenergic activity. Psychoneuroendocrino. 2017;80:80–91. Epub 2017/03/23. pmid:28324703; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC5502746.
  21. 21. Richardson MP, Strange BA, Dolan RJ. Encoding of emotional memories depends on amygdala and hippocampus and their interactions. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7(3):278–85. Epub 2004/02/06. pmid:14758364.
  22. 22. Ehlers A, Hackmann A, Michael T. Intrusive re-experiencing in post-traumatic stress disorder: phenomenology, theory, and therapy. Memory. 2004;12(4):403–15. Epub 2004/10/19. pmid:15487537.
  23. 23. Ehlers A, Hackmann A, Steil R, Clohessy S, Wenninger K, Winter H. The nature of intrusive memories after trauma: the warning signal hypothesis. Behav Res Ther. 2002;40(9):995–1002. Epub 2002/09/26. pmid:12296496.
  24. 24. Kleim B, Wallott F, Ehlers A. Are Trauma Memories Disjointed from other Autobiographical Memories in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder? An Experimental Investigation. Behav Cogn Psychother. 2008;36(2):221–34. Epub 2008/03/01. pmid:21241538; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC2889292.
  25. 25. Davies MI, Clark DM. Thought suppression produces a rebound effect with analogue post-traumatic intrusions. Behav Res Ther. 1998;36(6):571–82. Epub 1998/07/02. pmid:9648331.
  26. 26. Shipherd JC, Beck JG. The effects of suppressing trauma-related thoughts on women with rape-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Behav Res Ther. 1999;37(2):99–112. Epub 1999/02/17. pmid:9990742.
  27. 27. Vrana S, Lauterbach D. Prevalence of traumatic events and post-traumatic psychological symptoms in a nonclinical sample of college students. J Trauma Stress. 1994;7(2):289–302. Epub 1994/04/01. pmid:8012748.
  28. 28. Weathers FW, Litz BT, Huska JA, Keane TM. The PTSD Checklist-Vivilian Version (PCL-C) for DSM-IV. Boston: National Center for PTSD, Behavioural Sciences Division; 1994.
  29. 29. Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH. The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behav Res Ther. 1995;33(3):335–43. Epub 1995/03/01. pmid:7726811.
  30. 30. Lang P, Bradley M, Cuthbert B. International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida; 2008.
  31. 31. Segal SK, Cahill L. Endogenous noradrenergic activation and memory for emotional material in men and women. Psychoneuroendocrino. 2009;34(9):1263–71. Epub 2009/06/10. pmid:19505766.
  32. 32. Girden ER. ANOVA: Repeated measures: Sage; 1992.
  33. 33. McGaugh JL. Memory—a century of consolidation. Science. 2000;287(5451):248–51. Epub 2000/01/15. pmid:10634773.
  34. 34. Solberg O, Blix I, Heir T. The aftermath of terrorism: posttraumatic stress and functional impairment after the 2011 Oslo bombing. Front Psychol. 2015;6:1156. Epub 2015/08/25. pmid:26300833; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4528096.
  35. 35. Ehlers A, Clark DM. A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behav Res Ther. 2000;38(4):319–45. Epub 2000/04/13. pmid:10761279.
  36. 36. Ferree NK, Cahill L. Post-event spontaneous intrusive recollections and strength of memory for emotional events in men and women. Conscious Cogn. 2009;18(1):126–34. Epub 2009/01/10. pmid:19131257; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC2691747.
  37. 37. Brewin CR, Soni M. Gender, personality, and involuntary autobiographical memory. Memory. 2011;19(6):559–65. Epub 2011/09/17. pmid:21919583.
  38. 38. Rubin DC, Berntsen D. The frequency of voluntary and involuntary autobiographical memories across the life span. Mem Cognit. 2009;37(5):679–88. Epub 2009/06/03. pmid:19487759; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3044938.
  39. 39. Shepherd L, Wild J. Emotion regulation, physiological arousal and PTSD symptoms in trauma-exposed individuals. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2014;45(3):360–7. Epub 2014/04/15. pmid:24727342; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4053589.
  40. 40. Wenzlaff RM, Wegner DM. Thought suppression. Annu Rev Psychol. 2000;51:59–91. Epub 2001/02/07. pmid:10751965.
  41. 41. Stark EA, Parsons CE, Van Hartevelt TJ, Charquero-Ballester M, McManners H, Ehlers A, et al. Post-traumatic stress influences the brain even in the absence of symptoms: A systematic, quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;56:207–21. pmid:26192104.
  42. 42. Felmingham K, Williams LM, Kemp AH, Liddell B, Falconer E, Peduto A, et al. Neural responses to masked fear faces: sex differences and trauma exposure in posttraumatic stress disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2010;119(1):241–7. Epub 2010/02/10. pmid:20141261.
