Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Steroid hormones and psychological responses to soccer matches: Insights from a systematic review and meta-analysis

  • Maamer Slimani ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    maamer2011@hotmail.fr

    Affiliation Research Laboratory ‘‘Sport Performance Optimization”, National Centre of Medicine and Science in Sport (CNMSS), El Menzah, Tunisia

  • Julien S. Baker,

    Roles Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation University of the West of Scotland, Institute of Clinical Exercise and Health Science, Hamilton Campus, Hamilton, United Kingdom

  • Foued Cheour ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Foued Cheour, Lee Taylor, Nicola Luigi Bragazzi

    Roles Methodology, Supervision

    Affiliation High Institute of Applied Biology of Médenine, Medenine, Tunisia

  • Lee Taylor ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Foued Cheour, Lee Taylor, Nicola Luigi Bragazzi

    Roles Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Athlete Health and Performance Research Centre, ASPETAR, Qatar Orthopaedic and Sports Medicine Hospital, Doha, Qatar, Loughborough University, School of Sport, Exercise and Health Sciences, Loughborough, United Kingdom

  • Nicola Luigi Bragazzi

    Contributed equally to this work with: Foued Cheour, Lee Taylor, Nicola Luigi Bragazzi

    Roles Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Software, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations School of Public Health, Department of Health Sciences (DISSAL), Genoa University, Genoa, Italy, Department of Neuroscience, Rehabilitation, Ophthalmology, Genetics, Maternal and Child Health (DINOGMI), Section of Psychiatry, Genoa University, Genoa, Italy

Abstract

The present systematic review and meta-analysis aimed to assess the perturbations in hormonal and psychological homeostasis in response to soccer match-play. These perturbations were explored according to match outcome (i.e., win versus loss), gender, type of contest (i.e., competitive versus non-competitive fixtures) and competitive level (i.e., novice versus high-level). The review was conducted according to the Population/Intervention or Exposure/Comparison/Outcome(s) (PICO) criteria and the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) guidelines. Match outcome, type of contest and competitive levels were moderator variables in the examined steroid hormones responses to a soccer match-play. Different testosterone responses were seen between match winners (increase) and losers (decrease) when compared to pre-game or baseline values (p <0.05), whilst no changes could be detected for cortisol relative to match outcome in female soccer players. Males (Δ% = 6.26; ES = 0.28) demonstrated a marginally lower increase in testosterone levels when compared to females (Δ% = 49.16; ES = 1.00), though not statistically significant. Females (Δ% = 162.7; ES = 0.98) did not demonstrate elevated cortisol match response compared to males (Δ% = 34.60; ES = 1.20). Male novice soccer match-play increased cortisol levels compared to high-level soccer match-play (Q = 18.08, p<0.001). Competitive soccer matches increased cortisol levels compared to non-competitive fixtures (i.e., collegiate tournament). Additionally, competitive levels moderate the relationship between a soccer match and testosterone levels (p <0.001), regardless of gender differences. From the presented systematic review and meta-analysis it appears (1) cortisol changes are associated with cognitive anxiety in starter female soccer players, while (2) testosterone changes are associated with changes in mood state in females and social connectedness in male soccer players. This apparent psycho-physiological relationship may proffer the opportunity for targeted intervention(s) by practitioners to favorably influence performance and/or recovery agendas. Further mechanistic and/or applied evidence is required in this regard in addition to further data sets from females.

Introduction

Soccer is a high-intensity intermittent sport characterized by high variability within and between games [1,2]. Various contextual factors such as match score, tactics, fixture time and location, environmental conditions, League ranking, championship or cup competition, travel demands proximal to competition, amongst others, can influence match-play characteristics and post-game psycho-physiological responses [3,4]. These factors conspire to elicit psychological and physiological perturbations to homeostasis relative but not limited to, muscular, endocrine, and immune systems [5].

The adaptations of autonomic nervous system (sympathetic and parasympathetic systems) and other body systems during and response to various stressors have been extensively explored [68]. Adaptive changes to stressors can be classed as behavioural or physical, which can interact along with the brain and its peripheral components to simulate the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (HPA) and the autonomic sympathetic system [5]. These systems therefore have interrelated psychological (e.g., anxiety and mood state) and physiological (e.g., cortisol and testosterone responses) components (e.g., responses are psycho-physiological in nature). Consequently, monitoring the psycho-physiological response to training and competition, including the HPA axis, hypothalamic—pituitary—gonadal (HPG) axis and hypothalamic—pituitary—thyroid (HPT) axis has been seen within soccer [68]. Such HPT/HPA data has been used to determine player preparedness for subsequent training and competition, and inform individualized recovery strategies [911].

