Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Reliability of the parameters of the power-duration relationship using maximal effort time-trials under laboratory conditions

  • Christoph Triska ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Christoph Triska, Bettina Karsten, Bernd Heidegger, Bernhard Koller-Zeisler, Bernhard Prinz, Alfred Nimmerichter, Harald Tschan

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    christoph.triska@univie.ac.at

    Affiliation Centre for Sport Science and University Sports, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

  • Bettina Karsten ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Christoph Triska, Bettina Karsten, Bernd Heidegger, Bernhard Koller-Zeisler, Bernhard Prinz, Alfred Nimmerichter, Harald Tschan

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Life and Sport Science, University of Greenwich, Kent, United Kingdom, Department of Exercise and Sport Science, LUNEX International University of Health, Exercise and Sports, Differdingen, Luxembourg

  • Bernd Heidegger ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Christoph Triska, Bettina Karsten, Bernd Heidegger, Bernhard Koller-Zeisler, Bernhard Prinz, Alfred Nimmerichter, Harald Tschan

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation

    Affiliation Centre for Sport Science and University Sports, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

  • Bernhard Koller-Zeisler ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Christoph Triska, Bettina Karsten, Bernd Heidegger, Bernhard Koller-Zeisler, Bernhard Prinz, Alfred Nimmerichter, Harald Tschan

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Resources

    Affiliations Centre for Sport Science and University Sports, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria, Austrian Institute of Sports Medicine, Vienna, Austria

  • Bernhard Prinz ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Christoph Triska, Bettina Karsten, Bernd Heidegger, Bernhard Koller-Zeisler, Bernhard Prinz, Alfred Nimmerichter, Harald Tschan

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Resources

    Affiliation Training and Sports Sciences, University of Applied Sciences, Wr. Neustadt, Austria

  • Alfred Nimmerichter ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Christoph Triska, Bettina Karsten, Bernd Heidegger, Bernhard Koller-Zeisler, Bernhard Prinz, Alfred Nimmerichter, Harald Tschan

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Training and Sports Sciences, University of Applied Sciences, Wr. Neustadt, Austria

  • Harald Tschan

    Contributed equally to this work with: Christoph Triska, Bettina Karsten, Bernd Heidegger, Bernhard Koller-Zeisler, Bernhard Prinz, Alfred Nimmerichter, Harald Tschan

    Roles Conceptualization, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Centre for Sport Science and University Sports, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to assess the reliability of critical power (CP) and the total amount of work accomplished above CP () across repeated tests using ecologically valid maximal effort time-trials (TT) under laboratory conditions. After an initial incremental exercise test, ten well-trained male triathletes (age: 28.5 ± 4.7 years; body mass: 73.3 ± 7.9 kg; height: 1.80 ± 0.07 m; maximal aerobic power [MAP]: 329 ± 41 W) performed three testing sessions (Familiarization, Test I and Test II) each comprising three TT (12, 7, and 3 min with a passive recovery of 60 min between trials). CP and were determined using a linear regression of power vs. the inverse of time (1/t) (P = ∙ 1/t + CP). A repeated-measures ANOVA was used to detect differences in CP and and reliability was assessed using the intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC) and the coefficient of variation (CoV). CP and values were not significantly different between repeated tests (P = 0.171 and P = 0.078 for CP and , respectively). The ICC between Familiarization and Test I was r = 0.86 (CP) and r = 0.58 () and between Tests I and II it was r = 0.94 (CP) and r = 0.95 (). The CoV notably decreased from 4.1% to 2.6% and from 25.3% to 8.2% for CP and , respectively. Despite the non-significant differences for both parameter estimates between Familiarization, Test I, and Test II, ICC and CoV values improved notably after the familiarization trial. Our novel findings indicate that for both, CP and a familiarization trial increased reliability. It is therefore advisable to familiarize well-trained athletes when determining the power-duration relationship using TT under laboratory conditions.

