Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Avian Egg Odour Encodes Information on Embryo Sex, Fertility and Development

Abstract

Avian chemical communication is a rapidly emerging field, but has been hampered by a critical lack of information on volatile chemicals that communicate ecologically relevant information (semiochemicals). A possible, but as yet unexplored, function of olfaction and chemical communication in birds is in parent-embryo and embryo-embryo communication. Communication between parents and developing embryos may act to mediate parental behaviour, while communication between embryos can control the synchronicity of hatching. Embryonic vocalisations and vibrations have been implicated as a means of communication during the later stages of development but in the early stages, before embryos are capable of independent movement and vocalisation, this is not possible. Here we show that volatiles emitted from developing eggs of Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) convey information on egg fertility, along with the sex and developmental status of the embryo. Specifically, egg volatiles changed over the course of incubation, differed between fertile and infertile eggs, and were predictive of embryo sex as early as day 1 of incubation. Egg odours therefore have the potential to facilitate parent-embryo and embryo-embryo interactions by allowing the assessment of key measures of embryonic development long before this is possible through other modalities. It also opens up the intriguing possibility that parents may be able to glean further relevant information from egg volatiles, such as the health, viability and heritage of embryos. By determining information conveyed by egg-derived volatiles, we hope to stimulate further investigation into the ecological role of egg odours.

Introduction

The eggs of many species of fish and insects emit volatile compounds that act, incidentally, as cues to predators and parasites as to their location [14]. Birds’ eggs also produce odorous compounds during incubation [5] although their ecological significance has been almost completely unexplored; not least because until recently the sense of smell in birds has been largely underappreciated [611]. One possible role for egg odours may be as a component of parent-embryo and embryo-embryo interactions. It is well-established that embryo-parent communication occurs late on during incubation, when the embryo first begins to vocalise within the shell [12]. This is used to communicate thermal status [13,14] and may also be used to advertise developmental status, synchronise hatching, solicit care and discourage nest abandonment [1517]. However, because the embryonic vocalisation system does not develop until a few days prior to hatching [12], this would not be a possible means of embryo-parent communication during the majority of incubation. Instead, chemical cues released through the porous egg shell may allow parents to assess the status of developing embryos. For example, nitric oxide (NO) produced by developing embryos has been suggested to play a role in mediating brood-patch development, although this has been viewed as a passive process in which released NO directly stimulates development [18] and not as an active process in which egg odours mediate adaptive behavioural changes in incubating parents.

Embryo-embryo communication can facilitate the timing of hatching, which is important for early performance in many bird species [19]. In Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica), for example, placement of eggs at different stages of development in close proximity can act to advance or retard development so that hatching occurs synchronously, and both vocalisations and vibrations emanating from within adjacent eggs have been implicated as possible mechanisms [20]. However, as discussed above such cues are only useful during the latter stages of incubation, and no study has yet attempted to confirm or refute the possibility that the exchange of chemical cues between eggs may play a role in embryo-embryo communication.

Before the ecological role of egg odours can be investigated, however, the information conveyed by egg-derived volatiles must first be determined. Avian chemical ecology is a rapidly emerging field but is hampered by a critical shortage of information on bird semiochemicals [7] and no study has yet determined what information, if any, is conveyed in egg odours. In this paper, we look at odours produced by Japanese quail eggs during incubation, and specifically test whether egg volatiles reflect egg fertility, embryonic development, and embryo sex; information which is potentially valuable in both parent-embryo and embryo-embryo communication. At the beginning of incubation the embryo is poorly developed, with no known differences between male and female embryos [21] and no reason to expect compositional differences between eggs containing fertile or infertile embryos. All eggs should therefore have similar odours. However, as incubation progresses, it is expected that volatiles produced as a direct result of embryonic metabolism will result in increasingly divergent volatile composition between fertile and infertile eggs, while sex differences in embryonic growth rate, metabolism or physiology [2225], will result in emergent sex differences. We therefore make two specific predictions: (1) there will be an interaction between fertility and developmental stage, with egg volatile composition initially not differing between fertile and infertile eggs, but diverging later in development; and (2) there will be an interaction between embryo sex and developmental stage, with no sex differences during the early stages of incubation, but differences between eggs containing male and female embryos emerging during later stages.