Circulating testosterone and cortisol levels fluctuate during stressors or exercises [9]. As such, they have been used in an attempt to quantitatively determine the psycho-physiological stress/effort imposed by a soccer competition [1214] often alongside appropriate psychometric tools and/or inventories that enable researchers/practitioners to subjectively quantify the psychological status, such as the mood states [the profile of mood states (POMS)], the perception of exertion [rating of perceived exertion (RPE: [15])] and the anxiety states (anxiety rating [16]). However, large intra- and inter-individual variability in testosterone and cortisol responses to soccer competition are seen [17], hence conflicting results within the literature [18]. This variability has been attributed to a host of physical/physiological and/or cognitive factors, including but not limited to, match-play activity profile [15,19], match-outcome [win or loss [20], type of contest (i.e., competitive versus non-competitive fixtures), competitive level, player coping style (psychological apparatus to deal with personal and public pressure), training status, player support network, and gender [21]. Given the variability present, it appears logical to compile and subsequently systematically review the available evidence, to determine which factors are indeed moderator/mediator variables relative to the psycho-physiological responses to a soccer match-play. If psycho-physiological relationships are apparent (e.g., increased anxiety is related to match loss), it may proffer the opportunity for targeted intervention(s) by practitioners to favorably influence performance and/or recovery agendas.

Therefore, the aim of the present systematic review and meta-analysis was to determine the effects of soccer match-play on steroid hormones (i.e., testosterone and cortisol) and psychosocial responses (mood state, competitive anxiety, psychological stress, social connectedness), identifying key moderator/mediator variables like match-outcome, gender, type of contest and competitive level. It is hypothesized that (1) match outcome, type of contest and competitive level may moderate the soccer match-hormonal responses relationship, and (2) cortisol and testosterone changes would be associated with changes in anxiety and mood state respectively.

Methods

Search strategy

The present systematic review and meta-analysis was conducted according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analysis (PRISMA) guidelines [22] (Fig 1, S1 PRISMA Checklist). The systematic search was conducted using different databases, as recommended by the Cochrane Association, namely PubMed/MEDLINE, Scopus (Elsevier), SciVerse ScienceDirect (Elsevier), Institute for Scientific Information (ISI)/Web of Science (WoS), SPORTDiscus, ProQuest, Chemical Abstracts Service (CAS), the Directory of Open Access Journals (DOAJ), the Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (CDSR) of the Cochrane Library, the Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature (CINAHL), the Scientific Electronic Library Online (SciELO), and Google Scholar with dates ranging from the earliest record to April 2016. All study designs were included. The search terms included the following keywords: “soccer match”, “hormonal response”, “testosterone”, “cortisol”, and “stress”, connected using proper Boolean connectors and using Medical Subject Headings (MeSH) and wild-card options, when appropriate. Target journals have been hand-searched for capturing all potentially relevant studies.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analysis (PRISMA) flow-chart.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186100.g001

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Studies were included in the review if they met all the following Population/Intervention /Comparison/Outcome(s) (PICO) criteria:

  1. Population: studies recruiting male and female novice and/or high-level soccer players as participants.
  2. Intervention or Exposure: investigations studying the psychological changes over the duration of a soccer match and focusing on the hormonal responses using any hormonal measurements methods and collecting blood, urine or saliva samples. Salivary and serum derived hormone values were deemed equally valid, exhibiting strong positive correlations with one another [2326].
  3. Comparison: steroid hormones and psychological responses to a soccer match relative to match-outcome, gender, type of contest, and competitive level.
  4. Outcome(s): pre- to post-steroid hormones (i.e., testosterone and cortisol) changes to a soccer match-play and the correlation between the steroid hormones response and the psychosocial status.
  5. Design: original investigations published in scholarly, peer-reviewed journals.
  6. Time filter: from inception to April 2016.
  7. Language filter: English.

Studies were excluded according to the following criteria:

  1. Reviews, comments, opinions and commentaries, interviews, letter to editor, editorial, posters, conference abstracts, book chapters, and books were excluded; available reviews have been anyways scanned for increasing the chance of including potentially relevant articles.
  2. Focusing on methodological issues (for example, comparing endocrinological assays).
  3. Comparing hormonal responses between footballers and lay people and not over soccer matches.
  4. Assessing hormonal change after nutritional supplements or other kinds of intervention (interventional studies).
  5. Lacking quantitative information and details.

