Skip to main content
  • Systematic Map
  • Open access
  • Published:

Existing evidence of conceptual differences in research on climate change perceptions among smallholders? A systematic map

Abstract

Background

Climate change is having adverse effects on the livelihoods of small-scale populations, particularly in relation to their subsistence practices. Scientific literature widely acknowledges that smallholders must first perceive climate changes to take necessary precautions and adapt to the new conditions. However, variations exist in the terminology used across the literature, and in how it conceptualizes these perceptions. This variation complicates understanding of the literature and hinders empirical evidence comparisons. Therefore, in this review, we systematically mapped the literature considering variations in the concept's usage across different thematic areas. Our goal was to provide a comprehensive overview of the current state of the literature on smallholder climate change perceptions.

Methods

In our systematic map, we adhered to the Collaboration for Environmental Evidence guidelines. We searched the literature adopting English terms and using five electronic databases of scientific publications (Web of Science Core Collection, Scopus, BASE–Bielefeld Academic Search Engine, PubMed, and Science Direct Elsevier). We then screened the retrieved articles' titles, abstracts, and full texts according to predefined eligibility criteria. Articles meeting the eligibility criteria were chosen for full reading, data extraction, and coding, utilizing a prepared codebook. No validity appraisal occurred in this selection. A database containing coded metadata for all studies is accessible for reference.

Review findings

After screening 5358 articles (titles and abstracts), we identified and thoroughly reviewed 361 eligible articles at full text to map the usage of the climate change perception concept. Among these, 73 articles provided explicit definitions of perception, falling into seven categories: risk perception, perception based on psychological constructs and sensory stimuli, awareness, prior experience, observation of climate variables, beliefs, and uncertainties or threats. Implicit definitions of perception with various constructs were found, including those rooted in Cognitive Psychology, awareness, risk perception, traditional knowledge, beliefs, concerns about climate change, experiences of exposure to its effects, attitudes, worldviews, and scientific knowledge. Articles usually address multiple topics. Notably, 88% of the articles did not present any theory throughout their content. Geographically, Africa and Asia were the most frequently studied continents, with more focus on non-indigenous small-scale populations than indigenous ones.

Conclusions

In conclusion, the perception concept exhibits an interdisciplinary nature. Therefore, fostering continuous dialogue among diverse disciplines is imperative to establishing an interdisciplinary definition of the term. An in-depth understanding of the perception concept is essential, as its absence can result in erroneous conclusions, limited adaptation strategies, and a lack of awareness among small-scale populations regarding climate change impacts. Misconceptions about this concept can lead to ineffective policies, further endangering vulnerable populations. Defining the concept and its constructs facilitates article comparisons. Without this definition, meaningful comparisons become unfeasible. Moreover, the absence of proper perception definitions poses challenges for small-scale populations, researchers, and stakeholders in developing effective, efficient, and flexible adaptations over time. Perception is the first step in incorporating adaptation strategies and must be translated into policies to address climate change impacts efficiently.

Background

Human-induced climate change has impacted every part of the globe, with human activities leading to warming the Earth's soils, oceans, and atmosphere over the past two millennia. In addition to rising temperatures, since the 1950s, the likelihood of extreme events like heatwaves, severe droughts, heavy rainfall, flooding, and tropical cyclones has increased [1]. Looking ahead, the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC) anticipates that global warming will unavoidably surpass the 1.5 to 2 °C threshold in the twenty-first century unless there are substantial reductions in carbon dioxide and other greenhouse gas emissions in the coming decades [1, 2]. These changes in climate pose risks to various aspects of human life, including food security, health, access to water resources, and overall economic activities. As a result, human livelihoods, especially those of vulnerable populations in developing nations, are under threat [1]. Projections suggest that by 2030, climate change could push approximately 132 million people into poverty [3].

The livelihoods of small-scale rural communities, including indigenous groups, are already experiencing detrimental effects due to global warming [4]. This is primarily attributed to their heavy dependence on natural resources as a means of sustaining their way of life [5]. The impacts of climate change profoundly affect the lives and culture of small-scale populations. Notably, the increased frequency of extreme weather events, such as prolonged floods and droughts, has adversely impacted the agriculture of diverse small-scale populations worldwide [6,7,8,9], resulting in food insecurity [10, 11] and heightened vulnerability to diseases [12]. This, in turn, adversely affects both the physical and mental health of these populations [13, 14]. Furthermore, shifts in water temperature, water levels, water color, or turbidity have impacted fishing, compromising the livelihoods of populations dependent on this means of subsistence [15, 16]. In terms of hunting, rising temperatures, changes in rainfall patterns, and shifts in ecosystems have resulted in the scarcity of animals in specific regions, negatively impacting hunting-dependent communities [16, 17]. In addition to immediate impacts, climate change has prompted population migration due to the potential deterioration of living conditions for these communities [18, 19].

On the other side, small-scale populations have a long-term connection with their natural environments, which may allow them to detect, understand, and act to mitigate the adverse effects of environmental changes [20]. Detailed traditional knowledge, which developed from smallholders’ close interaction with the environment, is particularly important in this endeavor [21].

Nonetheless, to achieve these goals, small-scale communities must initially become aware of shifts in the climate. The ability to perceive these changes is crucial because small-scale populations are more inclined to take preventive measures, respond to ongoing negative effects, and capitalize on potential advantages when aware of these alterations [22, 23]. This process, often referred to as adaptation strategies [24], only commences once a population acknowledges the changes and possesses information regarding their probable consequences, whether accurate or not [25].

