Main

Functional brain organization can be assessed by applying analyses to EEG-sleep state architecture and continuity measures. The observation of sequentially recurring eye movement intervals alternating with no eye movement epochs was initially described in the newborn infant as early as the 1950s (1). Such a sequence suggests either the presence of a highly periodic process or, alternatively, the temporal coincidence, with variable predictability, among physiologic behaviors that sometimes define state transitions. Early investigators suggested the presence of periodicity in infant sleep measures (2,3). Sterman et al. (4) observed periodicity from early gestational ages (GAs), beginning with the basic rest-activity cycle in the fetus and the sleep cycle in infants. Globus (5) described cyclicity at all GAs, with predictable rhythmicity after 36 wk postconceptional age (PCA). Other authors stressed the clinical importance of sleep periodicity, as exemplified by reports of disturbed cyclic organization of sleep in infants with CNS disorders (6) as well as asymptomatic infants at risk for sudden infant death syndrome (7,8).

Specific investigations questioned the definition of sleep state cyclicity when applying alternative methods, particularly when states were considered categorical events (5,9,10). On the basis of these specific methodologic approaches, sleep cyclicity did not appear until at least 6 wk of life (11), 4–6 mo postnatally (9,12), or 1 y of life (13). Using the same strict method, Borghese et al. (14) demonstrated sleep cyclicity in 40% of healthy preterm infants at 36 wk PCA, with 90% cyclicity noted by 6 mo of age. These authors suggested that preterm cyclicity may be a response to ongoing stress on the CNS, whereas cyclicity at 6 mo alternatively reflects CNS competence.

Studies of sleep state cyclicity for preterm infants who are <30 wk GA or PCA have not been widely reported, although some authors have suggested predictable sleep state transitions in this group of preterm infants by 27–30 wk GA, using rapid eye movements (REMs) and specific EEG patterns (15,16). Given the improving survival rates of extremely premature infants with GAs as early as 23–24 wk (17), determination of state periodicity may help advance neurointensive care practices and predict neurodevelopmental outcome. Our neonatal sleep consortium chose to investigate the presence of sleep cyclicity in preterm neonates who were 25–30 wk PCA, combining data from one common subcortical neuronal pathway involved in state regulation (REMs) with a cerebral measure easily identified by visual analysis (EEG discontinuity).

METHODS

From a total cohort of 359 neonates, a subset of the youngest 33 preterm infants who were 25–30 wk PCA (23–29 wk GA, 4–50 d of age) were chosen to analyze the first of serial 2- to 3-h EEG-sleep recordings. Institutional Review Board–approved informed consent was obtained for all study subjects. Our total cohort was composed of both healthy and medically ill neonates, recruited from the neonatal services of three obstetric/neonatal centers in Pittsburgh, Cleveland, and North Carolina. Medical health status was tabulated by numeric scoring methods designed for medically ill neonates (18). Clinical and demographic information identified the medical status of each child. One trained neonatal electroencephalographer/polysomnographer scored EEG-sleep states using Insight (universal EEG reading software; Persyst Development Corp., Prescott, AZ), including scoring of discontinuous EEG, arousals, movements, and REM counts over a 2- or 3-h recording time. These scores were transferred to a relational database (Microsoft, Redmond, WA) to facilitate storage and processing of raw scores into epoch-by-epoch scores. Direct annotation by the EEG technician and from review of video recordings were also included in the database.

Two criteria of state identification—1) REM, a noncerebral measure represented a subcortical pathway involved in state expression, and 2) intervals of low-amplitude EEG activities, termed EEG discontinuity—represented a cerebral measure involved in corticothalamic neuronal pathways (19). Other scoring rules included arousal identification as a sudden desynchronization or diminution of amplitude of frequencies on EEG channels with coincident abrupt alteration in the respiratory rate regularity and sometimes increased electromyogram activity. Indeterminate or transitional state segments were also identified.

EEG-sleep studies were carried out in an environmentally controlled setting in which sound, light, humidity, and tactile stimulation were monitored. All infants were studied while sleeping prone or on their side in an open bed or isolette, whichever was their usual sleeping position in the nursery. Continuous recordings began after a diaper change and feeding during the late morning to early afternoon hours. Each 24-channel 2- to 3-h study was recorded using a digital EEG machine (Computational Diagnostics, Pittsburgh, PA; Nicolet Biomedical, Madison, WI; Nihon Kohden America, Foothill Ranch, CA).

A neonatal nurse assisted the EEG technologist with clinical care for each infant during the recording session. Feeding behavior, diaper changes, medication administration, and technical comments were documented in the digital EEG. No infants were given medications during the studies.

EEG-sleep studies were visually analyzed by a single neonatal neurophysiologist (M.S.S.) who specifically identified REM epochs and periods of continuous and discontinuous EEG intervals. For EEG discontinuity, the start and the end of a duration of EEG attenuation or quiescence were marked (Fig. 1A).

Figure 1
figure 1

Segments of EEG from a preterm neonate at 30 wk PCA. (A) Quiet sleep as indicated by the marked segments of discontinuity. (B) Active sleep as indicated by the “Continuous EEG” annotation and the occurrence of REMs. The EEG channel display settings were 30 s per page, 10 μV/mm equivalent sensitivity, neonatal montage, 0.3 s time constant, 35 Hz high filter, and 60 Hz notch filter on. C, central; T, temporal; P, parietal; F, frontal; O, occipital; and odd numbers left, even numbers right, and z along the centerline; ROC, right outer canthus; LOC, left outer canthus.