Materials and Methods

(a) Egg incubation

Freshly-laid Japanese quail eggs were obtained from a commercial supplier (Paslow Common Farm, Essex, UK) and stored in a refrigerator at 4°C for no more than 5 days until use. Immediately prior to incubation, eggs were gently wiped with ethanol to remove any residue before being quickly rinsed with water and dried using a paper towel. Eggs were then transferred to an incubator (Octagon 20 Advance Incubator with autoturn cradle, Brinsea Products Ltd, Sandford, UK) maintained at 37.5°C and 60% humidity, and incubated for 8 days (out of a total incubation period of ~17 days). This reflects the developmental period we are most interested in, prior to the onset of auditory and vibrational sensitivity during the second half of incubation [26,27].

(b) Volatile collection

Volatile collection was performed on day 1 and day 8 of incubation, as follows. A single egg was transferred to a glass jar (50 mm high, 34 mm diameter) sealed with aluminium foil and placed on a heating block to maintain the temperature at 37.5°C. The egg was left for 20 min to allow volatiles to equilibrate with the headspace of the jar, after which a 60μm PDMS/DVB Stableflex 24 gauge solid phase microextraction (SPME) fibre with holder (Supelco, Bellefonte, PA, USA) was inserted through the aluminium foil for 20 min. Eggs were returned to the incubator immediately following volatile collection. Volatiles were collected from empty glass jars, which were otherwise treated in exactly the same way, for comparison. After day 8, eggs were opened to check for the presence of developing embryos. In total, 65 eggs were incubated and subject to volatile collection, 50 of which had a developed embryo on day 8 and 15 of which had no visible embryo and so were deemed infertile. Incubation and volatile collection was carried out over two separate occasions, with approximately half the eggs present together in the same incubator on each occasion.

(c) Chemical analysis

Chemical identification was achieved by coupled gas chromatography-mass spectrometry (GCMS). Immediately after volatile collection, the SPME fibre was transferred to the injector port of a Shimadzu GCMS-QP2010S fitted with an HP-1MS Ultra Inert column (30m x 0.25mm, 0.25um film thickness). The injector port was at 250°C in splitless mode and fitted with an SPME liner. Oven temperature was maintained at 40°C for 1 min, then programmed at 10°C min−1 until 240°C and held for 15 min. The carrier gas was helium at 1 ml min−1. The transfer line temperature was 240°C and ion source was at 200°C. Ionization was by electron impact at 70eV. Peaks that were consistently present in greater amounts than in empty glass jar controls were identified by comparison of spectra with those of a database (NIST 05) and confirmed by comparison of mass spectra and retention times with those of commercially available authentic standards of each putative compound.

Quantification was achieved using single ion counts of identified peaks, selecting ions that were not present in neighbouring peaks on the chromatogram. Calibration curves were constructed for each compound by injecting authentic standards into a conventional injection port liner at a range of known concentrations in acetonitrile. In total 24 volatile compounds were detected (Table 1, S1 and S2 Tables), three of which could not be identified (denoted unidentified 1–3): they had Kovats retention indices of 988, 1087, and 1128, respectively, and their dominant ions were m/z 81, 110 and 79 (unidentified 1); 67, 95 and 124 (unidentified 2); and 41, 55 and 97 (unidentified 3). Since we could not determine concentrations of these compounds, for analysis we used mean single ion counts for these unidentified compounds (1, m/z 110; 2, m/z 124; and 3, m/z 97) and 1,3-diphenyl propane (m/z 92), for which no authentic standard was commercially available.

thumbnail
Table 1. Correlations of canonical axes of principle coordinates (CAP) axes with compounds identified in egg odour.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0116345.t001

For the analysis of the fertility data, CAP axis 1 corresponds to separation between day 1 and day 8 eggs, and CAP axis 2 corresponds to separation between fertile and infertile eggs (see also Fig. 1). For the analysis of the sex data, CAP axis 1 corresponds to separation between day 1 and day 8 eggs, and CAP axis 2 corresponds to separation between eggs containing male and female embryos (see also Fig. 2). Absolute correlation coefficients >0.5 are shown in bold.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Temporal variation in the odour profiles of fertile and infertile eggs.

Canonical analysis of principal coordinates (CAP) showing separation of the multivariate odour composition of fertile (black data points) and infertile (white data points) eggs on day 1 (circles) and day 8 (triangles) of incubation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0116345.g001

thumbnail
Figure 2. Temporal variation in the odour profiles of eggs containing male and female embryos.