Screening strategy

The studies have been independently screened by two authors (MS, NLB) looking at study titles and abstracts for potential eligibility. Screening questions have been ad hoc developed and pilot-tested with a subset of records before implementation. Disagreement has been assessed using κ statistics and has been resolved through discussion until consensus was reached; a third reviewer (JSB) and a forth reviewer (LT) have been involved when necessary.

Review questions

The review questions of the present review are the following:

  1. What hormonal changes (i.e., testosterone and cortisol) can be induced by a soccer match-play?
  2. Is there an association between steroid hormones responses and psychosocial status (e.g., mood state, anxiety, psychological stress and social connectedness) during a soccer match?
  3. Which are the possible moderator and mediator variables? The research literature suggests many potential moderators/mediators of the hormonal changes-soccer match relationship such as: gender, match outcome, competitive levels and the nature of the competition itself, among others.
    1. Are there differences in steroid hormones responses between genders?
    2. Is there a difference between winners and losers in testosterone or cortisol responses to a soccer match?
    3. Is there a difference between novice and high-level players in testosterone or cortisol response to a soccer match?
    4. Is there a difference between competitiveand non-competitive fixtures matches in testosterone or cortisol response to a soccer match?
    5. Has the change of cortisol or testosterone levels to a soccer match been driven by the changes in psychosocial status?

Statistical analysis

Effect sizes.

Effect sizes (ES) were calculated with its 95% confidence interval (CI) according to Cohen [27] and represent the difference between pre- to post- match only, means hormonal values divided by the baseline standard deviation. This method permits the determination of the magnitude of the differences or the changes between pre- to post- match for each study that provided absolute mean data and standard deviations. ES was interpreted with the following rule of thumb: ES <0.2 was defined as trivial; 0.2–0.6 was defined as small; 0.6–1.2 was defined as moderate; 1.2–2.0 was defined as large; >2.0 was defined as very large; and >4.0 was defined as extremely large. A significance level of p <0.05 was considered for all the analysis.

Correlation.

Correlated t-tests (two-tailed), Pearson correlation coefficient, Spearman rank correlation coefficients and Pearson Product-Moment Correlation were used to determine the association (r and/or p values) between testosterone or cortisol levels and psychosocial variables in the included studies. Correlations were evaluated as follows: small (0.1–0.29), moderate (0.30–0.49), large (0.50–0.69), very large (0.70–0.89), nearly perfect (0.90–0.99), and perfect (1.0) [28].

Heterogeneity.

Statistical heterogeneity in our systematic review and meta-analysis was assessed using the Q and I2 statistics. If the I2 was >50%, this was regarded as substantial heterogeneity. To identify sources of variation, further stratification was performed relative to the main characteristics of included studies, carrying out meta-regression analyses in order to quantitatively investigate the relationship between one or more covariates (moderators) at the study level and a dependent variable (that is to say, the effect size). In addition, for the sensitivity analyses, the stability of the pooled estimate with respect to each study was investigated by excluding individual studies from the analysis.

Results

Study selection

The search strategies yielded a preliminary pool of 921 possible papers. The full text of 39 articles were retrieved and assessed for eligibility against the inclusion criteria. After a careful review of their full texts, 22 articles were excluded with reason and the remaining 17 articles were eligible for inclusion in the current review (Fig 1). More specifically, 8 qualitative studies and 9 quantitative studies were noted. More specifically, qualitative study generates rich, detailed and valid process data that contribute to the in-depth understanding of a context. Quantitative study, on the other hand, generates reliable population-based and generalizable data that is suited to establishing cause-and-effect relationships. The main differences between quantitative and qualitative researches concern data sample, data collection, data analysis, and outcomes. From the quantitative studies, three interventions studied the testosterone and cortisol responses to a soccer match[4,28,29]; two trials investigated the cortisol response [16,17]; and one study assessed the testosterone response [19].

Characteristics of included studies

In total, 17 articles were identified and retained in the present research. The characteristics of the study population included novice (i.e., healthy participants) and high-level (i.e., elite, sub-elite, semiprofessional, professional, national) participants (Tables 1 and 2). In addition, 16 studies included high-level players as sample participants and one studies used novice players as sample subjects. The total number of participants included in this review was 333 (177 males, 130 females and 26 participants’ gender was not specified). Sample size ranged between 7 and 42, with age ranging from 8 to 31 years. All studies were characterized by a cross-sectional design and performed a pre- and post-match hormonal analysis (Tables 1 and 2). Participant’s characteristics, such as gender, competitive level and age were extracted and tabulated for each selected study (Tables 1 and 2). Testosterone and cortisol percentage change values (Δ%) were derived from the nmol/L post-match value relative to the pre-match value.

