Skip to main content

The ability of executive functions to predict deterioration in social abstraction in adolescents with idiopathic generalized epilepsy

Abstract

Background

Epilepsy is a prevalent disease worldwide. Executive functions and social cognition are essential in daily living functioning, academic performance, and social adjustment. This study attempts to investigate the executive functions and ability to abstract social concepts in adolescents who have idiopathic generalized epilepsy and assess if the deterioration in executive functions can predict deterioration in the ability to abstract social concepts. Sixty adolescents with idiopathic generalized epilepsy aged between 12 and 19 collected from the neurology outpatient clinic at Sohag University Hospital in Upper Egypt were compared with 60 age- and sex-matched healthy adolescents regarding performance on executive function tests and ability to abstract social concepts tests. We used the Tower test to assess planning, the design fluency test to assess cognitive flexibility, the verbal fluency test to assess verbal fluency, and the Stroop test (overlapping between colour and word test) to assess inhibition and ability to abstract the social concepts test, which is composed of verbal part and performance part.

Results

Adolescents who have idiopathic generalized epilepsy have worse executive function impairment in all executive functions than control adolescents. Adolescents with idiopathic epilepsy have significantly more impairment in the ability to abstract social concepts (verbal and performance) compared to control adolescents. There is a significant correlation between executive functions and the ability to abstract social concepts (verbal and performance) in epileptic and control adolescents. Impairment in some executive functions can predict impairment in the ability to abstract social concepts (verbal and performance) in epileptic and control adolescents. Executive function impairment is correlated with the duration of illness, frequency of seizures per year, time since last epileptic fit, and presence of interictal epileptic discharge in EEG in the patient group.

Conclusions

Executive functions and the ability to abstract social concepts are significantly impaired in adolescents who have idiopathic generalized epilepsy. Impairment in executive functions can predict impairment in the ability to abstract social concepts. There is a need to screen executive functions and social abilities of adolescents with epilepsy and programmes to enhance these abilities.

Background

Approximately 50–70 million individuals worldwide suffer from epilepsy [1]. Idiopathic epilepsy is described as an illness with no structural pathology or underlying aetiology other than a probably inherited predisposition for seizures that may be focal or generalized [2].

Executive functions cover the cognitive abilities required for goal-directed and complex activities, which include all areas including recalling short-term memory, forward-thinking, formulating plans, and initiating/inhibiting, as well as organizing and monitoring everyday life [3]. Executive functions are primarily high-level cognitive tasks regulated by the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). Cases with neuraxis structural anomalies may be predicted to exhibit executive dysfunction. Surprisingly, similar dysfunctions were detected in epilepsy patients with normal premorbid intelligence and normal neuroimaging examinations [3].

Social cognition (SC) has been characterized as the capacity to recognize, evaluate, and react appropriately to social cues in order to engage more effectively with the external environment [4, 5]. Multiple social cognitive skills can be recognized, and two are frequently studied in psychiatric as well as neurological populations: the first is social cue perception, which denotes our potential to utilize and perceive emotions from various sensory channels, such as peoples’ body language (e.g. postures, gestures, and gait), voices, and facial expressions and the second is the theory of mind (ToM) or mentalization, which refers to our thought processes and conceptual knowledge that help us to explain our emotions and others’ emotions [6, 7].

Many variables may contribute to cognitive impairment in epilepsy, including (a) seizures happening in cognitive development stages, (b) recurrent epileptiform discharges in interictal EEG, and (c) epilepsy pharmaceutical therapy (particularly polypharmacy). Cognitive impairment in epilepsy is thought to be caused by treatment-related malfunction, symptomatic epilepsy, and permanent, irreversible anatomical alterations [8].

This study attempts to assess the executive functions as well as the ability to abstract social concepts in adolescents who have idiopathic generalized epilepsy and assess the ability of executive functions to predict the deterioration in the ability to abstract social concepts.

Methods

This cross-sectional study was carried out between January 2020 and November 2020. We collected the patients from the neurology outpatient clinic at Sohag University Hospital. Controls were age- and sex-matched students from multiple schools in Sohag.

The population of the study is a convenient sample composed of 60 adolescents aged 12 to 19 years suffering from idiopathic generalized epilepsy and 60 age- and sex-matched healthy adolescents.

Inclusion criteria

Inclusion criteria include the following: age 12–19 years, I.Q (intelligence quotient) score of at least 80, and fluency in reading and writing. Patients were drug naïve (newly diagnosed or not compliant on treatment in the past 6 months) to exclude the effect of antiepileptic drugs on cognition.