Canonical analysis of principal coordinates (CAP) showing separation of the multivariate odour composition of eggs containing male (black data points) and female (white data points) eggs on day 1 (circles) and day 8 (triangles) of incubation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0116345.g002

(d) Molecular sexing

Following collection of volatiles on day 8, a random sample of developed embryos (n = 42) were subject to molecular sexing, blind and in a random order. Embryos were weighed (± 0.01 g) and genomic DNA was extracted from approximately 0.1 g embryonic tissue using the DNeasy Blood & Tissue Kit (Qiagen, Valencia, CA). Samples were sexed using PCR amplification of part of the sex-linked CHD1 genes, CHD1W and CHD1Z, which map to the avian W and Z chromosomes, respectively, using primers 2718R and 2550F [28]. PCR products were separated on 2% agarose gels and visualised with ethidium bromide staining. Females were characterized by displaying a CHD1W-specific fragment (1.2 kb in size) plus a shorter CHD1Z-specific fragment (0.7 kb), while males showed only the shorter Z-fragment.

(e) Statistical analysis

Quantities of each detected compound were standardised by dividing by the highest concentration of that compound collected. Four compounds that could not be quantified (unidentified 1, 2 and 3 and 1,3-diphenyl propane) were assumed to have a linear relationship between single ion count and quantity and standardised in the same way as for identified compounds. Since we had no a priori knowledge of which compounds are most important or most readily detected or discriminated, this transformation ensured that all compounds were treated equally in the analysis and there was no bias towards particularly abundant compounds.

To test whether variation in the composition of volatiles (i.e., odour profiles) was explained by an interaction between fertility and day of incubation, as predicted, we performed a permutational (non-parametric) multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) [29], using the ‘adonis’ function in the ‘vegan’ package [30] for R (version 2.15.2), with the interaction between fertility (fertile or infertile) and day of incubation (day 1 or day 8) as a fixed effect and permutations constrained by egg identity to account for measurements being taken from the same eggs on day 1 and day 8 of incubation. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons were conducted using canonical analyses [21,22], which include a permutation test of differences in odour composition between subgroups, and outputs the largest root test statistic, δ12. To test our second prediction, that variation in the odour profiles of fertile eggs could be explained by an interaction between embryo sex and stage of development, we performed a similar non-parametric MANOVA exploring the interaction between embryo sex and day of incubation. This general approach has been successfully used elsewhere to test for differences in avian odour composition [31].

Odour composition data were visualised using canonical analysis of principal coordinates (CAP) [32,33], a multivariate ordination technique. Pearson’s correlations between canonical axes and standardised abundance data for each compound were used to indicate the relative contribution of each compound to any separation observed [3133], with greater absolute values of the correlation coefficient denoting a greater contribution of that compound to separation between groups. Note that these correlations should not be interpreted in a causative way [31], and so we do not attempt to assign statistical significance to them (cf. [31]); instead, we simply note which compounds make a relatively large contribution to the observed separation (arbitrarily defined as those with an absolute correlation coefficient >0.5).

(f) Ethical statement

This study was approved by the University of Lincoln Ethical Review Committee, and was conducted in strict accordance with the laws of the UK.

Results

(a) Embryo fertility and development

There was a significant interaction between fertility and day of incubation in explaining variation in odour composition (F3,126 = 4.49, P < 0.001). All eggs exhibited a change in odour composition between day 1 and day 8 (fertile eggs: δ12 = 0.79, P < 0.001; infertile eggs: δ12 = 0.85, P < 0.001); however, while there was no significant difference in volatile composition between fertile and infertile eggs on day 1 (δ12 = 0.15, P = 0.738), by day 8 the difference was highly significant (δ12 = 0.86, P < 0.001) (Fig. 1).

The greatest contributions to separation along CAP axis 1 (Fig. 1), which corresponds to separation according to day of incubation, were made by methyl benzene, phenylethene, 5-isopropenyl-1-methyl-1-cyclohexene (which were more abundant on day 1 than day 8; S1 Table) and unidentified 2 (which was most abundant on day 8; S1 Table). No single compound contributed substantially to separation of volatile profiles from fertile and infertile eggs (along CAP axis 2 in Fig. 1) (Table 1).