Introduction

A reliable determination of critical power (CP) and the total amount of work accomplished above CP until task failure () has long been a question of interest. Whilst CP represents a work rate that can be sustained for a long time without a continuous loss of metabolic (e.g. pH, phosphocreatine) and systemic (blood lactate concentration, ) homeostasis [1], is an equivalent for a finite amount of work that can be accomplished above CP [2, 3]. Originally, the determination of CP and requires 3 to 5 constant-power time-to-exhaustion trials (TTE) on a cycle ergometer, leading to exhaustion within 2–15 min [e.g. 4, 5–7]. However, TTE have no predefined endpoints and therefore are not comparable to the tasks athletes are confronted with during competition.

Although TTE provide reliable results for CP (r = 0.90–0.96) [810], has consistently shown to be less reliable across repeated tests (r = 0.64–0.84) [810]. It should be noted that small differences in time-to-exhaustion between repeated trials might alter the parameter estimates (in particular ) [11, 12]. Therefore, TTE efforts should be used with caution when trying to detect small training induced changes in an athlete’s performance [13].

Fixed duration time-trials (TT) with a known endpoint are typically used when CP and are determined under field conditions [4, 6, 7, 12, 14]. TT are often described as an optimal approximation of real-world conditions and therefore, have a higher ecological validity compared to TTE [47, 14, 15]. In addition, TT were found to have a high test-retest reliability [16, 17] also when compared to TTE efforts [4, 18]. From a practical point, trained athletes are commonly accustomed to TT type efforts as this is the typical exercise modality in competitions. It is therefore recommended, to prefer TT over TTE when constructing the power-duration relationship [6, 15].

Hampson et al. [19] argued that during TT efforts, athletes are able to change the intensity according to perception of fatigue and motivation. Whilst intensity fluctuations add some variability to the measurement [13], Jeukendrup and Currell [20] debated that pacing is an inherent strategic component of real-world performance and therefore, is an integral part of performance tests. The only recent work suggesting an improved performance using TTE was performed just recently by Coakley and Passfield [21]. Comparing time-matched TTE with TT, a higher average power output (PO) for the 80% TTE resulted in significantly higher values for CP and significantly lower values compared to those derived from the TT. Despite this finding, it is currently unclear, if CP derived from TTE represent a sustainable intensity. As a result of the constant power profile during TTE, as opposed to power fluctuations during TT, pain, discomfort and peripheral fatigue might be delayed [22, 23], and therefore could increase mean PO.

When using TT for the determination of CP and , Galbraith et al. [15] and Karsten et al. [7] demonstrated a high reliability for critical speed (the mode equivalent of CP in running) and CP respectively using ecologically valid TT efforts in the field (coefficient of variation [CoV] = 1.3–2.0% [15]; CoV = 2.2–2.5% [7]). However, similar to TTE efforts both studies demonstrated poor reliability for TT determined values of [7, 15] (CoV = 9.8–18.4% [15]; CoV = 46.0–46.7% [7]). Karsten et al. [7] speculated that differences in environmental conditions (e.g. terrain, cadence) or in the seating position might have affected reliability of , whilst Galbraith et al. [15] found an increased reliability after a familiarization session.

In contrast, Triska et al. [12] and Black et al. [24] found non-significant differences and a significant correlation in between TTE and TT running and cycling using time/work-matching TTE and TT efforts. However, a high intra-individual variation did not allow the interchangeable use of [12].

When testing for CP and , even well-trained cyclists appear to require two familiarization sessions when using fixed-duration TT in the laboratory. This was demonstrated by Parker Simpson and Kordi [25] who found significantly lower CP values during testing sessions 1 and 2 compared to subsequent sessions. Interestingly, no differences were found for across all trials. The importance of familiarization trials is further corroborated by other studies, showing a smaller CoV after familiarization [14, 15]. Galbraith et al. [15] argued that altered pacing strategies can result in smaller CoV values post familiarization. The same authors demonstrated a poor reliability of (ICC r = 0.75 and CoV = 32.7%) even though participants were familiarized [14]. However, the duration of the respective predictive runs were not matched in the latter study, what has been shown to affect the parameter estimates [12]. The reason for the high day-to-day variation of is still unclear and questions on whether can be accurately determined using the power-duration relationship, and if the estimated equals ‘physiological’ , remains to be elucidated [12, 26, 27].