Apart from the significance of the population's direct awareness of climate change and its consequences, it is imperative to comprehend the mechanisms underlying this process for two principal reasons. Firstly, pertinent and useful traditional knowledge in this regard may currently be overlooked in the scientific literature. Secondly, insights derived from local knowledge can provide valuable information about climate conditions in regions where meteorological data and monitoring stations are insufficient [26]. In essence, it is paramount to investigate how individuals within small communities perceive climate change and the timing and methods of their perception. Scientists have acknowledged this significance, leading to numerous studies on this subject [27, 28].

Nevertheless, many studies on climate change fail to incorporate a specific or at least a transparent definition of perception (e.g., [29,30,31,32,33,34,35]). While the term may vary in its interpretations, even within the same research domain, a shared aspect in most definitions of perception is its engagement with the examination of and individual’s sensory information. Therefore, when cognitive psychologists discuss perceptions, they typically address the fundamental cognitive procedures associated with interpreting sensory information received from individuals' senses [36]. Nevertheless, within the literature on climate change that defines “perceptions” (e.g., [37,38,39,40,41]), the conceptualizations employed are varied. They encompass not just sensory perceptions (e.g., [38, 42,43,44]), but also individuals’ subjective interpretations (e.g., [45]), awareness or the understanding of environmental experiences (e.g., [26, 34]), beliefs regarding ongoing changes (e.g., [39, 41]), and past experiences linked to climate fluctuations (e.g., [41]).

The diverse range of definitions for climate change perception adopted presents three key challenges that hinder the comparison and synthesis of the scientific literature. Firstly, numerous thematic areas encompass incorporate the concept of climate change perception. However, the precise meaning of the term’s usage remains unclear. This lack of standardization within and across areas, but also within a single study, makes it difficult to synthesize and compare results, given the presentation of different interpretations. For example, the concept is integrated into studies concerning adaptation strategies [23], traditional or local knowledge [32, 46] and its comparison with actual meteorological data [45, 47], cognitive biases [30], and the identification of factors that explain perceptions [48]. Despite that, the concept varied across and within these areas.

Secondly, although cognitive perceptions pertain to individuals, studies diverge in their choice of sample units for investigating the phenomenon, which may refer to individuals (e.g., [49,50,51,52]), households (e.g., [53,54,55,56]), individual and household (e.g., [33, 39, 57,58,59]) or even communities and large-scale populations (e.g., [60]). This factor adds another layer of confusion in interpreting and comparing results.

Thirdly, studies employ diverse investigation methods [61], including qualitative (e.g., [62,63,64,65]), quantitative (e.g., [66,67,68,69]), or mixed approaches (e.g., [70,71,72,73]). Again, these methodological differences have implications for how results are interpreted and for the definition of the perception concept itself.

Thus, this conceptual map summarizes information on various interpretations of climate change perception, enhancing our understanding of its variability across different domains and fostering the standardization of terminology in this field. Consequently, this article aims to streamline and enhance the reliability of information synthesis and comparisons within the existing empirical literature. These evaluations hold significance in formulating and implementing adaptation policies, especially within small-scale societies. In this regard, this article also strives to pinpoint areas where further assessments and syntheses on climate change perception are necessary, thereby assisting in identifying knowledge gaps for future research.

Theoretical framework on perception

The study of human perception is primarily conducted within the realm of Cognitive Psychology [74, 75], a scientific discipline that began to take shape in the 1950s but was formally recognized as a distinct field with its own methods in the 1970s [75]. Beyond the study of human perceptions, this field explores various other mental processes, including memory, language, attention, problem-solving, consciousness, emotion, reasoning [76], learning, and motivation [75].

In the realm of Cognitive Psychology, perception is defined as a series of intricate steps whereby an individual processes information derived from environmental stimuli and assigns meaning to them [75]. Perception encompasses sensory analysis [77], because it is from the sensations detected through sensory receptors (such as ears, nose, tongue, skin, and eyes) that individuals recognize, interpret, and organize information [78].

Within the domain of Cognitive Psychology, the theories explaining how human perception functions can be categorized into two groups: top–down and bottom–up theories [75].

Bottom-up theories, also known as direct perception theories, posit that stimuli and sensory information are the initial components of perception [77]. In this view, a person observes something, and the information gathered by sensory receptors is conveyed to the brain [75]. Consequently, individuals would rely on their senses (sight, hearing, smell, touch) when encountering extreme weather events and the consequences of climate change.

Top-down theories, also called constructive perception theories, propose that humans initially employ pre-existing knowledge stored in memory, already-existing expectations about what they are perceiving, cognitive processes, and, at times, prior experiences to shape their processing of sensory information [75]. Under this framework, what an individual perceives depends on his/her existing knowledge [76]. Individuals would first draw upon their existing knowledge to form an idea of climate change and its impacts, and only after observing it would they comprehend its true nature. This prior knowledge may originate from information acquired through (i) indirect exposure (such as reports from other people or access to the media), (ii) direct exposure (first-hand contact with the impacts of changes), (iii) traditional knowledge transmitted through generations, and (iv) scientific knowledge.

While these alternative theories are often presented as opposing viewpoints in the literature, they can actually complement each other because individuals are likely to employ both processes in perception [75] as they address different aspects of perception [76]. Both theories are relevant to the understanding of climate change perception, as the direct perception of sensory stimuli, combined with knowledge and prior experiences, contributes to an individual's understanding of climate change.