thumbnail
Table 1. Qualitative studies assessed hormonal responses between pre- to post- soccer match (mean ± SD and/or relative effect %).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186100.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Quantitative studies assessed hormonal responses between pre- to post- soccer match (mean ± SD and/or relative effect %).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186100.t002

Pooled effect-sizes

The forest plot of cortisol [nmol/L] changes induced by soccer match-play is shown in Fig 2. Fixed-Effects Model Pooled ES for cortisol [nmol/L] was 1.01 ([95% CI -1.66/-0.36], p = 0.002) when including Coelho et al. [29] study’ (Fig 2a) and 0.67 ([95% CI -1.01/-0.33], p = 0.001) when removing Coelho et al. [29] study’ (Fig 2b). There was heterogeneity (Q = 22.82, I2 = 73.70, p = 0.001) when including Coelho et al. [29] study’ (Fig 3a), however, there was no heterogeneity (Q = 4.74, I2 = 0.00, p = 0.448) when removing Coelho et al. [29] study’ (Fig 3b).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Forest plot of cortisol [nmol/L] changes induced by soccer match, including (2a) and removing (2b) Coelho et al. [29].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186100.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Funnel plot showing evidence of publication bias for the meta-analysis concerning cortisol changes induced by soccer match when pooling all studies, including (3a) and removing (3b) Coelho et al. [29].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186100.g003

The forest plot of testosterone [nmol/L] changes induced by soccer match-play is shown in Fig 4. Fixed-effects model pooled ES for testosterone [nmol/L] was 0.46 ([95% CI -1.43/0.51], p = 0.35) when including Coelho et al. [29] study’ (Fig 4a) and 0.36 ([95% CI -1.73/1.02], p = 0.60) when removing Coelho et al. [29] study’ (Fig 4b). There was heterogeneity when including Coelho et al. [29] study’ (Q = 18.42, I2 = 83.72, p<0.001; Fig 5a) and when removing Coelho et al. [29] study’ (Q = 18.28, I2 = 89.06, p<0.001; Fig 5b).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Forest plot of testosterone [nmol/L] changes induced by soccer match, including (4a) and removing (4b) Coelho et al. [29].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186100.g004

thumbnail
Fig 5. Funnel plot showing evidence of publication bias for the meta-analysis concerning testosterone changes induced by soccer match when pooling all studies, including (5a) and removing (5b) Coelho et al. [29].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186100.g005

Potential moderator and mediator variables

Male novice soccer match contest caused a large increase in cortisol levels compared to pre-match levels (Δ% = 44.36; ES = 3.73; p<0.001), the magnitude of response was significantly higher (Q = 18.08, p<0.001) than that seen in response to a high-level soccer match (Δ% = 32.16; ES = 0.57) (Table 3). Male players reported a lower percentage increase in cortisol levels (Δ% = 34.60; ES = 1.20) compared to females (Δ% = 162.7; ES = 0.98) (Table 3), even though not statistically significant. Additionally, the meta-regression analyses showed that the type of contest moderate a soccer match-cortisol response relationship (p <0.001) (Tables 4 and 5).

thumbnail
Table 3. Pre- and post- soccer match value or percentage of change (Δ%) of cortisol (nmol/L).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186100.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. Meta-regression analyses of studies concerning cortisol changes induced by soccer match-play, when including Coelho et al. [29].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186100.t004

thumbnail
Table 5. Meta-regression analyses of studies concerning cortisol changes induced by soccer match-play, when removing Coelho et al. [29].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186100.t005

Only within competitive matches testosterone, regardless of gender and type of contest, demonstrated a small increase pre-to-post match (Δ% = 20.38; ES = 0.45), without statistical significance (Table 6). Regardless of match contest, male players reported a lower pre- to post-match percentage increase in testosterone levels (Δ% = 6.26; ES = 0.28) than females (Δ% = 49.16; ES = 1.00), without statistical significance (Table 6). A statistically significant moderator variable relationship was seen with respect to experience level of the players (p <0.001, Tables 7 and 8).

thumbnail
Table 6. Pre- and post- soccer match value or percentage of change (Δ%) of testosterone (nmol/L).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186100.t006

thumbnail
Table 7. Meta-regression analyses of studies concerning testosterone changes induced by soccer match-play, when including Coelho et al. [29].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186100.t007

thumbnail
Table 8. Meta-regression analyses of studies concerning testosterone changes induced by soccer match-play, when removing Coelho et al. [29].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186100.t008