To date the reliability of TT determined CP and values has not been demonstrated in the laboratory. Given present findings for [7, 12, 14], familiarization, controlled conditions, and matched durations of respective trials might provide some further insight into this apparent conundrum of a low reproducibility of . Therefore, the aim of this study was to assess the reliability and potential learning effects when using TT efforts to determine CP and under controlled conditions. We hypothesized non-significant differences for CP and , a smaller CoV, and higher ICC after familiarization.

Material and methods

Participants

Ten well-trained male triathletes (age: 28.5 ± 4.7 years; body mass: 73.3 ± 7.9 kg; height: 1.80 ± 0.07 m; maximal aerobic power [MAP]: 329 ± 41 W) volunteered to participate in this study. All participants were involved in regular training and competition for at least three years on a national competition level and were experienced in performing TT. Before entering the study, participants completed a health questionnaire and provided written informed consent after the nature and risks of the study had been explained. The ethics committee of the University of Vienna (#00216) approved all experimental procedures and the study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Study design

The study followed a repeated laboratory test design where participants reported to the laboratory on four occasions separated by at least 72 h. A preliminary graded exercise test (GXT) was followed by three visits consisting of three TT each. These TT were between 3 and 12 min in duration and interspersed by 60 min passive rest to allow blood lactate [La] to return to baseline values in order to minimize any effect of prior exercise on uptake kinetics on the subsequent trial [5, 27]. Tests were performed at the same time of the day (± 2 h) in an air-condition controlled laboratory. Temperature and relative humidity were between 22–23°C and 45–55%, respectively. Participants were instructed to arrive at the laboratory in a fully hydrated state and to avoid strenuous exercise and alcohol intake in the 24 h prior to testing. Participants were also required to refrain from food and caffeine 3 h prior to testing. For all tests, a Cyclus2 ergometer (RBM Elektronics, Leipzig, Germany) was used where participants used their own racing or TT bike, which was mounted to the ergometer. During all tests, participants were strongly verbally encouraged. Testing was completed within 3 weeks to avoid effects of training and detraining. All tests were performed outside of the competitive season (i.e. during the participants’ off-season) during which each participant trained between 3 to 5 h per week. The majority of the participants completed the tests within 12–13 days, with the exception of a single participant who completed the tests within 16 days. However, in this single participant the GXT and the familiarization session were separated by 7 days and the two CP-tests were separated by 72 h.

Graded exercise test

A GXT was performed to determine MAP. After an unloaded cycling phase for 3 min, resistance was set to 100 W and was increased by 20 W every 3 min until volitional exhaustion. If the last work stage could not be fully completed, MAP was calculated using the following equation of Kuipers et al. [28]: (1) where MAP is the maximum aerobic power (W), Plast is the last fully completed work stage (W) and t is the duration of the incomplete work stage (s).

TT to determine the power-duration relationship

Participants performed three identical tests to determine the power-duration relationship. The first test was used as a familiarization session and it was included in the analysis. The first test is consequently termed Familiarization, and the second and third test Test I and Test II, respectively. During the TT participants were advised to produce the highest mean power output for 12, 7 and 3 min in that order [29] and were instructed to complete each trial maximally (‘maximal TT effort’) [5]. Participants were able to manipulate their cadence and gear throughout the trials by using the virtual gear changer mounted to the handlebar thus simulating field-based TT. Moreover, participants used a self-selected pacing strategy. Transitions from rest to work were with an increase of pedal cadence to the participants’ own preferred value after a 3-min unloaded cycling phase. During the TT, PO increased as a function of cadence and pedal force.

Estimation of CP and

Mean PO for each TT was plotted against the inverse-of-time using a linear regression where PO is the mean power output (W), is the total amount of work accomplished above CP until task failure (J) and CP is the critical power (W): (2)

Least square modelling procedures were used to fit the parameter estimates. The y-intercept represents CP and the slope represents . The individual SEE was calculated for each participant and each parameter estimate in absolute and relative values. Nimmerichter et al. [30] demonstrated that the model power vs. the inverse of time provides notably lower SEE compared to other two parameter models [30]. Analysing the parameter estimates of the three most commonly used models to estimate CP and (i.e. hyperbolic model of power vs. time, linear model of work vs. time, and linear model of power vs. inverse of time) revealed non-significant differences between the models, neither for CP (P = 0.353, P = 0.887, and P = 0.909 for Familiarization, Test I and Test II, respectively) nor for (P = 0.180, P = 0.867, and P = 0.812 for Familiarization, Test I and Test II, respectively). Consequently, we decided to use the model that provides the smallest error of the estimates (SEE) and thus results in most accurate estimates of CP and [30].