Exclusion criteria

Exclusion criteria include the following: the presence of other neurological diseases, any lesion in brain imaging, organic cause of fits, patients with hearing or a visual impairment, patients with a history of substance abuse, or patients who were subjected to similar psychological tests like those applied in our study in the past 6 months.

Tools

Clinical interview with the patients using a preformed sheet covering important epidemiological, socioeconomic, and clinical data.

Brain imaging by computed tomography (C.T) or magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) brain.

Electroencephalography (EEG).

Psychometric tests

Stanford-Binet Intelligence Test fifth edition [9]

Assessment of the executive functions

  1. 1.

    Tower test to assess planning [10], which was first developed by Shallice in 1982. The participant moves discs of different sizes to form a specific shape presented by the examiner with respect to two rules: first, moving one disc at a time, and second not putting a larger size disc above a smaller size one. Total scoring by measuring f total time during performing all shapes.

  2. 2.

    Design fluency test to assess cognitive flexibility. First developed by Regard et al. in1982 [11]. We used the Arabic version by Khalil (2010) [12]. The participant draws as many shapes as possible for 60 s. Each participant has three trials. The test’s total score is the total number of shapes drawn in the three trials.

  3. 3.

    Verbal fluency test to assess verbal fluency. The participant has to find words initiated with particular letters as much as possible during 60 s. The total score is the number of words said by the participant. We utilized the Arabic version [11, 12].

  4. 4.

    Stroop test (overlapping between colour and word test) to assess inhibition Stroop (1935) [13]. We used the Arabic version by Al-Ghatani et al. (2010) [14]. The participant reads the colour of the ink or the actual word as demanded by the examiner. The total score is the total time spent in completing the test.

Assessment of social abstraction

We used the ability to abstract the social concepts test, which is composed of two parts [15]:

  1. 1.

    The ability to abstract the social concepts verbal test. This test is composed of 20 items. Each item is a sentence representing a social situation, and the participant is asked to choose the correct interpretation for this situation. The final score is determined by the number of right answers.

  2. 2.

    The ability to abstract the social concepts performance test. This test is composed of 85 cards; each card contains a picture representing a social situation. The cards are divided into 17 groups, and each group contains 5 cards, 4 of them are related to a specific social theme such as cooperation, selling, and rescue. The participant is asked to interpret why these cards are grouped together and which card does not belong to the group. The total score is the number of correct answers.

Steps and sequence of examination

At first, the consultant neurologist refers the cases with a confirmed diagnosis with idiopathic epilepsy with the exclusion of symptomatic causes and abnormal findings in C.T or MRI brain, then an interview by the researchers is done with the patients and their caregiver to collect the demographic and clinical data. Then, an I.Q test is done to the case, then the application of the executive functions tests, and then the social abstraction tests.

The control subjects were selected from multiple schools in Sohag, with an interview at the beginning, then I.Q testing, executive functions testing, and social abstraction testing. The interview was done in the psychologist’s room in the school.

We examined 65 epileptic patients, and we excluded 5 of them due to the presence of hysterical fits comorbidity or learning disabilities. We examined 62 controls and excluded 2 of them due to the presence of learning disabilities.

Ethical consideration

The research was authorized by the Faculty of Medicine’s Scientific Research Ethical Committee, Sohag University. Moreover, all subjects as well as their parents provided informed consent. Also, we got the agreement of the Ministry of Education for recruiting the control students.

Statistics analysis

The 16th version of SPSS software was utilized for data analysis. For descriptive statistics, quantitative data were expressed as standard deviation (SD) and mean, whereas qualitative data were expressed as percentages as well as frequencies. The study depended on the statistical analyses as follows: (1) independent sample t-test to compare between mean and standard deviations of the studied variables, (2) Pearson linear correlation coefficient, (3) Mann-Whitney test, and (4) stepwise multiple regression analysis. Statistical significance was set as a p value < 0.05.

Table 1 shows that there is no statistically significant difference between controls as well as patients in terms of sex, age, handedness, education years, or total I.Q score. The minimum I.Q score in both groups was 90 and the maximum score was 117 in patients and 120 in controls.

Table 1 Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics of patients and control subjects

Table 2 shows that all patients had normal C.T and MRI brain. Thirty-eight (63.33%) of patients had interictal epileptic discharge in EEG.