For perception to take place, two naturally occurring stimuli are necessary: distal and proximal. In the distal stimulus, a physical object or event is situated at some physical distance from the individual [75, 78]. The image of this object or event reaches the sensory receptors, known as the proximal stimulus. The proximal stimulus can be influenced by various factors, such as lighting conditions, color, viewing angle, alterations in shape [78], tactile information, sound waves, olfactory or gustatory stimulation, or chemical molecules, with variations depending on each receptor [75]. As a result, perception emerges when an individual elaborates on what is being sensory perceived, effectively reflecting the external world [75].

In the context of climate change (based on bottom-up theory), perception can result from the combination of various sensations and stimuli, including what an individual can visually observe from a certain physical distance. This occurs, for instance, when one observes extreme weather events such as floods, heavy rainfalls, severe droughts, wildfires, and numerous others. In addition to sight, tactile sensations produced by the event may also play a role. For example, during extreme heatwaves, the sensation of heat is intensified, while abrupt temperature drops tend to make cold sensations more pronounced. During floods, individuals can feel the water on their skin. The sound emitted by the event is another critical stimulus, including sounds like rain, noise from rising river levels, wind knocking down trees, or the crackling of a forest fire. The sense of smell also contributes to perception; for instance, individuals may perceive the smell of burning trees or emitted polluting gases. Furthermore, based on top-down theory, individuals would also use experiences with adverse climate impacts, such as encountering heatwaves, extreme droughts, or floods, to perceive climate change. In addition to this perception, pre-existing memory and knowledge are incorporated, acquired directly through experiences with changes or indirectly through various sources of information (media, individual reports, and school).

While perception enables individuals to become aware of the impacts of climate change, adapt to them, and interact with the world, it can be constrained by at least four factors.

Firstly, many climate changes unfold slowly, at least when viewed from a non-geological perspective (e.g., the slowdown of ocean circulation, the disintegration of ice sheets, permafrost thaw) [79]. Consequently, humans may not readily discern what is occurring and, as a result, underestimate these changes because they cannot visually perceive climate change as an ongoing process [80]. Contributing to this limitation is the fact that humans only perceive a limited range of the electromagnetic spectrum (visible light), can only hear frequencies between 20 and 20,000 Hz, and are unable to perceive events that occur too rapidly or too slowly [78].

A second factor to consider is that perception can be prone to inaccuracies, as physical phenomena can create optical illusions [78]. In other words, what we perceive through our sensory organs may not always align with reality [75].

Thirdly, human perception is selective because attention tends to be directed toward specific aspects while disregarding the surroundings. Consequently, our attention can be diverted when a more attention-grabbing event occurs [78]. Therefore, what is perceived through the senses is contingent on what captures our attention, is selected, and forms within the mind [75].

Lastly, various factors, such as the context [78, 81], individual and societal beliefs, cultural practices, and individual’s prior experiences, can influence one's perception of climate change [80].

The objective of the review

The objective of this systematic mapping was to identify, categorize, and provide an overview of the existing evidence found in scientific literature concerning the diverse terms and ideas used to describe how people perceive climate change.

This review has focused on the literature of the last five years (2018 to 2022) regarding small-scale rural populations, including indigenous societies. We selected small-scale rural communities because these societies directly rely on natural resources, putting them at a higher risk of experiencing the negative impacts of climate change [5]. In addition, the perception of climate change can assist them in adapting their subsistence practices [82], suffering fewer potential adverse effects.

Our primary research question was, therefore:

“What evidence exists on the alternative definitions of climate change perceptions adopted in the literature about small-scale populations of the last five years (2018 to 2022)?”

The secondary research questions were:

“How do the definitions of climate change perception vary and are interpreted across articles, according to their thematic areas, populations of interest, and geographical origin?

What constructs differentiate one definition from another? Are there similarities?”

Constructs are theoretical concepts that help test hypotheses based on observable phenomena, defined as “a complex idea or concept formed from a synthesis of simpler ideas” [83].

The components of the primary question were as follows:

Population (P): Small communities inhabiting remote rural regions, composed of individuals who sustain their livelihoods through family labor and have limited to no capacity to generate excess production for commercial purposes [84].

Exposure (E): Climate change, including extreme events such as floods, heatwaves, intense droughts, heavy rainfalls, tropical cyclones, among others.

Outcome (O): Implicit of explicit definitions of climate change perceptions.

Methods

This map was conducted according to the published protocol [85]. The map follows the Collaboration for Environmental Evidence (CEE) Guidelines and Standards for Evidence Synthesis [86], and conforms to the Reporting Standards for Systematic Evidence (ROSES) [87] (see Additional file 1).

Deviations from the protocol

In this article, we deviated from our published protocol [85] as regards the time frame due to limited resources. Although the original protocol did not specify the review period, we confined it to a five-year timeframe (2018 to 2022). The review team (hereafter referred as team) acknowledges that this deviation may have introduced limitations to the results, as discussed toward the end of this article.

Search for articles

Searches were conducted in five electronic scientific databases of publications: “Web of Science Core Collection” (WoS)”, “Scopus”, “BASE–Bielefeld Academic Search Engine”, “PubMed” and “Science Direct Elsevier”, between March 2022 and February 2023, seeking articles between 2018 and 2022. We selected these databases because they are extensive, cover a wide range of disciplines, and include peer-reviewed publications. We assumed that they would include the majority of publications related to climate change perceptions within the Environmental Sciences field. Moreover, all five chosen databases have established procedures to ensure the quality of published research.