Statistical analyses

After testing for normality using Shapiro-Wilk procedures, a repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to assess differences between the tests. If the assumption of sphericity had been violated (P < 0.001) the Greenhouse-Geisser correction has been used [31]. Significant main effects were followed-up by Bonferroni post-hoc procedures. Partial eta-squared was used to provide an estimate of effect size of the ANOVA (small ; moderate ; large ). Effect size for the post-hoc tests was calculated using Cohen’s d (small d = 0.2; moderate d = 0.5; large d = 0.8) [32]. The intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC) and the coefficient of variation (CoV) were calculated using a spreadsheet [33]. An ICC >0.9 indicates high reliability, values >0.8 indicate moderate reliability, values >0.6 indicate questionable reliability, and values <0.6 indicate poor reliability of repeated tests. The coefficient of variation (CoV) was used to rate intra-individual variation. An upper limit of 5% [33] or 10% [34] is proposed to provide reliable results when repeating two tests. The Bland-Altman’s method of 95% limits of agreement (LoA) assessed the agreement between repeated tests for CP and [35]. Pearson product moment correlation assessed the strength of an association between repeated tests. Statistical significance was accepted at P < 0.05. Before the beginning of the study an a priori power-analysis was conducted and revealed that 10 participants were required to detect a significant difference of 15 W and 3 kJ for CP and , respectively with a statistical power of >80% [36]. A difference of 15 W in CP and 3 kJ in would result in a calculated TT20min time difference of <5%. That is within the typical day-to-day variation of TT performance [12].

Results

Table 1 represents results of Familiarization, Tests I and II (S1 File), Table 2 illustrates data reporting reliability and agreement between repeated tests (Figs 1 and 2), and Table 3 reports the ICC and CoV of individual TT. Figs 1 and 2 illustrate the correlation of CP and between repeated tests. Between tests non-significant differences were found for CP (F2,18 = 1.949; P = 0.171; ) and (F2,18 = 2.951; P = 0.078; ). Significant differences were found for the absolute SEE for CP (F2,18 = 10.847; P = 0.001; ) and (F2,18 = 10.865; P = 0.001; ) and the relative SEE for CP (F2,18 = 5.935; P = 0.001; ) and (F2,18 = 5.428; P = 0.014; ). Bonferroni post-hoc procedures for the absolute SEE revealed significant differences between Familiarization and Test I for CP and (P = 0.042 and d = 1.20 for both parameters) and between Familiarization and Test II for CP and (P = 0.008 and d = 1.74 for both parameters). No significant differences were found for the absolute SEE for CP (P = 0.989 and d < 0.01) and the absolute SEE for (P = 0.945 and d < 0.01) between Test I and Test II. Bonferroni post-hoc procedures for the relative SEE revealed significant differences between Familiarization and Test I and between Familiarization and Test II for CP only (P = 0.043, d = 1.04 and P = 0.005, d = 1.85, respectively), but not for (P = 0.185, d = 0.75 and P = 0.075, d = 0.96, respectively). No significant differences were found for the relative SEE for CP (P = 0.850 and d = 0.12) and the relative SEE for (P = 0.841 and d = 0.12) between Test I and Test II.

thumbnail
Table 2. ICC (95%CL), CoV (95%CL), mean bias and 95% LoA for and CP.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0189776.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. ICC (95% CL), and CoV (95% CL) for individual TT and test.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0189776.t003

thumbnail
Fig 1. Relationships (panels a and b) and Bland-Altman plots of the differences (panels c and d) between repeated tests of CP.

The black solid line represents the linear regression and the grey-dotted line represents the line of identity (panel a and b). The solid grey line represents the mean bias and the dotted black line represent the 95% limits of agreement (panel c and d).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0189776.g001

thumbnail
Fig 2. Relationships (panels a and b) and Bland-Altman plots of the differences (panels c and d) between repeated tests of .