(b) Embryo sex and development

Variation in odour composition was significantly explained by the interaction between embryo sex and day of incubation (F3,80 = 4.76, P < 0.001). There was a significant change in volatile composition between days 1 and 8 for eggs containing both male (δ12 = 0.92, P < 0.001) and female (δ12 = 0.57, P < 0.001) embryos, and, surprisingly, there were differences in odour profiles between eggs containing male and female embryos on both day 1 (δ12 = 0.63, P = 0.042) and day 8 (δ12 = 0.57, P = 0.014) (Fig. 2). There was no difference in weight between male and female embryos on day 8 (two-sample t-test: t38 = −0.01, P = 0.995).

For day of incubation (separation along CAP axis 1 in Fig. 2), the greatest contributions were made by methyl benzene, phenylethene, 5-isopropenyl-1-methyl-1-cyclohexene and unidentified 2, as in the fertility analysis, with the addition of unidentified 1 and 1,3-diacetylbenzene (both most abundant on day 8; S1 Table). The compounds contributing the most to separation of volatile profiles from male and female eggs (along CAP axis 2 in Fig. 2) were benzaldehyde, phenol, acetophenone, 2-nonanone, 2-decanone and 2-undecanone (Table 1), which were all more abundant in eggs containing female embryos (S1 Table).

Discussion

The results of this study show that both the fertility of an egg and the day of incubation interactively affect the composition of volatiles emitted during incubation, and, for fertile eggs, volatile composition is predictive of embryo sex, not only on day 8 as hypothesised but also on day 1 of incubation. To our knowledge, this is the first study to characterise the odour profiles of bird eggs containing developing embryos. As such, many of those volatiles identified are not widely reported in the literature and add to our limited knowledge of potential bird semiochemicals [7].

The lack of difference observed between the odour profiles of fertile and infertile eggs on day 1 was not unexpected since at this stage embryonic development has barely begun and there would be no reason to predict differences in egg composition. By day 8, however, a significant difference in volatile composition had developed, presumably driven by a combination of embryonic metabolism and utilisation of egg components that were absent from infertile eggs, possibly augmented by biochemical and microbial changes in infertile eggs [34]. Birds invest considerable time and energy in the production and incubation of eggs and being able to detect fertile eggs within a clutch could be advantageous, for example to facilitate decisions on whether or not to abandon the nest in the event of increased predation or reduced availability of food resources; although whether olfaction could be used to mediate nest abandonment decisions is not known and would make an interesting area of future research.

The finding that the volatile composition differed between eggs containing male and female embryos, not only on day 8—by which time sex differences in embryonic growth rate or selective utilisation of egg components [35] may have contributed to the observed differences—but also on day 1 was extremely unexpected. It is unlikely that action by the embryos themselves could have contributed to this difference (although this cannot be ruled out entirely), and so we suggest that the observed differences in odour composition may have been driven by differential maternal allocation of resources to male and female eggs, resulting in differential emission of volatiles. For example, female birds are known to allocate various yolk components, including hormones [36], antibodies [37] and possibly antioxidants and vitamins [38], to eggs in a sex-specific manner, although it has never been investigated whether these compounds could contribute to the odour of avian eggs. However, it is noteworthy that one of the volatile ketones which differed between eggs containing male and female embryos (2-undecanone), has previously been identified as a hormone-linked constituent of avian odour [39,40]. Regardless of the underlying mechanism, the implications of sex differences in the volatiles from avian eggs are considerable. In particular it may provide a mechanism by which parents can selectively allocate incubation effort to eggs of one sex over the other, perhaps to differentially enhance or retard growth in species with hatching asynchrony [23,41], and possibly to form the basis for a post-ovulatory means of sex ratio manipulation [42]. More pragmatically, the poultry industry would benefit from the ability to detect egg sex at an early developmental stage [43].