Search terms and language

The search query for this review, which involved the use of Boolean operators (AND, OR) to combine key terms, consisted of English terms that fell into three distinct conceptual groups: (i) perception or awareness; (ii) climate change or global warming; and (iii) smallholders, including indigenous population. We employed an asterisk as a special character to account for variations in word endings and plurals, except when using BASE, Science Direct, and PubMed, as these platforms do not support this procedure. Only documents in English were reviewed because of (i) familiarity with this idiom and (ii) English is the universal scientific language (for more information, see protocol [83].

The team searched five databases by looking for the chosen terms within the article's title, abstract, or keywords. The final search strings are described below.

  • Scopus: (TITLE-ABS-KEY ((("perception*" OR "local perspective*" OR "awareness") AND ("climat* chang*" OR "global warming" OR "chang* climat*" OR "climat* variabilit*" OR "climat* event*") AND ("indigenous*" OR "smallholder*" OR "small scale*" OR "livelihood*" OR "fisher*" OR "peasant*" OR "hunter*" OR "agricultur*" OR "forager*" OR "agropastoralist*" OR "horticultur*" OR "pastoralist*" OR “herder*” OR “small-island*”))))

  • WoS: ALL FIELDS (("perception*" OR "local perspective*" OR "awareness") AND ("climat* chang*" OR "global warming" OR "chang* climat*" OR "climat* variabilit*" OR "climat* event*") AND ("indigenous*" OR "smallholder*" OR "small scale*" OR "livelihood*" OR "fisher*" OR "peasant*" OR "hunter*" OR "agricultur*" OR "forager*" OR "agropastoralist*" OR "horticultur*" OR "pastoralist*" OR “herder*” OR “small-island*”))

  • BASE: Entire document: ("perception" OR "awareness") AND ("climate change" OR "global warming") AND ("indigenous" OR "smallholder" OR “small-island”)

  • Science Direct: Title, abstract, keywords: (("perception" OR "awareness") AND ("climate change" OR "global warming") AND ("indigenous" OR "smallholder" OR “small-island”))

  • PubMed: (("perception" OR "awareness") AND ("climate change" OR "global warming") AND ("indigenous" OR "smallholder" OR “small-island”))

Search sources and results

The team accessed the five databases using the University of São Paulo's institutional subscription via a Virtual Private Network (VPN) in Brazil. In Scopus and WoS, we applied two filters to the search fields: (i) we looked specifically for articles in the document type, and (ii) we narrowed it down to articles written in English. In BASE, we conducted a basic search, filtered the results by articles written in English, and categorized them as article contributions in the document type. We opted for an advanced search for Science Direct, filtering by title, abstract, or author-specified keywords, and entered the search string. In PubMed, we did not apply any filters. Our selection criteria were limited to articles that presented primary data. Books and book chapters were also omitted from the study because we could not ensure access to them. In summary, the following types of articles were also excluded: (i) documents in languages other than English and (ii) review articles, books, book chapters, conference papers, proceedings papers, conference reviews, editorials, letters, and data papers.

Prior to the screening process, we identified and removed duplicate articles retrieved from all five databases using an Excel® spreadsheet. Team members did not participate in decisions regarding inclusion and evaluation when an article was authored by any of them. Only one article met this criterion, corresponding to the published protocol, which was excluded from the database [85]. Thus, no team member was excluded from participating in any coding decisions.

The review team did not pursue further endeavors to acquire literature (i) in other languages and (ii) from non-academic (grey) literature, and (iii) consultations with experts or other stakeholders. Although this is a limitation of our map that may be advanced in further studies, our time and resources were restricted.

Estimating the comprehensiveness of the search

As described in our protocol [85], the comprehensiveness of the search was assessed using a list of 95 benchmark articles compiled by the team members. The final search string applied to the three databases yielded 94 articles (representing 99% of the pre-defined benchmark articles), leading us to conclude that the search strategy's comprehensiveness was satisfactory.

Article screening and study eligibility criteria

Screening process

We conducted a two-step screening process to evaluate the eligibility of the articles: first by examining the title and abstract, and then by reviewing the full text. Two separate reviewers independently conducted these screenings using an Excel® spreadsheet, and their assessment outcomes were compared for consistency. To ensure consistency, we conducted a calibration exercise (details below) during the screening process. Articles that met the eligibility criteria based on the title and abstract proceeded to the second stage of screening. We conducted a full reading of articles with only a title but without an abstract to determine whether they should be included in the second screening stage. The second stage involved thoroughly reading articles that satisfied the eligibility criteria from the initial screening stage. Any articles that did not meet the criteria at this stage were also excluded. The team held weekly meetings throughout both stages to address any evaluation discrepancies.

To ensure the uniformity and accuracy of inclusion decisions during the screening process, two reviewers assessed consistency by randomly selecting 5% of the total article sample for screening, which included (i) titles and abstracts (n = 175) and (ii) full-text articles (n = 21). Within this sample, the team calculated the consistency rate between reviewers considering the list of articles that met the inclusion criteria for both stages 1 and 2. The results of this consistency check were compared between the reviewers, and any disagreements were thoroughly discussed until the consistency level reached a minimum of 80%. Ultimately, the consistency levels achieved for stages 1 and 2 were 90%.