The black solid line represents the linear regression and the grey-dotted line represents the line of identity (panel a and b). The solid grey line represents the mean bias and the dotted black line represent the 95% limits of agreement (panel c and d).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0189776.g002

Discussion

The main novel findings of the present study were that both, CP and values provide reliable results in a cohort of well-trained athletes after a familiarization trial. Importantly, this is the first study, which demonstrates such a high reliability for the estimates of (ICC r = 0.94). Even though participants were familiar with TT efforts in the field, they produced slightly higher CP estimates (~3.5%) and notably lower estimates (~13%) after the familiarization trial. Although non-significant differences in the parameter estimates were revealed, the effect size is of a moderate order for both parameter estimates, small effects were observed between Familiarization and Test I for CP (d = 0.28) and (d = 0.47). The effect sizes for CP and between Tests I and II were trivial (d = -0.04 and d = -0.06, respectively). Considering effect sizes seems to be more appropriate when assessing smaller sample sizes and small mean differences [37].

Results demonstrate a notable improvement for ICC and CoV values related to both parameter estimates after familiarization using TT of equal duration (i.e. 12, 7, and 3 min). Recently, it was demonstrated that the high intra-individual variation in parameter estimates can be reduced when using iso-duration TT compared with TTE efforts [12]. The predictive error of however, remained too high to be used for detecting small training induced changes (i.e. 18.7% [12]). Previous studies suggested that small changes in TTE durations affect [11, 12] and consequently, using fixed-duration TT can alleviate these negative influences thus increasing reliability of the parameter estimates.

ICC values for CP between Familiarization and Test I and between Tests I and II can be interpreted as moderate and highly reliable, respectively. The CoV for CP notably decreased following the familiarization trial (4.1% vs. 2.6%). But both testing trials were within what is currently acknowledged as an accepted range (i.e. <10% for [34] and <5% for CP [33]). Our CP results are consistent with studies where reliability of CP was evaluated using TT under laboratory conditions [25] and under field conditions [7]. Karsten et al. [7] found similar ICC values and CoV compared to the present results (ICC r = 0.99 and CoV = 2.2%). A recent study by Wright et al. [31] found comparable ICC values (r = 0.97–0.99 [31]) and comparable CoV (1.2–1.9% and 8.4% [31] for CP and , respectively), when using the three minute all-out test (3MT). However, whilst employing TT for the determination of the parameter estimates is a valid method [5], the validity of the 3MT compared to the traditional determination using TTE is poor (i.e. SEE >5% and >26% for CP and , respectively) [31]. This suggests that the determination of the parameter estimates using multiple TT provides more accurate parameter estimates compared to a single effort, i.e. the 3MT.

While the ICC value for is interpreted as poor between Familiarization and Test I, it changes to be highly reliable between Tests I and II. Furthermore, the CoV was >10% for between Familiarization and Test I, whilst it improved to values that according to Atkinson and Nevill [34] can be interpreted as reliable (i.e. <10%) between Tests I and II, confirming to be reliable post familiarization. However, such a high reliability was not present in a field-based study using a similar methodology (ICC r = 0.16 and CoV = 46% [7]). Karsten et al. [7] speculated that differences in environmental conditions (e.g. level vs. uphill) might have influenced the results for . With the exclusion of this factor, our laboratory-based parameter estimates demonstrate a high level of reliability after familiarization (ICC r = 0.95). It is therefore suggested that standardized and controlled laboratory conditions alleviate influencing effects on and consequently result in a higher reliability of the parameter estimate. When using the 3MT, Wright et al. [31] found comparable reliability for (ICC r = 0.94–0.98 and CoV = 5.4–8.4% [31]).