Some compounds showed a marked decrease in emission from day 1 to day 8 of incubation. Chicken (Gallus gallus) eggs are capable of absorbing extraneous odours [44] and it is possible that some volatiles may have been absorbed through the porous shell during or after laying and then re-emitted, at decreasing rates as specific volatiles are expended, over time. A similar phenomenon has been observed in plants that can absorb and re-emit volatiles produced by neighbouring plants [45]. This could explain the emission patterns of compounds such as methylbenzene and phenylethene, which are not commonly associated with biological activity. However, several compounds (e.g. 1,3-diacetyl benzene, heptanal and unidentified 1 and 2 in S1 Table) were produced in greater quantities by day 8 eggs compared to day 1 eggs showing that differences in odour over time are not solely due to gradual depletion of absorbed volatiles. If eggs are capable of absorbing odours from the nest or from the incubating parent, this raises the possibility that absorbed odours may facilitate egg recognition. Japanese quail can detect visual differences between host and foreign eggs, although the use of visual cues alone may not be sufficient when differences in maculation patterns and shape are minor [46]. Given the accumulating evidence that birds can recognise the odour of their nests [8,9,47], odour cues may facilitate egg recognition in such situations which could possibly be aided by absorption of nest odours.

Chemical communication between animals has largely focussed on insects and mammals. The lack of such studies on birds is probably a result of an under-appreciation of the avian olfactory sense, despite an increasing number of studies showing that olfaction is a well-developed and ecologically important trait in many bird species [6,7]. This is the first study to characterise the volatile chemicals given off by developing bird eggs and show that these volatiles convey ecologically relevant information on the developmental status of the embryo. Whether or not these volatiles play a role in mediating parental incubation behaviour or embryo-embryo communication remains to be determined, however, and future work should address this, for example by employing artificial eggs emitting volatile blends based on those identified. Egg odours may also provide information on a range of other factors relevant to egg development long before this information is available via other sensory modalities. These include embryonic health (for example volatile markers of oxidative stress caused by the breakdown of lipids by rapidly developing embryos [48], or odours resulting from microbial infection), growth rate, egg temperature, and the recognition of eggs from inter- and intra-specific brood parasites. For instance, hexanal is an important volatile decomposition product of hydroperoxides formed from n-6 polyunsaturated lipids [49], which are present in the yolk of quail eggs [50], and so may indicate embryonic susceptibility to lipid peroxidation. Odours may also be used to solicit parental care and allow assessment of maternal quality [51], although the behavioural responses of parents to variation in egg odour remains to be established. These results therefore open up a host of interesting questions on the possible role of egg semiochemicals, and could pave the way for further advances in avian chemical ecology.

Supporting Information

S1 Table. Egg volatiles as a function of developmental stage and fertility.

List of compounds and mean (± SE) quantities (ng) collected over 20 min from fertile and infertile Japanese quail eggs on day 1 and day 8 of incubation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0116345.s001

(PDF)

S2 Table. Egg volatiles as a function of developmental stage and embryo sex.

List of compounds and mean (± SE) quantities (ng) collected over 20 min from Japanese quail eggs containing male and female embryos on day 1 and day 8 of incubation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0116345.s002

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We thank Charles Deeming for valuable discussion and advice.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: BW WH TWP. Performed the experiments: BW WH. Analyzed the data: BW TWP. Wrote the paper: BW WH TWP.