Table 2 Clinical characteristics and brain imaging findings in the patient group

Table 3 shows that there is a negative statistically significant correlation between duration of illness and cognitive flexibility and inhibition (cognitive flexibility and inhibition decrease as the duration of illness increases). There is a negative statistically significant correlation between cognitive flexibility and frequency of seizures per year. There is a positive statistically significant correlation between all executive functions (planning, cognitive flexibility, inhibition, and verbal fluency) and the time since the last epileptic fit.

Table 3 Correlation between executive functions and clinical variables of the seizure

Table 4 shows that patients with interictal epileptic discharge in EEG have more impairment in cognitive flexibility and verbal fluency.

Table 4 Effect of interictal EEG findings on executive functions in the patient group

Table 5 shows that the mean scores of executive functions in epileptic adolescents are significantly lower than in control adolescents.

Table 5 Comparison between means of executive functions in patients and controls

Table 6 shows that the mean scores of patients’ ability to abstract social concepts (both verbal and performance tests) are significantly lower than the scores of controls.

Table 6 Comparison between means of ability to abstract social concepts in patients and controls

Table 7 shows that there is a significant positive correlation of moderate degree between planning and verbal fluency as executive functions and the ability to abstract social concepts (verbal and performance tests).

Table 7 Pearson linear correlation coefficient between each executive function and ability to abstract social concepts in epileptic adolescents

There is a significant positive correlation of weak degree between inhibition and the ability to abstract social concepts (verbal test only).

Table 8 shows that there is a significant positive correlation between planning and cognitive flexibility as executive functions and the ability to abstract social concepts (verbal and performance tests). There is a significant positive correlation between inhibition and the ability to abstract social concepts (performance test only). There is a significant positive correlation between verbal fluency and the ability to abstract social concepts (verbal test only).

Table 8 Pearson linear correlation coefficient between each executive function and the ability to abstract social concepts in the control group

Table 9 shows that deterioration in planning or verbal fluency can predict deterioration in the ability to abstract social concepts (both verbal and performance tests).

Table 9 Multiple linear regression analysis in patients

Deterioration in inhibition can predict deterioration in the ability to abstract social concepts (verbal test only).

Deterioration in cognitive flexibility is not a significant predicting factor for deterioration of the ability to abstract social concepts.

Table 10 shows that deterioration in planning or cognitive flexibility can predict deterioration in the ability to abstract social concepts (both verbal and performance tests).

Table 10 Multiple linear regression analysis in controls

Deterioration in inhibition can predict deterioration in the ability to abstract social concepts (performance test only).

Deterioration in verbal fluency can predict deterioration in the ability to abstract social concepts (verbal test only).

Discussion

Main findings of our study

Adolescents with idiopathic generalized epilepsy have more executive function impairment compared to control adolescents. Adolescents with idiopathic epilepsy have significantly more impairment in the ability to abstract social concepts (verbal and performance) compared to control adolescents. Executive function impairment is correlated with the duration of illness, frequency of seizures per year, time since last epileptic fit, and presence of interictal epileptic discharge in EEG in the patient group.

There is a strong correlation between executive functions and the ability to abstract social concepts (verbal and performance) in both epileptic and control adolescents. Impairment in executive functions can predict impairment in the ability to abstract social concepts (verbal and performance) in epileptic and control adolescents.

Comparison to previous studies

Impairment in executive functions in epilepsy patients is well studied in several studies. These studies differed in the type of epilepsy syndrome, patients’ age, or the battery utilized to assess executive functions.

Gelziniene and his colleagues in their study detected executive impairment in trail-making test, Stroop test, and verbal fluency in adolescent patients with idiopathic generalized epilepsy, similar to our findings. Fifty-nine patients and 59 normal subjects were compared in this research [16].

Lima et al. [17] compared the executive skills of adolescents as well as children with idiopathic epilepsy to those of controls using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST). According to the findings of this research, people with epilepsy exhibited worse executive function outcomes.

Modi et al. found that 30%, 18%, and 17% of adolescents suffering from epilepsy experienced impairments in working memory, planning, and cognitive flexibility, respectively [18].

In their study, Shehata and Bateh [19] assessed cognitive functions among 71 adult men with idiopathic epilepsy and 58 controls. This study found that patients with epilepsy reported highly significant impairment in all cognitive measures.