Eligibility criteria

The following criteria were met for the inclusion of each of the articles in this review in (1) title and abstract, and (2) full text:

  • Population: We incorporated articles exclusively focused on small-scale populations, including indigenous communities residing in rural areas. Consequently, we excluded articles that: (i) addressed extensive rural properties, such as those engaged in commercial monoculture targeted at commodity markets or agribusiness; (ii) small-scale populations that moved to urban areas, and (iii) those cases in which there was no specification of the target population. Eligible small-scale populations included subsistence farmers or horticulturists, fishers operating small to medium-sized wooden boats, pastoralists, herders or family agropastoralists, and hunter-gatherers or foragers.

  • Outcomes: The article tackled the notion of climate change perception, regardless of whether it had been defined explicitly or left to implicit interpretation.

  • Study Design Types: we considered empirical studies that relied on primary data and employed a combination of quantitative and/or qualitative data collection methods.

Study validity assessment

Evaluating the validity and quality of studies is not mandatory in systematic mapping, as per established guidelines [88]. Due to the descriptive nature of this mapping, assessing causal relationships or generalizing findings to certain contexts was deemed unnecessary.

No formal study validity assessment was conducted since the interest and purpose was to describe the variability in usage of the perception concept in the published literature, as a way to obtain an overview of the conceptual similarities and differences in the climate change literature. Therefore, there was no restriction on the choice of eligible articles regarding quality or aspects related to internal or external validity.

Data coding strategy

To ensure consistency between coders, two team members independently coded a sample comprising 5% of the total number of articles (n = 21). These articles were selected based on their chronological order in the output list, starting with the most recent dates at the top. After coding this sample, the team discussed any discrepancies or uncertainties. In cases of disagreements, a third team member was consulted to reach a resolution.

All eligible articles underwent a dual screening throughout the coding process to ensure consistency. Weekly team meetings were scheduled to tackle any issues and synchronize efforts among reviewers, ensuring consistent coding practices.

During the full-text screening, as previously explained, the team carried out data extraction and coding for the articles that fulfilled the inclusion criteria. For data extraction and coding, the team adhered to a codebook that had been prepared before the mapping process (Additional file 2). The team adopted the following format for data extraction and coding for articles that met the eligibility criteria for full review.

  • Bibliographic information: Title, authors, journal, year, DOI.

  • Study location: Country.

  • Stated origin of the researched population: All non-indigenous, all indigenous, mixed indigenous and non-indigenous, non-specified.

  • Subsistence strategy of the investigated population: Forager or hunter-gatherer or fisher-gatherer, horticulturalist, small-scale agriculturalist, small-scale agropastoralist, pastoralist or herder, other.

  • Topics addressed in the article: Adaptation, awareness, comparison between individual perceptions with scientific data, traditional knowledge with and without indicators, scientific knowledge, observed changes in the environment, observed changes in livelihood activities, perception determinants or drivers, risk perception, climate change mitigation, resilience, another thematic area.

  • Presence or absence of a clearly stated definition of perception: Explicit, implicit, other.

  • Section of the text where the definition of perception is found: introduction, methods, results, discussion, conclusion, or elsewhere.

  • Explanation of the authors' explicit definition of climate change perception: As provided in the article.

  • Identification of the component(s) (“constructs”) included in the definition(s) of climate change perception used, indicating whether perception is assessed in terms of environmental observations, sensory experiences, people's belief in climate change, traditional knowledge, attitudes, and similar aspects.

  • Description of how the article presents the perception concept within its results, discussion, or conclusion, irrespective of whether a formal definition of perception was provided. This may include, for instance: Portraying perception as observations of changes, either traditional or scientific knowledge, or an individual's level of concern regarding climate change's impact on livelihood activities, among other aspects (see Additional file 2 for more details).

  • Detailing how perception is depicted in the results: Following the authors' description (if explicitly provided).

  • Explanation of the phenomenon under investigation in the article: whether it pertains to the physical, biological, human, or another type of phenomenon.

  • Explanation of the unit from which data were collected in the methodology: individual, household, both individual and household, community or village, organizations (e.g., associations, NGOs), and any other relevant categories.

  • Explanation of the type of data employed in the article for the analysis: primary, secondary, a combination of primary and secondary, and any other applicable categories.

  • Identification of the theories referenced in the article as outlined by the authors (when explicitly presented).

After completing the full-text article screening, we compiled an Excel® spreadsheet database to document all the studies (Additional file 3). The team collected data exclusively from the main text and did not engage in subsequent communication with the authors to retrieve the missing information.

Data mapping method

The team compiled and presented the number of articles identified at each stage of this review using a ROSES flow diagram. The information that was extracted was organized into a database in an Excel® spreadsheet format. All explicitly identified definitions of climate change perception have been incorporated in this and are also available in the Additional file 4.

A summary outlining the various constructs and theories presented in the literature has been provided in additional files as described in the results section. We have synthesized the evidence base using bar plots, with a focus on (i) year of publication; (ii) characteristics of the studied population (population’s origin; subsistence activities practiced by the population under study); (iii) definitions of perception; (iv) methodological approaches, and (v) thematic areas. Information about the geographic distribution of the evidence has been gathered and displayed on a map highlighting the countries involved.

Review findings

Review descriptive statistics

In total, 5358 scientific articles were initially identified in the five databases under investigation, and after removing 1853 duplicate articles, there remained a total of in 3,505 articles covering the period between 2018 and 2022. Following the screening of titles and abstracts, 422 articles were initially selected for potential full-text examination. Out of these 422 articles, 30 were inaccessible (due to subscription requirements), and 31 did not meet the eligibility criteria. As a result, a total of 361 articles were included for data extraction during the full-text reading phase. Figure 1 provides a quantitative overview of the search and screening process employed to identify the included articles. For the complete reference list of selected articles, including those that were inaccessible or not selected for inclusion, please refer to Additional file 5. Additionally, for the complete list of articles that were excluded during the title and abstract screening phase along with the reasons for their exclusion, please consult Additional file 6. All data extracted from the articles are provided in Additional file 7.