Finally, mood state and competitive anxiety resulted to be mediator variables of hormonal change in response to competition in soccer players. Cortisol changes were driven by changes in cognitive anxiety (very large correlation) for starter female soccer players, while testosterone changes were driven by changes in mood state (moderate correlation) for females and social connectedness (large correlation) for male soccer players (Table 9).

thumbnail
Table 9. Correlations between psychological state and cortisol and testosterone concentrations.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186100.t009

Discussion

The aim of the current systematic review and meta-analysis was to determine the hormonal (i.e., testosterone and cortisol) responses to a soccer match according to match outcome (i.e., win, loss), gender, type of contest (i.e., competitive vs. non-competitive fixtures) and competitive level. Testosterone response was found to vary according to the game outcome, with a larger response in winners compared to losers whereas cortisol concentrations did not vary with regard to match outcome. Competitive level may have moderated the cortisol response-soccer match relationship, with greater levels of cortisol reactivity in male novice compared to high-level soccer players. Thus, competitive soccer matches increased cortisol levels to a greater magnitude compared to non-competitive fixtures (i.e., collegiate tournament). Additionally, regardless of gender differences, higher testosterone reactivity in high-level compared to novice players was shown. When psycho-physiological stress was evaluated post-match, cortisol changes appeared to be driven by changes in cognitive anxiety, while testosterone changes were driven by changes in mood state and social connectedness.

Moderator variables

Match outcome and type of contest.

Match outcome moderated the testosterone level in response to a soccer match (higher in winners) within the present data, in agreement with earlier meta-analyses [30]. This differential testosterone response between winners and losers proffers the opportunity for practitioners to employ a precompetitive or half-time cognitive intervention to enhance positive psychological states, such as the mood state and motivation to win, given they are related to game outcome. In contrast, cortisol increase appeared to follow general physical activity [31] with no difference between winners and losers, yet competitive compared to non-competitive matches demonstrated greater cortisol reactivity. Competitive level differences on cortisol are likely due to the higher psycho-physiological effort during competitive matches than non-competitive fixtures, specifically mental toughness, self-confidence, aerobic and anaerobic capacities [32,33]. Practitioners should therefore seek to make training sessions/matches as externally valid to competition as possible, to encourage players to develop robust coping apparatus with and familiarity to highly-competitive externally valid competition scenarios.

Competitive levels.

It has been previously demonstrated that the competitive level is a moderator variable for the testosterone response to a soccer match, likely due to differences in players psycho-physiological competences and traits [32,33]. Increasing expertise is associated with greater physical capacities [33] and more robust psychological apparatus [3235] in soccer players. Indeed, high-level soccer players (high-level vs. ‘other’ standards of athlete) are well acknowledged as psycho-physiologically unique [32,33]. Thus, it is worth noting that high-level players possess a high capability to cope with the game demands and stress as shown by the fast recovery pattern and lower performance impairments [36]. On the basis of the available scientific investigations, it appears that distinguishing competitive levels in regards to the hormonal responses to a soccer match-play may be useful for trainers and coaches in the development processes. More specifically, practitioners should plan specific match/training session soliciting the endocrine system in novice soccer players, in order to simultaneously decrease the psycho-physiological stress and improve the recovery pattern and performances as high-level players, and adopt the best strategy to control the stress-recovery balance.

Gender.

It seems that gender did not moderate the hormonal response to a soccer match-play. However, this might be attributable to the small sample size and number of included studies (two studies recruiting female players; [16,19]) and so further research is warranted to determine effect of gender on hormonal responses during a soccer match. In contrast, a previous review showed that the stress response was different between males and females [37]. The differences were attributed to (a) the higher hormone concentrations for men at rest (particularly testosterone levels) (b) higher psychological stress in men immediately before the test which represents a challenge and (c) the differences in body composition (e.g., higher fat mass in women) and/or in sexual hormonal status [37].

Hormonal changes during the recovery period after a soccer match-play

Regarding the hormonal responses during the recovery period, the competitive soccer match did not altered the testosterone level during the recovery period; however, plasma cortisol concentration significantly increased at 24 h and 48 h as compared to baseline, which returned to baseline after 72 h of recovery [36]. Furthermore, cortisol and testosterone concentrations were found to be diminished after 24 (cortisol ~ -36%, testosterone ~ -25%) and 48 h post-simulated match (cortisol ~ -32%, testosterone ~ -30%) [38]. However, in line with the findings of Silva et al. [36], the authors did not observe alterations in plasma testosterone (free testosterone) concentration throughout the 144 h of the recovery period [18]. In summary, high-level non-competitive and competitive soccer matches led to an increased catabolic hormonal environment until 48 h into the recovery period. This finding suggests that an interval of at least 72–144 h between competitions should be considered by coaches working with high-level soccer players.