Chowdhury et al. [20] used a battery of neuropsychological tests sensitive to frontal lobe impairment to investigate 36 patients with idiopathic generalized epilepsy (IGE), 38 first-degree relatives, and 40 healthy controls. They also found that patients with IGE have deficiencies in working memory, attention, verbal generativity, and nonverbal reasoning. According to Tian et al. [21], children with IGE have a severe deficiency in their total response time as well as executive control network. They did not, however, reveal any deficits in their orienting or alerting networks. These findings imply that IGE substantially impacts the executive control network.

Motamedi et al. [22] assessed neuropsychological functions such as backward and forward digit spans, orientation, logical memory, visual memory, mental control, associative learning, Stroop test, preservative errors, and IQ score. They illustrated that generalized epilepsy is linked to dysfunction in particular cognitive skills, especially in the prefrontal, frontal, and memory domains.

Koganti and his colleagues found problems in planning (Tower of London), verbal fluency (verbal n-back, category fluency, and word fluency), and inhibition (Stroop test) in 75 idiopathic generalized epilepsy adults [23].

Kumar and colleagues found that 54% of adults with idiopathic generalized epilepsy have impairment of executive functions [24].

Sherif and colleagues found impaired executive functions (planning, cognitive flexibility, and working memory) in adults with generalized epilepsy [25].

Consistent with our findings, Turaga et al., Black et al., and Kim et al. all found that executive functions are impaired in epileptic patients [26,27,28]. These studies, however, are not similar to ours. Turaga et al. studied chronic mesial temporal lobe epilepsy, Black et al. studied temporal lobe epilepsy, and Kim et al. studied the cognitive effects of specific antiepileptic medication.

The ability to abstract social concepts is considered one of the domains of social cognition. Previous studies that assessed social cognition in cases with epilepsy stressed two primary abilities: theory of mind (ToM) as well as facial emotion recognition. Operto et al. compared 62 children and adolescents aged between 7 and 16 years with 32 age- and sex-matched controls regarding executive functions and social cognition (theory of mind as well as facial emotion recognition). They found that epileptic patients have significantly more defects in social cognitive tests (theory of mind and facial emotion recognition) and that deficits in social cognition correlate to deficits in executive functions [29].

Multiple previous studies found that epileptic cases experience more impairment in some advanced as well as primary elements of social cognition like theory of mind and facial emotion recognition than their peers [30, 31]. Seizures may disrupt the functioning of temporal and frontal areas, which are thought to be important in facial and ToM emotion perception [32].

In our study, we found that the duration of illness and frequency of seizures per year have an impact on executive functions in epileptic adolescents; these results are in concordance with the findings of Holmes [33] and the findings of Morad et al. [34].

In our study, we found that the time since the last epileptic fit before the application of cognitive tests has an impact on all executive functions studied; this is in line with the findings of Agah et al. [35] who found that the occurrence of a seizure within a week before application of cognitive tests affected the performance of the patients on these tests.

In our study, we found that epileptic adolescents with interictal discharges in EEG have more impairment in cognitive flexibility and verbal fluency, which is in line with the findings of Lenck-Santini and Scott [36] and findings of Aldenkamp and Arends [37] who found that the presence of interictal discharges in EEG is correlated with cognitive impairment in epileptic patients. Theoretically, harm could come in two ways: (1) interictal discharges (IDs) could alter ongoing cognitive processes (this will be referred to as “transient cognitive impairment” [TCI]), and (2) IDs could lead to long-term structural changes. Therefore, if IDs do alter neural substrates in the form of molecular changes, cell death, or sprouting of axonal projections, it is also possible that, in the long term, these changes alter the network functions involved in cognitive processing (Lenck-Santini and Scott).

Limitations

The number of participants is relatively small, so the results obtained cannot be generalized. This study is a cross-sectional one, so we did not conclude cause-effect relationship (we are not certain if some of these cognitive defects were present before illness or not). The control group was collected from multiple schools in the Sohag Governorate which may affect the accuracy of the results.

Recommendation

The study recommended the necessity of developing protective, guiding, treatment, and rehabilitation programmes for generalized epilepsy patients and the people surrounding them. These programmes should consider all aspects of the patient’s life: the psychological, cognitive, physical, and social ones.

Conclusions

Executive functions and the ability to abstract social concepts are significantly impaired in adolescents suffering from idiopathic generalized epilepsy. Impairment in executive functions can predict impairment in the ability to abstract social concepts. There is a need for screening of executive functions and social abilities of adolescents with epilepsy and programmes to enhance these abilities.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed in this work are available after a reasonable request from the corresponding author.