  43. 43. Nixon RD, Bryant RA. Induced arousal and reexperiencing in acute stress disorder. J Anxiety Disord. 2005;19(5):587–94. Epub 2005/03/08. pmid:15749575.
  44. 44. Brewin CR. The nature and significance of memory disturbance in posttraumatic stress disorder. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2011;7:203–27. Epub 2011/01/12. pmid:21219190.
  45. 45. Felmingham KL, Tran TP, Fong WC, Bryant RA. Sex differences in emotional memory consolidation: the effect of stress-induced salivary alpha-amylase and cortisol. Biol Psychol. 2012;89(3):539–44. pmid:22248928.
  46. 46. Felmingham KL, Fong WC, Bryant RA. The impact of progesterone on memory consolidation of threatening images in women. Psychoneuroendocrino. 2012;37(11):1896–900. pmid:22534403.
  47. 47. Bryant RA, Felmingham KL, Silove D, Creamer M, O'Donnell M, McFarlane AC. The association between menstrual cycle and traumatic memories. J Affect Disord. 2011;131(1–3):398–401. Epub 2010/11/26. pmid:21093927.
  48. 48. Soni M, Curran VH, Kamboj SK. Identification of a narrow post-ovulatory window of vulnerability to distressing involuntary memories in healthy women. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2013;104:32–8. Epub 2013/04/25. pmid:23611942.
  49. 49. Ferree NK, Kamat R, Cahill L. Influences of menstrual cycle position and sex hormone levels on spontaneous intrusive recollections following emotional stimuli. Conscious Cogn. 2011;20(4):1154–62. Epub 2011/03/01. pmid:21353599; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3126908.
  50. 50. Cheung J, Chervonsky L, Felmingham KL, Bryant RA. The role of estrogen in intrusive memories. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2013;106:87–94. Epub 2013/07/31. pmid:23891994.
  51. 51. Wegerer M, Kerschbaum H, Blechert J, Wilhelm FH. Low levels of estradiol are associated with elevated conditioned responding during fear extinction and with intrusive memories in daily life. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014;116:145–54. Epub 2014/12/03. pmid:25463649; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4256064.
  52. 52. Albert K, Pruessner J, Newhouse P. Estradiol levels modulate brain activity and negative responses to psychosocial stress across the menstrual cycle. Psychoneuroendocrino. 2015;59:14–24. Epub 2015/07/01. pmid:26123902; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4492530.
  53. 53. Bourne C, Mackay CE, Holmes EA. The neural basis of flashback formation: the impact of viewing trauma. Psychol Med. 2013;43(7):1521–32. Epub 2012/11/23. pmid:23171530; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3806039.
  54. 54. Bangasser DA, Wiersielis KR, Khantsis S. Sex differences in the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system and its regulation by stress. Brain Res. 2016;1641(Pt B):177–88. Epub 2015/11/27. pmid:26607253; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4875880.
  55. 55. Barel E, Abu-Shkara R, Colodner R, Masalha R, Mahagna L, Zemel OC, et al. Gonadal hormones modulate the HPA-axis and the SNS in response to psychosocial stress. J Neurosci Res. 2018;96(8):1388–97. Epub 2018/05/10. pmid:29741787.
  56. 56. Brohawn KH, Offringa R, Pfaff DL, Hughes KC, Shin LM. The neural correlates of emotional memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68(11):1023–30. Epub 2010/09/22. pmid:20855060.
  57. 57. Andreano JM, Cahill L. Sex influences on the neurobiology of learning and memory. Learn Mem. 2009;16(4):248–66. Epub 2009/03/26. pmid:19318467.
  58. 58. Pillemer DB, Wink P, DiDonato TE, Sanborn RL. Gender differences in autobiographical memory styles of older adults. Memory. 2003;11(6):525–32. Epub 2004/02/26. pmid:14982120.
  59. 59. Pohl RF, Bender M, Lachmann G. Hindsight bias around the world. Exp Psychol. 2002;49(4):270–82. Epub 2002/11/29. pmid:12455333.
  60. 60. Spalek K, Fastenrath M, Ackermann S, Auschra B, Coynel D, Frey J, et al. Sex-dependent dissociation between emotional appraisal and memory: a large-scale behavioral and fMRI study. J Neurosci. 2015;35(3):920–35. pmid:25609611.
  61. 61. Goodman LA, Corcoran C, Turner K, Yuan N, Green BL. Assessing traumatic event exposure: general issues and preliminary findings for the Stressful Life Events Screening Questionnaire. J Trauma Stress. 1998;11(3):521–42. Epub 1998/08/05. pmid:9690191.