Psychological mediators

Alterations in the hormonal [3943] and psychological variables over a soccer game are often observed, suggesting that combined psychological and hormonal changes during competition could be useful to monitor the stress in relation to soccer match performance. The assessment of psycho-physiological stress after a soccer match showed contradictory results throughout different studies. The correlation between cortisol changes and psychological states over a soccer match were investigated in five studies. Some studies showed no significant correlations between changes in cortisol over the game and changes in mood state, anxiety state in female soccer players [15], whereas only one study demonstrated a significant correlation between cortisol changes and cognitive anxiety in starter female soccer players [16].

Concerning testosterone changes after a soccer match, some studies showed significant correlations between testosterone changes and social connectedness in male soccer players [44] and mood state changes in female players [15]. In contrast, there was no significant correlation between testosterone changes, anxiety state [15] and social connectedness changes in women players [44]. For these reasons, coaches and scientists should be attentive to the role of social and environmental stress, personality, and other psychological metrics as measurable experimental variables that can influence data outcomes during a soccer match. These data also proffers the opportunity for practitioners to employ a precompetitive, or half-time, cognitive intervention that improves the participants’ mood state and consequently the participant’s chances of winning, as well as, manage the cognitive anxiety and psychosocial stress and consequently help cope both the physical and psychological demands as elite players.

Limitations

A number of limitations affecting both the primary data and the current systematic review and meta-analysis should be properly acknowledged. First, there was a considerable amount of small numbers of included studies, particularly in female and novice players. While this review could identify important moderators of soccer match-hormonal changes, it is possible that other factors that were not assessed could also explain the observed heterogeneity. For example, training programs and time of the day when competition was played can affect the hormonal stress affecting the chronobiological system. Casanova et al. [45] observed a decrease in testosterone and cortisol levels (pre-to-post-matches), which might be explained by the circadian effect, the time of collecting samples, rather than the effect of the match per se. Furthermore, because of the cross-sectional design of included studies, the observed correlation between hormonal and psychological changes should not implicit as a causal relationship.

Conclusions

This systematic review and meta-analysis provides readers with the first rigorous analytical synthesis of data concerning psychological and hormonal changes induced by soccer matches-play. In fact, the present review showed significant difference in the testosterone response to soccer games between winners and losers, with positive and negative changes in winners and losers, respectively. Furthermore, cortisol concentrations did not vary with regard to the contest outcome. Thus, it has been shown that testosterone reactivity was higher in high-level compared to novice players. Male novice soccer match contests increased cortisol levels to a greater magnitude compared to high-level soccer match. When psycho-physiological stress was evaluated after soccer matches, cortisol changes were found to be driven by changes in cognitive anxiety, while testosterone changes were driven by changes in mood state and social connectedness.

The current review highlights that match outcome and competitive levels should be considered as the key moderator variables of the soccer match-hormonal changes relationship. A psycho-physiological assessment of soccer players could give sports coaches and managers the opportunity to (1) understand the processes involved in the stress response, (2) identify how an athlete copes with stress induced by a competition, (3) reduce and mitigate ‘stress’ response of players pre, post or between games, (4) modulating the training/play load according to the specific hormonal response and (5) to design and implement various ad hoc mental/recovery/coping strategies for performance enhancement and optimization. Particularly, positive reevaluation and active recovery should be recommended.

Supporting information

S1 PRISMA Checklist. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analysis (PRISMA) checklist [22].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186100.s001

(DOC)

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to declare that no sources of funding were used in the preparation of this review. They would also like to affirm that they have no conflict of interest that is directly or indirectly relevant to the content of the present review.