Abbreviations

C.T.:

Computed tomography

DLPFC:

Dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex

EEG:

Electroencephalography

IDs:

Interictal discharges

I.Q:

Intelligence quotient

IGE:

Idiopathic generalized epilepsy

MRI:

Magnetic resonance imaging

SC:

Social cognition

ToM:

Theory of mind

WCST:

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

References

  1. Granbichler CA, Zimmermann G, Oberaigner W, Kuchukhidze G, Ndayisaba JP, Taylor A et al (2017) Potential years lost and life expectancy in adults with newly diagnosed epilepsy. Epilepsia 58(11):1939–1945. https://doi.org/10.1111/epi.13902

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. (1989) Proposal for revised classification of epilepsies and epileptic syndromes. Commission on Classification and Terminology of the International League Against Epilepsy. Epilepsia 30(4):389-399. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1528-1157.1989.tb05316.x

  3. Lezak MD, Howieson DB (2004) and Loring DW (2004) Neuropsychological assessment, 4th edn. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  4. Lieberman M (2007) Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core process. Annu Rev Psychol 58(259):289

    Google Scholar 

  5. Saxe R (2006) Uniquely human social cognition. Curr Opin Neurobiol 16(2):235–239

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Green MF, Penn DL, Bentall R, Carpenter WT, Gaebel W, Gur RC et al (2008) Social cognition in schizophrenia: an NIMH workshop on definitions, assessment, and research opportunities. Schizophr Bull 34(6):1211–1220. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbm145

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Green MF, Horan WP, Lee J (2015) Social cognition in schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci 16(10):620–631. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn4005

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Helmstaedter C, Witt JA (2012) Clinical neuropsychology in epilepsy: theoretical and practical issues. Handb Clin Neurol 107:437–459

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Roid GH (2003) Stanford-Binet intelligence scales, fifth edition, technical manual. Riverside Publishing, Rolling Meadows

    Google Scholar 

  10. Shallice T (1982) Specific impairments of planning. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci 298(1089):199–209

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Regard M, Strauss E, Knapp P (1982) Children’s production on verbal and non-verbal fluency tasks. Percept Mot Skills 55(3, Pt. 1):839–844

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Khalil MS (2010) Preliminary Arabic normative data of neuropsychological tests: the verbal and design fluency'. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 32(9):1028–1035

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Stroop JR (1935) Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 18(6):643–662. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054651

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Al-Ghatani A, Obonsawin M, Al-Moutaery K (2010) The Arabic version of the Stroop Test and its equivalency to the English version. Pan Arab J Neurosurg 14:112–115

    Google Scholar 

  15. Al-Sabwa MN (1992) Ability to abstract social concepts in chronic schizophrenia patients. J Psychol Stud 4(2):577–617

    Google Scholar 

  16. Gelziniene G, Jurkevicienė G, Marmiene V et al (2011) Executive functions in adolescents with idiopathic generalized epilepsy. Medicina (Kaunas) 47(6):313–319

    Google Scholar 

  17. Lima AB, Moreira F, Gomes Mda M, Maia-Filho H (2014) Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of executive function in a sample of children and adolescents with idiopathic epilepsy. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 72(12):954–959

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Modi AC, Vannest J, Combs A, Turnier L, Wade SL (2018) Pattern of executive functioning in adolescents with epilepsy: a multimethod measurement approach. Epilepsy Behav 80:5–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2017.12.021

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Shehata GA, Bateh AM (2009) Cognitive function, mood, behavioural aspects, and personality traits of adult males with idiopathic epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav 14:121–124

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Chowdhury FA, Elwes RD, Koutroumanidis M, Morris RG, Nashef L, Richardson MP (2014) Impaired cognitive function in idiopathic generalized epilepsy and unaffected family members: an epilepsy endophenotype. Epilepsia 55(6):835–840

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Tian Y, Dong B, Ma J, Zhou S, Zhou N, Wang K (2010) Attention networks in children with idiopathic generalized epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav 19:513–517

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Motamedi M, Nasergivehch S, Karamghadiri N, Noroozian M (2014) Juvenile myoclonic epilepsy (JME): neuropsychological profile and related factors with cognitive dysfunction. Iran J Psychiatry 9:14–19