References

  1. 1. Boal JG, Prosser KN, Holm JB, Simmons TL, Haas RE, et al. (2010) Sexually Mature Cuttlefish are Attracted to the Eggs of Conspecifics. J Chem Ecol 36: 834–836. pmid:20593225
  2. 2. Meiners T, Westerhaus C, Hilker M (2000) Specificity of chemical cues used by a specialist egg parasitoid during host location. Entomol Exp Appl 95: 151–159.
  3. 3. Mirza RS, Chivers DP (2002) Attraction of slimy sculpins to chemical cues of brook charr eggs. J Fish Biol 61: 532–539.
  4. 4. Yavno S, Corkum LD (2011) Round goby Neogobius melanostomus attraction to conspecific and heterospecific egg odours. J Fish Biol 78: 1944–1953. pmid:21651542
  5. 5. Spurr EB (1999) Developing a long-life toxic bait and lures for mustelids. Science for Conservation 127A: 1–24.
  6. 6. Balthazart J, Taziaux M (2009) The underestimated role of olfaction in avian reproduction? Behav Brain Res 200: 248–259. pmid:18804490
  7. 7. Campagna S, Mardon J, Celerier A, Bonadonna F (2012) Potential Semiochemical Molecules from Birds: A Practical and Comprehensive Compilation of the Last 20 Years Studies. Chem Senses 37: 3–25. pmid:21798850
  8. 8. Cunningham GB, Van Buskirk RW, Hodges MJ, Nevitt GA (2012) Responses of common diving petrel chicks (Pelecanoides urinatrix) to burrow and colony specific odours in a simple wind tunnel. Antarct Sci 24: 337–341.
  9. 9. Rajchard J (2008) Exogenous chemical substances in bird perception: a review. Vet Med-Czech 53: 412–419.
  10. 10. Steiger SS, Fidler AE, Valcu M, Kempenaers B (2008) Avian olfactory receptor gene repertoires: evidence for a well-developed sense of smell in birds? P Roy Soc B-Biol Sci 275: 2309–2317. pmid:18628122
  11. 11. Wallraff HG (2004) Avian olfactory navigation: its empirical foundation and conceptual state. Anim Behav 67: 189–204.
  12. 12. Brua RB (2002) Parent-embryo interactions. In: Deeming DC, editor. Avian incubation: behaviour, environment and evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 88–99.
  13. 13. Evans RM (1990) Embryonic Fine Tuning of Pipped Egg Temperature in the American White Pelican. Anim Behav 40: 963–968.
  14. 14. Evans RM (1990) Vocal Regulation of Temperature by Avian Embryos—a Laboratory Study with Pipped Eggs of the American White Pelican. Anim Behav 40: 969–979.
  15. 15. Berlin KE, Clark AB (1998) Embryonic calls as care-soliciting signals in budgerigars, Melopsittacus undulatus. Ethology 104: 531–544.
  16. 16. Brua RB, Nuechterlein GL, Buitron D (1996) Vocal response of Eared Grebe embryos to egg cooling and egg turning. Auk 113: 525–533.
  17. 17. Schwagmeyer PL, Mock DW, Lamey TC, Lamey CS, Beecher MD (1991) Effects of Sibling Contact on Hatch Timing in an Asynchronously Hatching Bird. Anim Behav 41: 887–894.
  18. 18. Ar A, Ifergan O, Feldman A, Zelik L, Reizis A (2004) Possible role of nitric oxide emission from bird embryos. Avian Poult Biol Rev 15: 105–106.
  19. 19. Reed WL, Clark ME (2011) Beyond Maternal Effects in Birds: Responses of the Embryo to the Environment. Integr Comp Biol 51: 73–80. pmid:21624931
  20. 20. Vince MA, Cheng R (1970) Retardation of Hatching in Japanese Quail. Anim Behav 18: 210–213.
  21. 21. McNabb FMA, Scanes CG, Zeman M (1998) Endocrine control of development. In: Starck JM, Ricklefs RE, editors. Avian Growth and Development. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 288–304.
  22. 22. Badyaev AV (2002) Growing apart: an ontogenetic perspective on the evolution of sexual size dimorphism. Trends Ecol Evol 17: 369–378.
  23. 23. Cook MI, Monaghan P (2004) Sex differences in embryo development periods and effects on avian hatching patterns. Behav Ecol 15: 205–209.
  24. 24. Kochhar HPS, Peippo J, King WA (2001) Sex related embryo development. Theriogenology 55: 3–14. pmid:11198086
  25. 25. Petrie M, Schwabl H, Brande-Lavridsen N, Burke T (2001) Maternal investment—Sex differences in avian yolk hormone levels. Nature 412: 498–498. pmid:11484039
  26. 26. Golubeya TB, Tichonov AV. The voice of hearing of birds in ontogeny. Acta XVIII Congressus Internationalis Ornithologici; 1985. pp. 259–274.