References

  1. 1. Aldous JW, Chrismas BC, Akubat I, Dascombe B, Abt G, Taylor L (2016) Hot and hypoxic environments inhibit simulated soccer performance and exacerbate performance decrements when combined. Front Physiol:6: 421. pmid:26793122
  2. 2. Aldous JWF, Akubat I, Chrismas BCR, Watkins SL, Mauger AR, Midgley AW, Abt G, Taylor L (2014) The reliability and validity of a soccer-specific nonmotorised treadmill simulation (intermittent soccer performance test). J Strength. Cond Res:28: 1971–1980. pmid:24169475
  3. 3. Gregson W, Drust B, Atkinson G, Salvo VD (2010) Match-to-match variability of high-speed activities in premier league soccer. Int J Sports Med:31: 237–42. pmid:20157871
  4. 4. Thorpe R, Sunderland C (2012) Muscle damage, endocrine, and immune marker response to a soccer match. J Strength Cond Res:26: 2783–2790. pmid:22124357
  5. 5. Salvador A, Costa R (2009) Coping with competition: Neuroendocrine responses and cognitive variables. Neurosci Biobehav Rev:3: 160–170.
  6. 6. Arruda AF, Aoki MS, Freitas CG, Spigolon LM, Franciscon C, Moreira A (2015) Testosterone concentration and lower limb power over an entire competitive season in elite young soccer players. J Strength Cond Res:29: 3380–5. pmid:26595131
  7. 7. Morgans R, Owen A, Doran D, Drust B, Morton JP (2015) Prematch salivary secretory immunoglobulin a in soccer players from the 2014 world cup qualifying campaign. Int J Sports Physiol Perform:10: 401–3. pmid:25117424
  8. 8. Moreira A, Bradley P, Carling C, Arruda AF, Spigolon LM, Franciscon C, Aoki MS (2016) Effect of a congested match schedule on immune-endocrine responses, technical performance and session-rpe in elite youth soccer players. J Sports Sci 1–7.
  9. 9. Hayes LD, Grace FM, Baker JS, Sculthorpe N (2015) Exercise-induced responses in salivary testosterone, cortisol, and their ratios in men: A meta-analysis. Sports Med:45: 713–726. pmid:25655373
  10. 10. Nédélec M, Halson S, Abaidia A, Ahmaidi S, Dupont G (2015) Stress, sleep and recovery in elite soccer: A critical review of the literature. Sports Med:45: 1387–1400. pmid:26206724
  11. 11. Nédélec M, Mccall A, Carling C, Legall F, Berthoin S, Dupont G (2012) Recovery in soccer. Sports Med:42: 997–1015. pmid:23046224
  12. 12. Brondino N, Lanati N, Giudici S, Arpesella M, Roncarolo F, Vandoni M (2013) Testosterone level and its relationship with outcome of sporting activity. J Mens Health:10: 40–47.
  13. 13. Casto KV, Edwards DA (2016) Testosterone, cortisol, and human competition. Horm Behav:82: 21–37. pmid:27103058
  14. 14. Gatti R, De Palo EF (2011) An update: Salivary hormones and physical exercise. Scand J Med Sci Sports:21: 157–69. pmid:21129038
  15. 15. Oliveira T, Gouveia M, Oliveira RF (2009) Testosterone responsiveness to winning and losing experiences in female soccer players. Psychoneuroendocrinology:34: 1056–1064. pmid:19278791
  16. 16. Haneishi K, Fry AC, Moore CA, Schilling BK, Li Y, Fry MD (2007) Cortisol and stress responses during a game and practice in female collegiate soccer players. J Strength Cond Res:21: 583–588. pmid:17530979
  17. 17. Moreira A, Arsati F, Arsati YBdOL, Da Silva DA, De Araújo VC (2009) Salivary cortisol in top-level professional soccer players. Eur J Appl Physiol:106: 25–30. pmid:19159948
  18. 18. Ispirlidis I, Fatouros IG, Jamurtas AZ, Nikolaidis MG, Michailidis I, Douroudos I, Margonis K, Chatzinikolaou A, Kalistratos E, Katrabasas I (2008) Time-course of changes in inflammatory and performance responses following a soccer game. Clin J Sport Med:18: 423–431. pmid:18806550
  19. 19. Gravina L, Ruiz F, Lekue JA, Irazusta J, Gil SM (2011) Metabolic impact of a soccer match on female players. J Sports Sci:29: 1345–1352. pmid:21777165
  20. 20. Filaire E, Alix D, Ferrand C, Verger M (2009) Psychophysiological stress in tennis players during the first single match of a tournament. Psychoneuroendocrinology:34: 150–157. pmid:18845399
  21. 21. Kivlighan KT, Granger DA, Booth A (2005) Gender differences in testosterone and cortisol response to competition. Psychoneuroendocrinology:30: 58–71. pmid:15358443
  22. 22. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman D. G. (2009) Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The prisma statement. Ann Intern Med:151: 264–269. pmid:19622511
  23. 23. Archer J (1991) The influence of testosterone on human aggression. Br J Psychol:82: 1–28. pmid:2029601
  24. 24. Book AS, Starzyk KB, Quinsey VL (2001) The relationship between testosterone and aggression: A meta-analysis. Aggress Violent Behav:6: 579–599.