The mean bias of CP and between Tests I and II was close to zero after a familiarization session (Figs 1 and 2). Furthermore, the 95% LoA for both parameters showed notably closer LoA after Familiarization (Figs 1 and 2) which is consistent with findings using TT in well-trained runners [15]. Galbraith et al. [15] found an improvement of 95% LoA for from ±80 m to ±45 m (reduction of ~50%) after familiarization, and in the present study a familiarization session resulted in an even greater improvement of the 95% LoA from ± 10,000 J to ± 2,500 J (reduction of ~75%). These results provide evidence of a learning effect even in well-trained cyclists. Similar to the LoA, the SEE became notably smaller for both parameter estimates after a familiarization session (Figs 1 and 2). Our participants were able to provide a more consistent performance thereby reducing SEE by ~30% (CP) and by ~50% () after familiarization, also showing the presence of a learning effect. After a familiarization trial, a high agreement of the regression line and the line of identity for both parameter estimates was evident (Figs 1b and 2b). The SEE values between Tests I and II (±12 W and ±1.3 kJ for CP and , respectively) are also within day-to-day variations and are lower compared to the recent field-based study by Karsten et al. [7]. The SEE for CP in our study is slightly higher compared to another laboratory-based investigation using TT, however, the SEE for is similar [25]. It is important to note that Parker-Simpson and Kordi [25] used a different testing methodology by performing the third TT on a different day.

Moreover, Black et al. [24] and Karsten et al. [6] speculated that different pacing patterns (i.e. fast start vs. slow start) between efforts could have affected the determination of CP and . Galbraith et al. [14] reported a pacing related learning effect in well-trained runners which might be the cause for the low reliability between Familiarization and Test I in the present study. Contrary to this, Parker-Simpson and Kordi [25] stated the need of two familiarization sessions using TT, but in contrast to the present study, participants were not allowed to change gear ratios during the TT, which lowered ecological validity and likely added to a larger learning effect. Participants in the present study seem to have adapted a reproducible pacing strategy as the mean PO within the first 60 s was not different between respective trials (P = 0.561–0.836).

Coakley and Passfield [21] argued that TTE are superior compared to TT as TTE provide a higher mean PO during the longest trial (i.e. ~12 min) compared to TT. However, during the TT in the present study participants were able to select a self-selected pacing strategy with a known end-point and therefore these TT approximated real-world conditions as close as possible. Moreover, the work-rate during the TTE was not constant and participants were able to change PO in a small range [21]. Depending on their research question investigators can take a more informed decision which mode (i.e. TT vs. TTE) to choose. A fast start, as seen during most real-world TT efforts, will stimulate Type III/IV neurons [23], increase the level of pain [22] and thereby the overall exertion, which might result in a reduced PO. However, fluctuations in PO during TT more closely mimic real-world TT and therefore, TT should be preferred to construct the power-duration relationship.

Even though individual TT were highly reliable throughout repeated tests (ICC r = 0.94–0.97 and CoV = 2.0–3.0%) (Table 3), lower SEE values of the individual power-duration relationships (i.e. elevated quality of the model) were demonstrated post familiarization (Table 1). Thereafter, SEE values remained low in subsequent tests. The present results support the argument by Karsten et al. [5] who stated that assessing the SEE is an important measure for the quality of the model. The differences in absolute and relative SEE of CP and between Familiarization and Test I are of a large effect size, which shows a learning effect and consequently the need for familiarization. Recently, it was suggested that SEE values above recommended limits (i.e. 2% for CP and 10% for [38, 39]) may affect the parameter estimates [5, 12].

Generally, the reasons for the higher reliability in the current study compared to earlier work could have been threefold: (i) controlled laboratory conditions; (ii) same TT durations across visits; (iii) no differences in pacing strategy after a familiarization session.

A potential limitation of the study was the use of fixed-duration TT. These, whilst arguably carrying a higher ecological validity compared to constant-power TTE, are limited by competitive races commonly using fixed-distances rather than fixed-times. Yet, fixed-duration TT should be preferred to reduce the level of random error and construct the power-duration relationship reproducibly [12]. More research can be suggested to investigate the potential supremacy of fixed-distance TT in the laboratory and the field.

Conclusion

To reduce the error inherent in testing, present results demonstrate that trained athletes experienced in TT and competition require to be familiarized when determining CP and using TT in the laboratory. Even though highly reliable results for individual mean TT PO across multiple tests were evident, the quality of the model increased in subsequent testing sessions. Therefore, using TT is valid, reliable, and ecologically valid (i.e. own pacing strategy, change of cadence and gearing). It is consequently suggested that laboratory TT are preferable over TTE efforts and should be considered as a recommended method of best practice when determining CP and .

Supporting information

S1 File. Individual data for each participant.