  27. 27. Rubel EW M, editor. Handbook of sensory physiology: development of sensory stimulation. Berlin: Springer. pp. 135–237.
  28. 28. Fridolfsson AK, Ellegren H (1999) A simple and universal method for molecular sexing of non-ratite birds. J Avian Biol 30: 116–121.
  29. 29. Anderson MJ (2001) A new method for non-parametric multivariate analysis of variance. Austral Ecol 26: 32–46. pmid:11469182
  30. 30. Oksanen JF, Blanchet G, Kindt R, Legendre P, Minchin PR, et al. (2013) vegan: Community Ecology Package.
  31. 31. Mardon J, Saunders SM, Anderson MJ, Couchoux C, Bonadonna F (2010) Species, Gender, and Identity: Cracking Petrels’ Sociochemical Code. Chem Senses 35: 309–321. pmid:20190009
  32. 32. Anderson MJ, Robinson J (2003) Generalized discriminant analysis based on distances. Aust Nz J Stat 45: 301–318.
  33. 33. Anderson MJ, Willis TJ (2003) Canonical analysis of principal coordinates: A useful method of constrained ordination for ecology. Ecology 84: 511–525.
  34. 34. Jahantigh M (2012) A study of bacteriologic status of infertile ostrich (Struthio camelus) eggs. Comp Clin Pathol 21: 1049–1051.
  35. 35. Martins TLF (2004) Sex-specific growth rates in zebra finch nestlings: a possible mechanism for sex ratio adjustment. Behav Ecol 15: 174–180.
  36. 36. Gilbert L, Rutstein AN, Hazon N, Graves JA (2005) Sex-biased investment in yolk androgens depends on female quality and laying order in zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Naturwissenschaften 92: 178–181. pmid:15668780
  37. 37. Saino N, Romano M, Ferrari RP, Martinelli R, Moller AP (2003) Maternal antibodies but not carotenoids in barn swallow eggs covary with embryo sex. J Evolution Biol 16: 516–522.
  38. 38. Badyaev AV, Seaman DA, Navara KJ, Hill GE, Mendonca MT (2006) Evolution of sex-biased maternal effects in birds: III. Adjustment of ovulation order can enable sex-specific allocation of hormones, carotenoids, and vitamins. J Evolution Biol 19: 1044–1057. pmid:16780506
  39. 39. Whittaker DJ, Soini HA, Gerlach NM, Posto AL, Novotny MV, et al. (2011) Role of Testosterone in Stimulating Seasonal Changes in a Potential Avian Chemosignal. J Chem Ecol 37: 1349–1357. pmid:22173888
  40. 40. Whittaker DJ, Soini HA, Atwell JW, Hollars C, Novotny MV, et al. (2010) Songbird chemosignals: volatile compounds in preen gland secretions vary among individuals, sexes, and populations. Behav Ecol 21: 608–614. pmid:22475692
  41. 41. Magrath RD (1992) Roles of Egg Mass and Incubation Patter in Establishment of Hatching Hierarchies in the Blackbird (Turdus-Merula). Auk 109: 474–487.
  42. 42. Pike TW, Petrie M (2003) Potential mechanisms of avian sex manipulation. Biol Rev 78: 553–574. pmid:14700391
  43. 43. Weissmann A, Reitemeier S, Hahn A, Gottschalk J, Einspanier A (2013) Sexing domestic chicken before hatch: A new method for in ovo gender identification. Theriogenology 80: 199–205. pmid:23726296
  44. 44. Maga JA (1982) Egg and Egg Product Flavor. J Agr Food Chem 30: 9–14.
  45. 45. Himanen SJ, Blande JD, Holopainen JK (2010) Plant-emitted semi-volatiles shape the infochemical environment and herbivore resistance of heterospecific neighbors. Plant Signaling & Behavior 5: 1234–1236. pmid:25562843
  46. 46. Pike TW (2011) Egg recognition in Japanese quail. Avian Biol Res 4: 231–236.
  47. 47. Grubb TC (1974) Olfactory Navigation to Nesting Burrow in Leachs Petrel (Oceanodroma-Leucorrhoa). Anim Behav 22: 192–202. pmid:4836743
  48. 48. Surai PF (2002) Natural antioxidants in avian nutrition and reproduction. Nottingham: Nottingham University Press. pmid:25057650
  49. 49. Frankel EN, Hu ML, Tappel AL (1989) Rapid Headspace Gas-Chromatography of Hexanal as a Measure of Lipid-Peroxidation in Biological Samples. Lipids 24: 976–981. pmid:2615566
  50. 50. Trimeche A, Anton M, Renard P, Gandemer G, Tainturier D (1997) Quail egg yolk: A novel cryoprotectant for the freeze preservation of Poitou jackass sperm. Cryobiology 34: 385–393. pmid:9200823
  51. 51. Moreno J, Osorno JL (2003) Avian egg colour and sexual selection: does eggshell pigmentation reflect female condition and genetic quality? Ecol Lett 6: 803–806.