  25. 25. Slimani M, Moalla W, Baker JS (2017) Hormonal responses to a rugby match: a brief review. J Sports Med Phys Fitness [Epub ahead of print]
  26. 26. Wang C, Plymate S, Nieschlag E, Paulsen CA (1981) Salivary testosterone in men: Further evidence of a direct correlation with free serum testosterone. J Clin. Endocrinol Metab: 53: 1021–1024. pmid:7197285
  27. 27. Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. Hillside. NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.
  28. 28. Hopkins WG (2002) A scale of magnitudes for effect statistics. http://sportsciorg/resource/stats/effectmaghtml.Accessed October 20, 2013.
  29. 29. Coelho DB, Pimenta EM, Veneroso CE, Morandi RF, Pacheco DAS, Pereira ER, Coelho LGM, Silami-Garcia E (2013) Assessment of acute physiological demand for soccer. Rev Bras Cineantropom Desempenho Hum:15: 667–676.
  30. 30. Archer J (2006) Testosterone and human aggression: an evaluation of the challenge hypothesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev:30: 319–345. pmid:16483890
  31. 31. Parmigiani S, Dadomo H, Bartolomucci A, Brain PF, Carbucicchio A, Costantino C, Ferrari PF, Palanza P, Volpi R (2009). Personality traits and endocrine response as possible asymmetry factors of agonistic outcome in karate athletes. Aggress Behav:35(4): 324–33. pmid:19373899
  32. 32. Souglis A, Bogdanis G, Giannopoulou I, Papadopoulos C, Apostolidis N (2015) Comparison of inflammatory responses and muscle damage indices following a soccer, basketball, volleyball and handball game at an elite competitive level. Res Sports Med:23: 59–72. pmid:25630247
  33. 33. Morris T (2000) Psychological characteristics and talent identification in soccer. J. Sports Sci:18(9): 715–726. pmid:11043897
  34. 34. Slimani M, Bragazzi NL, Tod D, Dellal A, Hue O, Cheour F, Taylor L, Chamari K. (2016) Do cognitive training strategies improve motor and positive psychological skills development in soccer players? Insights from a systematic review. J Sports Sci:34(24): 2338–2349. pmid:27842463
  35. 35. Stolen T, Chamari K, Castagna C, Wisløff U (2005) Physiology of soccer: An update. Sports Med:35: 501–536. pmid:15974635
  36. 36. Silva JR, Ascensão A, Marques F, Seabra A, Rebelo A, Magalhães J (2013) Neuromuscular function, hormonal and redox status and muscle damage of professional soccer players after a high-level competitive match. Eur J Appl Physiol: 113: 2193–2201. pmid:23661147
  37. 37. Zouhal H, Jacob C, Delamarche P, Gratas-Delamarche A (2008) Catecholamines and the effects of exercise, training and gender. Sports Med:38: 401–423. pmid:18416594
  38. 38. Romagnoli M, Sanchis-Gomar F, Alis R, Risso-Ballester J, Bosio A, Graziani R, Rampinini E (2016) Changes in muscle damage, inflammation, and fatigue-related parameters in young elite soccer players after a match. J Sports Med Phys Fitness:56(10): 1198–1205. pmid:26558831
  39. 39. Aizawa K, Nakahori C, Akimoto T, Kimura F (2006) Changes of pituitary, adrenal and gonadal hormones during competition among female soccer players. J Sports Med Phys Fitness:46: 322. pmid:16823365
  40. 40. Carli G, Bonifazi M, Lodi L, Lupo C, Martelli G, Viti A (1986) Hormonal and metabolic effects following a football match. Int J Sports Med:7: 36–38. pmid:3007380
  41. 41. Lupo C, Baldi L, Bonifazi M, Lodi L, Martelli G, Viti A, Carli G (1985) Androgen levels following a football match. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol:54: 494–496. pmid:4085477
  42. 42. McHale TS, Zava DT, Hales D, Gray PB (2016) Physical competition increases dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) and androstenedione rather than testosterone among Juvenile boy soccer players. Adapt Human Behav Physiol:2: 44–56.
  43. 43. Peñailillo L, Maya L, Niño G, Torres H, Zbinden-Foncea H (2015) Salivary hormones and IgA in relation to physical performance in football. J Sports Sci:33: 2080–2087. pmid:26146019
  44. 44. Edwards DA, Wetzel K, Wyner DR (2006) Intercollegiate soccer: Saliva cortisol and testosterone are elevated during competition, and testosterone is related to status and social connectedness with teammates. Physiol Behav:87: 135–143. pmid:16233905
  45. 45. Casanova N, Palmeira-de-Oliveira A, Pereira A, Crisóstomo L, Travassos B, Costa A (2016) Cortisol, testosterone and mood state variation during an official female football competition. J Sports Med Phys Fitness:56(6): 775–81. pmid:26154730