File contains data for CP, , and relative and absolute SE for CP and , respectively.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0189776.s001

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

Open access funding provided by University of Vienna. The corresponding author wants to express his sincere thanks to the Austrian Institute of Sports Medicine and the University of Applied Science Wr. Neustadt for providing equipment of their laboratories.

References

  1. 1. Jones AM, Wilkerson DP, DiMenna F, Fulford J, Poole DC. Muscle metabolic responses to exercise above and below the "critical power" assessed using 31P-MRS. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2008;294(2):R585–93. pmid:18056980.
  2. 2. Hill DW. The critical power concept. A review. Sports Med. 1993;16(4):237–54. pmid:8248682.
  3. 3. Moritani T, Nagata A, deVries HA, Muro M. Critical power as a measure of physical work capacity and anaerobic threshold. Ergonomics. 1981;24(5):339–50. pmid:7262059.
  4. 4. Triska C, Tschan H, Tazreiter G, Nimmerichter A. Critical Power in Laboratory and Field Conditions Using Single-visit Maximal Effort Trials. Int J Sports Med. 2015;36(13):1063–8. pmid:26258826.
  5. 5. Karsten B, Baker J, Naclerio F, Klose A, Bianco A, Nimmerichter A. Time Trials versus Time to Exhaustion Tests: Effects on Critical Power, W′ and Oxygen Uptake Kinetics. Int J Sports Physiol Perform. 2017:1–22. pmid:28530476.
  6. 6. Karsten B, Jobson SA, Hopker J, Jimenez A, Beedie C. High agreement between laboratory and field estimates of critical power in cycling. Int J Sports Med. 2014;35(4):298–303. pmid:24022574.
  7. 7. Karsten B, Jobson SA, Hopker J, Stevens L, Beedie C. Validity and reliability of critical power field testing. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2015;115(1):197–204. pmid:25260244.
  8. 8. Gaesser GA, Wilson LA. Effects of continuous and interval training on the parameters of the power-endurance time relationship for high-intensity exercise. Int J Sports Med. 1988;9(6):417–21. pmid:3253231.
  9. 9. Nebelsick-Gullett LJ, Housh TJ, Johnson GO, Bauge SM. A comparison between methods of measuring anaerobic work capacity. Ergonomics. 1988;31(10):1413–9. pmid:3208733.
  10. 10. Smith JC, Hill DW. Stability of parameter estimates derived from the power/time relationship. Can J Appl Physiol. 1993;18(1):43–7. pmid:8471993.
  11. 11. Vandewalle H, Vautier JF, Kachouri M, Lechevalier JM, Monod H. Work-exhaustion time relationships and the critical power concept. A critical review. J Sports Med Phys Fitness. 1997;37(2):89–102. pmid:9239986.
  12. 12. Triska C, Karsten B, Nimmerichter A, Tschan H. Iso-duration Determination of D′ and CS under Laboratory and Field Conditions. Int J Sports Med. 2017;38(7):527–33. pmid:28514809.
  13. 13. Hinckson EA, Hopkins WG. Reliability of time to exhaustion analyzed with critical-power and log-log modeling. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2005;37(4):696–701. pmid:15809572.
  14. 14. Galbraith A, Hopker J, Lelliott S, Diddams L, Passfield L. A single-visit field test of critical speed. Int J Sports Physiol Perform. 2014;9(6):931–5. pmid:24622815.
  15. 15. Galbraith A, Hopker JG, Jobson SA, Passfield L. A novel field test to determine critical speed. J Sport Medic Doping Studie. 2011;01(01):1–4.
  16. 16. Jeukendrup AE, Saris WH, Brouns F, Kester AD. A new validated endurance performance test. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1996;28(2):266–70. pmid:8775164.
  17. 17. Hopkins WG, Schabort EJ, Hawley JA. Reliability of power in physical performance tests. Sports Med. 2001;31(3):211–34. pmid:11286357.
  18. 18. Laursen PB, Francis GT, Abbiss CR, Newton MJ, Nosaka K. Reliability of time-to-exhaustion versus time-trial running tests in runners. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2007;39(8):1374–9. pmid:17762371.