Fig. 1
figure 1

ROSES flow diagram illustrating the literature search and screening process. Source: [89]

Year of publication and geographical location of the fields of study

During the examined period (2018 to 2022), the years that saw the highest number of article publications were 2021 (constituting 27% of total; n = 99), and 2022 (representing 24%; n = 87) (Fig. 2a). This suggests a notable upward trend in the number of publications, indicating a recent surge of in research interest regarding how small-scale populations perceive climate change.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Characteristics of the 361 articles included in the mapping

Out of the 361 articles, nearly all (97%, n = 353) focused on a single country, while only five (1.38%) publications encompassed two countries, and three (0.83%) covered three. Continents were represented unevenly, with a substantial proportion of the articles examining small-scale populations in Africa and Asia (Fig. 2b), accounting for 54% and 35%, respectively. Within the African continent, the most frequently studied countries with small-scale populations were Ethiopia (n = 47), Ghana (n = 25), Kenya (n = 24), Nigeria (n = 17), South Africa (n = 14), and Tanzania (n = 12). In the Asian context, this list includes India (n = 36), Nepal (n = 29), Bangladesh (n = 14), Vietnam (n = 11), Pakistan (n = 7), and China (n = 7) (Fig. 2c).

At least two factors may account for the heightened research focus on these countries.

Researchers have dedicated substantial attention to these two continents, as evidenced by their inclusion in 54% and 35% of the 361 articles. This extensive coverage can be attributed to the fact that the populations in these regions are among the world’s poorest and rely heavily on weather conditions for their subsistence activities, rendering them highly susceptible to the impacts of climate change [3, 90]. Both continents are predicted to experience severe changes, including temperature anomalies, rising sea levels, ice sheet melting, and flooding [91]. For instance, Mozambique, Bangladesh, Pakistan, and Nepal were among the top ten countries most affected by extreme weather events between 2000 and 2019 [92]. In Africa, there has already been a temperature increase of by 0.3ºC per decade between 1991 and 2021, resulting in numerous countries grappling with extreme droughts, exemplified by Ethiopia and Kenya [91, 93, 94]. These events may have contributed to a 34% reduction in agricultural productivity in Africa since 1961 [93].

Secondly, the high number of articles may also be indicative of a publication bias, as certain countries have research groups and/or partnerships with foreign institutions that specialize in climate change investigations.

Less studied continents and regions with small-scale populations include: Europa, with RussiaFootnote 1 (n = 2), Finland, Sweden, and Bosnia-Herzegovina (all with n = 1); Oceania, with Samoa (n = 2), Vanuatu, Papua New Guinea, Australia, Solomon Islands, and Tonga (all with n = 1); and Central America, with Haiti, Jamaica, and San Vincent and the Grenadines (all with n = 1). In South America, the most researched countries were Brazil (n = 8), Ecuador (n = 5), and Colombia (n = 5); and, in North America, Mexico (n = 5), United States (n = 3), and Canada (n = 2) (Fig. 2c).

Based on the evidence presented, some countries with small-scale populations were not studied. For example, it was expected to find studies in Nicaragua, Costa Rica, Panama, and Guatemala. There are seven indigenous peoples living in Nicaragua and eight in Costa Rica, while 12% of Panama's population and 44% of Guatemala's are autochthonous [4]. Furthermore, we should anticipate a greater volume of research in certain countries, such as Brazil, because the country is home to 305 identified indigenous peoples. The same applies to Bolivia, where 48% of the population is indigenous, albeit many reside in urban areas, and Peru, where four million indigenous people live in varied conditions [4].

There are four plausible reasons to explain the absence of these studies in our sample. The first reason is the challenging research environment that emerged between 2019 and 2022 due to the covid-19 pandemic [95, 96]. Second, certain areas inhabited by indigenous peoples and traditional populations can be challenging to access (e.g., the Wayana indigenous people in French Guyana and Suriname, the Tiriyó people in Suriname [97, 98], as well as populations in Greenland [4]). Third, researchers might avoid specific locations due to risks associated with illicit activities, such as mining near Indigenous Lands (e.g., Guyana) [4], or war and violent conflicts, such as in South Sudan [99], Israel and Palestine [4], Syria, Iraq, Ukraine, Somalia, Chad, Lybia, Yemen, and Saudi Arabia [100]. Fourth, some European countries with small-scale populations did not yield articles (Fig. 2c). We speculate this is possibly due to the lower scientific “appeal” of studying traditional European small-scale populations compared to those in developing countries.

General characteristics of the investigated populations

Out of the 361 articles, a significant majority (70%, n = 252) focused on non-indigenous small-scale populations residing in rural areas (non-urban, e.g., smallholder, farmer, small-scale fisher/hunter). Approximately one-third (27%, n = 99) addressed indigenous people living in rural areas, referred to in the articles as indigenous people, native people, natives, aboriginal, autochthonous, or ethnic. Only 3% (n = 10) of the articles addressed both indigenous and non-indigenous populations. With exception of the European and American continents, the data indicated that non-indigenous populations were more frequently studied than indigenous ones (Fig. 3a).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Characteristics of investigated populations: origin and subsistence strategy

The most common subsistence strategies in the studied populations (whether indigenous, non-indigenous, or both) were agropastoralism (a combination of agriculture and pastoralism; 38.5%, n = 139), followed by small-scale agriculture (31%, n = 113), horticulture (16%, n = 58), and pastoralism (7%, n = 27). The least investigated activity was foraging (1%, n = 3) (Fig. 3b), as exemplified by the Twa people, an indigenous group of hunter-gatherers the Democratic Republic of Congo [64]; by the Inuit in Canada [101], and five other ethnicities in the United States (Quinault, Salish Kootenai, Siletz, Shoshone, Shivwits Band of Paiutes) [102].