Our analytic method applied an autocovariance analysis (see Appendix) to a measure of state. All calculations and analyses were performed using Igor Pro (Wavemetrics, Lake Oswego, OR). The state measure was defined from a weighted sum of REM per minute (upward) and seconds of EEG discontinuity per minute (downward), with weightings of +1 for REM and −1/2 for discontinuity). During epochs in which no REM and EEG discontinuity were present, such as may occur during segments of transitional or indeterminate sleep, the state measure was simply 0. The autocovariance is a measure of similarity of a signal to itself when the signal is shifted by the time τ relative to a copy of itself. Application of the autocovariance to the state measure results in a function that is positive when the signal and its shifted copy are in phase and negative when they are out of phase. The function is bounded between −1 and +1 and cycles above and below 0 when the signal is periodic. The first dominant peak along the time shift axis (excluding τ = 0) is the dominant average cycle period of the signal. Figure 1A shows a 30-s sample of discontinuous EEG data taken from a 30-wk preterm infant. Figure 1B shows a sample of continuous EEG with REM taken from the same infant 6 min later. Figure 2 illustrates an example of a 27-wk preterm infant showing the recording of REM, EEG discontinuity, and combined state epochs, followed by a smoothed graph of the autocovariance function of the combined REM/EEG state measure.

Figure 2
figure 2

A sample EEG-sleep cycle from a 27-wk corrected age preterm neonate. This figure illustrates the alternating occurrence of EEG discontinuity and REM based on visual analysis scoring (top) and the autocovariance function for the combined EEG state and REM (bottom).

RESULTS

Twenty-four neonates showed a mean cycle duration of 68 ± 19 min with a range of 37–100 min. Nine neonates had poor or no cyclicity present. No differences in sex, race, GA, PCA, ventilatory care, medication (e.g. theophylline), postnatal illnesses, or prenatal risk were seen between groups with recognizable cyclicity and those with poor or no cyclicity. Sleep state cyclicity was similar in duration within individuals, but cycle lengths were variable between individuals. Figures 3 and 4 indicate the combined state and autocovariance for selected preterm infants with and without recognizable cycling.

Figure 3
figure 3

Selected infants who demonstrated state cyclicity. (Left) Combined state (REM and EEG discontinuity). (Right) Autocovariance function of the combined state for the same infants, with the mean for cycle length for each neonate designated by an open circle. The postconceptional age is shown to the right of each infant.

Figure 4
figure 4

Selected infants who exhibited absent or poor sleep cyclicity (displayed as in Fig. 3).

DISCUSSION

Although it is generally assumed that CNS control of sleep begins during early life (1), on the basis of the recognition of REM and non-REM cycles, Goldie (20) argued that cyclicity may not be highly predictable and may only alternatively consist of a “sequence of coincident or coalescent behaviors” earlier in life. Cyclicity was initially suggested on the basis of the results of studies during the neonatal period by several authors during the 1970s and early 1980s (2,3,5,20,21), with the absence of cyclicity observed in encephalopathic infants (6) as well as asymptomatic high-risk infants for sudden infant death syndrome (22). Later reports by certain authors argued that more strict methods to define cyclicity failed to document sleep cycles until later during the first year of life. The appearance of sleep cyclicity was noted at 6 wk of age (11), 4–6 mo of age (9), or 1 y of age (13).

A study using a movement-sensitive mattress to detect state-related movements and respiratory behaviors documented cyclicity for 20 of 49 infants using these 24-h studies by 36 wk GA (14). Thirty-seven of 42 infants of this same cohort expressed sleep cyclicity using the same method at 6 mo of age. The authors documented a similar sleep cycle duration of 60 min that remained unchanged at the two ages when studies were performed. These authors also reported the presence of cyclicity with developmental outcome using a motor-weighted developmental score (the Bayley Score of Infant Development). The presence of cyclicity during the preterm period (at 36 wk gestation) correlated negatively with development at 6 mo of age, whereas sleep cyclicity correlated positively with developmental outcome at 6 mo of age. The authors speculated that the presence of cyclicity in the very immature brain may reflect stress on functional brain organization and maturation that later negatively affects outcome.

No studies have indicated the presence or absence of sleep state cyclicity in preterm infants who are <30 wk GA, although selected studies have suggested the coincidence among different physiologic behaviors during state transitions (15,16). Our findings suggest that preterm neonates with PCAs of 25–30 wk do express sleep state cyclicity, based on two criteria of state identification. We chose two separate neuronal networks that represent either a subcortical circuitry involving largely the upper brainstem (REM) and cerebral circuit involving thalamocortical pathways (EEG discontinuity).

We understand that there are several limitations to the present study. First, studies were performed over 2–3 h between feedings, which were too short to ascertain the diurnal effect on sleep state cyclicity, as well as the stability of sleep state cycling over a 24-h period. Second, electrographic detection of REMs using electro-oculographic channels (without supplementation by visual observation) would not be optimal during the preterm ages for all subjects. During this range of prematurity, eye movements create only a weak dipole because of an immature retina, sometimes resulting in a lack of discernible deflections on the electro-oculogram channels. Third, the number of days spent in the nursery and the child's sleep position compared with the other infants studied might influence cyclicity. These three limitations may in part explain why poor or absent sleep state cyclicity was noted for nine of our infants. Finally, our present population size is too small to compare neurodevelopmental outcome in those with and without cyclicity. Despite these limitations, we conclude that the presence of sleep state cyclicity exists for a majority of infants who are <30 wk PCA, based on an autocovariance estimate of sleep state cyclicity, independent of demographic or selected clinical influences.