  19. 19. Hampson DB, St Clair Gibson A, Lambert MI, Noakes TD. The influence of sensory cues on the perception of exertion during exercise and central regulation of exercise performance. Sports Med. 2001;31(13):935–52. pmid:11708402.
  20. 20. Jeukendrup AE, Currell K. Should time trial performance be predicted from three serial time-to-exhaustion tests? Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2005;37(10):1820; author reply 1. pmid:16260987.
  21. 21. Coakley SL, Passfield L. Cycling performance is superior for time-to-exhaustion versus time-trial in endurance laboratory tests. J Sports Sci. 2017:1–7. pmid:28892462.
  22. 22. Marcora SM. Role of feedback from Group III and IV muscle afferents in perception of effort, muscle pain, and discomfort. J Appl Physiol. 2011;110(5):1499; author reply 500. pmid:21562154.
  23. 23. Amann M, Blain GM, Proctor LT, Sebranek JJ, Pegelow DF, Dempsey JA. Group III and IV muscle afferents contribute to ventilatory and cardiovascular response to rhythmic exercise in humans. J Appl Physiol. 2010;109(4):966–76. pmid:20634355
  24. 24. Black MI, Jones AM, Bailey SJ, Vanhatalo A. Self-pacing increases critical power and improves performance during severe-intensity exercise. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. 2015;40(7):662–70. pmid:26088158.
  25. 25. Parker Simpson L, Kordi M. Comparison of Critical Power and W′ Derived from Two or Three Maximal Tests. Int J Sports Physiol Perform. 2016:1–24.
  26. 26. Galbraith A, Hopker J, Passfield L. Modeling Intermittent Running from a Single-visit Field Test. Int J Sports Med. 2015;36(5):365–70. pmid:25665002.
  27. 27. Karsten B, Hopker J, Jobson SA, Baker J, Petrigna L, Klose A, et al. Comparison of inter-trial recovery times for the determination of critical power and W′ in cycling. J Sports Sci. 2017;35(14):1420–5. pmid:27531664.
  28. 28. Kuipers H, Verstappen FT, Keizer HA, Geurten P, van Kranenburg G. Variability of aerobic performance in the laboratory and its physiologic correlates. Int J Sports Med. 1985;6(4):197–201. pmid:4044103.
  29. 29. Jenkins DG, Quigley BM. Endurance training enhances critical power. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1992;24(11):1283–9. pmid:1435180.
  30. 30. Nimmerichter A, Steindl M, Williams CA. Reliability of the Single-Visit Field Test of Critical Speed in Trained and Untrained Adolescents. Sports. 2015;3(4):358–68.
  31. 31. Wright J, Bruce-Low S, Jobson SA. The Reliability and Validity of the 3-min All-out Cycling Critical Power Test. Int J Sports Med. 2017;38(6):462–7. pmid:28388783.
  32. 32. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, N.J.: L. Erlbaum Associates; 1988. xxi, 567 p. p.
  33. 33. Hopkins WG. A new view on statistics: Internet Society for Sport Science; 2000 [updated 17 August 2011]. Available from: http://www.sportsci.org/resource/stats/.
  34. 34. Atkinson G, Nevill AM. Statistical methods for assessing measurement error (reliability) in variables relevant to sports medicine. Sports Med. 1998;26(4):217–38. pmid:9820922.
  35. 35. Bland JM, Altman DG. Statistical methods for assessing agreement between two methods of clinical measurement. Lancet. 1986;1(8476):307–10. pmid:2868172.
  36. 36. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. 2009;41(4):1149–60. pmid:19897823.
  37. 37. Buchheit M. The Numbers Will Love You Back in Return-I Promise. Int J Sports Physiol Perform. 2016;11(4):551–4. pmid:27164726.
  38. 38. Ferguson C, Wilson J, Birch KM, Kemi OJ. Application of the speed-duration relationship to normalize the intensity of high-intensity interval training. PLoS One. 2013;8(11):e76420. pmid:24244266
  39. 39. Dekerle J, de Souza KM, de Lucas RD, Guglielmo LG, Greco CC, Denadai BS. Exercise Tolerance Can Be Enhanced through a Change in Work Rate within the Severe Intensity Domain: Work above Critical Power Is Not Constant. PLoS One. 2015;10(9):e0138428. pmid:26407169