When focusing on indigenous populations, the same pattern emerged, with the exception of horticulture, which was more commonly practiced (7%, n = 27) than small-scale agriculture (4%, n = 16), consistent with the traditional activities of indigenous communities worldwide [4]. Among non-indigenous populations, small-scale agriculture (26%, n = 93) was less common than agropastoralism (27%, n = 98). These findings align with expectations, as agriculture and pastoralism are relied upon by 78% of the world’s poorest population, who reside in rural areas [103] (Fig. 3c).

Thematic areas

All 361 articles on climate change perception covered multiple topics. As anticipated, 97% of the articles (n = 349) delved into environmental changes (e.g., increase in drought) and their resulting impacts. Similarly, 94% of the articles explored changes in the livelihoods of small-scale communities, specifically their ability to satisfy basic survival needs such as water, food, shelter, and clothing (n = 340). The majority (74% of the articles; n = 267) also examined adaptation to these changes. The frequently addressed themes included gender issues (2%, n = 7), health and food security (3%, n = 10), vulnerability (7%, n = 26), mitigation of the effects of changes (12%, n = 44), and risk perception (13%, n = 46). For a comprehensive table with reference examples for the thematic areas, please refer to Additional file 8.

The topic of traditional knowledge was slightly more frequently addressed among non-indigenous populations (n = 56, 50%) than among indigenous populations (n = 53 or 47%) (Fig. 4). Of these (n = 112), 39% (44 articles) focused on physical and/or biological indicators derived from environmental observations. For instance, studies among indigenous peoples (e.g., in Bolivia [104], South Africa [105], Namibia [106], and Vietnam [107]), and among non-indigenous people, for example, in Zimbabwe [108], Mexico [109], and Nigeria [110] presented traditional knowledge related to various climatological predictors. These included precipitation, storms, droughts, unusually hot or cold years, and floods, assessed through atmospheric (clouds, wind), astronomical cues (moon phases, star positions), and biological indicators (flora and fauna).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Thematic area by origin of the investigated population

Methods used in the articles

More than half of the publications (61%, n = 222) utilized both primary and secondary data in their analyses. In contrast, the remaining publications (39%, n = 139) relied solely on primary information, which refers to data collected directly from the studied population. Secondary data sources encompassed various types, including data from meteorological stations (e.g., [58, 70, 72, 111,112,113]), satellite-based precipitation estimates (e.g., [114]), official government data (e.g., [115]), census information (e.g., [116]), or literature reviews (e.g., [71, 113]). However, recall that no publications exclusively based on secondary data were identified, as one at the inclusion criteria specified the presence of primary data.

The sampling unit most commonly employed in the articles was the household (35%, n = 127). However, a nearly equal proportion of articles focused on the individual (33%, n = 121), with the remainder of them encompassing both. (Fig. 5a). There were exceptions, such as Salvadeo's study [60], which examined the community as the sample unit.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Proportion and number of articles (above bars) with different methodological approaches (total number = 361)

Regarding the methodology used, mixed-method approaches (combining qualitative and quantitative methods) were employed in a larger proportion of the publications (63%; n = 227), while about one-third utilized exclusively quantitative methods (26%; n = 93) (Fig. 5b). When analyzing the combination of methodological approaches (quantitative, qualitative, mixed) and data types used (primary, primary, and secondary), it becomes evident (Fig. 5c), that the most common approach (n = 160) involves employing mixed methods and incorporating both primary and secondary data.

Theoretical approach of the articles

Out of the 361 articles, 43 (22%) incorporated 18 different theories. A small proportion of the reviewed articles explicitly mentioned at least one theory (10% of all articles, n = 36), while 2% (n = 7) presented multiple theories [9, 66, 69, 117,118,119,120]. Additional details on how these theories were utilized or cited in the articles can be found in Additional file 9.

The large majority of articles (88%) do not appear to be based on theories, at least explicitly mentioned, indicating a limited theoretical foundation in this research area. Furthermore, even in studies with theoretical foundations, the theory was often not evaluated in the article’s results, except for some theoretical frameworks like the Capital Approach Framework [121, 122]. Other theories were only briefly referenced in the articles (e.g., Integral Theory, Norgaard's Theory of Denial, Grounded Theory) [108, 117, 118, 123]. Additionally, very few articles (4%, n = 15) combined at least one theory with a definition of perception.

Given the limited focus on theoretical approaches, it can be inferred that there is no consensus on which theories should serve as the foundation for studying climate change perceptions among smallholders. The majority of theories adopted come from Psychology (35%) and Economics (23%), with some being multidisciplinary (12%). The most commonly addressed approach is Cognitive biases, which were discussed in 16 articles. This is followed by Theories of Rational Choice, particularly the Expected Utility Theory, which appeared in eight articles. These articles explored cognitive biases as psychological barriers to accurate climate perceptions [9, 69, 124, 125] or as factors influencing individual decision-making [30, 55, 118].