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Koganti H, Paneyala S, Sundaramurthy H, Sc N, Kashyap RS, Joshi S et al (2020) The impact of idiopathic generalized epilepsy on executive functions. Ann Neurosci 27(3-4):131–135. https://doi.org/10.1177/0972753120968751 Epub 18 Feb 2021. PMID: 34556951; PMCID: PMC8454999

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Kumar KS, Narisetty V, Praveen C (2018) Executive dysfunction in patients with idiopathic epilepsy. IAIM 5(6):27–37

    Google Scholar 

  25. Sherif HS, Abulmakarim F, Kishk NA (2021) Executive functions in an Egyptian sample of adults with generalized epilepsy. Am J Appl Psychol 10(1):10–15. https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ajap.20211001.12

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Turaga SP, Motturi P, Pakeeraiah et al (2016) Frontal lobe dysfunction in chronic mesial temporal lobe epilepsies. Austin J Neurol Disord Epilepsy 3(3):1026

    Google Scholar 

  27. Black LC, Schefft BK, Howe SR et al (2010) The effect of seizures on working memory and executive functioning performance. Epilepsy Behav 17(3):412–419

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Kim SY, Lee HW, Jung DK et al (2006) Cognitive effects of low dose topiramate compared with oxcarbazepine in epilepsy patients. J Clin Neurol 2:126–133

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Operto FF, Pastorino GMG, Mazza R, Di Bonaventura C, Marotta R, Pastorino N et al (2020) Social cognition and executive functions in children and adolescents with focal epilepsy. Eur J Paediatr Neurol 28:167–175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpn.2020.06.019 Epub 13 Jul 2020. PMID: 32718867

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Stewart E, Catroppa C, Lah S (2016) Theory of mind in patients with epilepsy: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychol Rev 26(1):3–24. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-015-9313-x Epub 21 Jan 2016. PMID: 26797753

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Edwards M, Stewart E, Palermo R, Lah S (2017) Facial emotion perception in patients with epilepsy: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 83:212–225. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.10.013 Epub 16 Oct 2017. PMID: 29045812

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Gomez-Ibanez A, Urrestarazu E, Viteri C (2014) Recognition of facial emotions and identity in patients with mesial temporal lobe and idiopathic generalized epilepsy: an eyetracking study. Seizure 23(10):892–898. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.seizure.2014.08.012

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Holmes GL (2015) Cognitive impairment in epilepsy: the role of network abnormalities. Epileptic Disord 17(2):101–116. https://doi.org/10.1684/epd.2015.0739

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Morad OA, Abdellatif MG, Arafa AG, Amin AE (2020) Cognitive implications in epilepsy. Sohag Med J 24(1):133-139

  35. Agah E, Asgari-Rad N, Ahmadi M, Tafakhori A, Aghamollaii V (2017) Evaluating executive function in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy using the frontal assessment battery. Epilepsy Res 133:22–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2017.03.011 Epub 5Apr 2017. PMID: 28407518

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Lenck-Santini PP, Scott RC (2015) Mechanisms responsible for cognitive impairment in epilepsy. Cold Spring Harbor Perspect Med 5(10):a022772

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Aldenkamp A, Arends J (2004) The relative influence of epileptic EEG discharges, short nonconvulsive seizures, and type of epilepsy on cognitive function. Epilepsia 45:54–63

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank everyone who took part in this research for their time and effort.

Funding

This study has received no funding.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

S.H.: developing the study topic, administering surveys, doing statistical analyses, drafting the manuscript, revising references, and submitting the paper. H.K.I.: diagnosis and evaluation of epilepsy in the patients, interpretation of neurological investigations, and writing of the manuscript. T.D.: research concept selection, application of tests and psychometric tests, article writing, and statistical analysis. S.M.S.: idea of research and application of the psychometric tests. T. A. S.: conducting interviews, administering questionnaires, doing statistical analyses, and drafting and rewriting the article. The paper has been reviewed and approved by all authors.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Saber Hadad.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The present research protocol was approved by the Faculty of Medicine’s Medical Research Ethics Committee, Sohag University. Informed consent was obtained from parents and research participants (both controls as well as patients).

Consent for publication

The authorization to publish was obtained.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hadad, S., Ibrahim, H.K., Desoky, T. et al. The ability of executive functions to predict deterioration in social abstraction in adolescents with idiopathic generalized epilepsy. Middle East Curr Psychiatry 29, 58 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s43045-022-00223-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s43045-022-00223-y

Keywords