Additional theories were utilized to elucidate climate change perception and risk perception, including Cultural Theory [126], Value-Believe-Norm Theory [127, 128], the Perceptual Geography Approach [129], the Theory of Planned Behavior [130, 131], and the Model of Private Proactive Adaptation to Climate Change [66, 132]. The Expected Utility Theory was applied to gain insight into how individuals make choices regarding adaptation strategies to maximize their utility, considering factors like higher profit or reduced impact of climate change [133,134,135,136].

In response to climate risk, the behavior of individuals was explored through the Prospect Theory [69], and this behavior could influence the choice of adaptation strategy, as elucidated by the Protection Motivation Theory [54, 137, 138]. Other theories also delved into adaptation, such as the Climate Change Response Model [70], which examined the connection between vulnerability, perception, and adaptation strategies. Lastly, the Capital Theory (or Capital Approach Framework) examined the relationship between perception and adaptive assets or adaptation strategies [122, 139].

In summary, the literature in the area is seldom underpinned by theoretical frameworks. When articles incorporate theory, the explanations for perception are frequently rooted in Psychology and rationality, except for cognitive biases and the Prospect Theory, which provide non-rational explanations.

Explicit definition of perception and perception constructs

Out of the 361 articles, only 20% (n = 73) included an explicit definition of the concept of perception. A smaller percentage (4%; n = 15) presented a theoretical definition throughout the article (see Additional file 9), rooted in specific theories of the Human or Social Sciences. As anticipated, when comparing the years 2018 to 2022, there was an increase from 12% (n = 9) to 30% (n = 22) in the use of explicit concepts in published articles (Fig. 6a). Among the articles that provided an explicit definition, the vast majority presented the concept only in the introduction (80%, n = 58), with a smaller number mentioning it exclusively in the Methods section (8%, n = 6), the Results (8%, n = 6), or the Discussion (4%, n = 3).

Fig. 6
figure 6

Information about the definitions of perception

The explicit definitions identified in the articles were categorized into seven groups, with inclusion criteria detailed in Table 1. These groups reflect the primary context of the explicit definition utilized. Each definition was assigned to a group based on its main characteristic, even if specific definitions could potentially fit into more than one group. All explicit definitions are presented in Table 2.

Table 1 Description of the criteria for inclusion in one of the seven perception groups
Table 2 Explicit definitions of the concept of perception divided into categories (n = 73 articles)

Certain words appeared more frequently in the definitions of perception, including terms like awareness, knowledge, experience(s), risk, process, environment, information, psychological, beliefs, subjective, personal, understanding, and stimuli (Fig. 6b). This observation indicates that climate change perception definitions amalgamate elements from Cognitive Psychology (e.g., stimulus, awareness) with other constructs, such as experience, beliefs, knowledge, and understanding.

All 361 articles included at least one perception construct. The most frequently utilized constructs for defining the concept of perception, either explicitly or implicitly, were direct exposure (99%, n = 359) and perception based on sensory stimuli (96%, n = 345) (Fig. 7). Interestingly, none of the sample articles incorporated the construct “consciousness.” In Cognitive Psychology, consciousness is defined as the “state of wakefulness, the ability to control behavior and be aware of surroundings and mental experiences” [187]. It encompasses various psychological constructs, such as attitudes, knowledge, beliefs, values, and actions (behavior) [188, 189]. Environmental consciousness, or environmental concern, is considered a synonym of consciousness in the context of environmental issues [168]. It can indeed lead to engaging in pro-environmental behaviors as a response to climate change. This means that individuals with a heightened environmental consciousness are more likely to take action to mitigate, minimize, or avoid the adverse impacts of climate change. However, it's worth noting that this construct was not incorporated into the articles, possibly because climate change perception primarily focuses on understanding individuals' awareness and comprehension of climate change rather than directly addressing their subsequent behaviors or pro-environmental actions, which are typically studied in the context of the adoption of adaptive strategies.

Fig. 7
figure 7

Perception constructs adopted in the 361 articles

In the context of climate change literature, it is evident that the constructs implicitly defining the concept of perception extend beyond the purview of bottom-up or top-down theories within Cognitive Psychology. This departure is due to the fact that, in studies of climate change perception, the literature on perception constructs encompasses more than merely sensory stimuli, which is what we might anticipated with using bottom-up theories. Furthermore, the literature does not exclusively revolve around prior experiences (both direct and indirect exposure) or knowledge (whether scientific or traditional), which are typically associated with top-down theories.

Instead, the concept of perception in climate change literature encompasses a broader array of constructs. These include individual beliefs, feelings of concern, risk perception, attitudes, diverse worldviews, and even an individual's awareness of climate change, as depicted in Fig. 7.

This underscores the interdisciplinary nature of research into climate change perceptions. For additional details about these constructs, please refer to Additional file 10. Additionally, you can explore the distribution of these constructs by geographical region in Additional file 11.

Among the 361 articles, each one included at least two perception constructs. However, not every article that contained one construct necessarily contained another specific construct. For example, articles featuring the construct of “perception” did not necessarily include the construct of “awareness”—the same pattern applied to other constructs. Nevertheless, there were two exceptions to this trend.

Firstly, the construct of “direct exposure” consistently appeared in combination with various other constructs such as “concern”, “indirect exposure”, “risk perception”, “attitude”, and “worldview”.

This result leads us to consider four conjectures: (i) Individuals who perceive climate change as a concern probably have experienced and been exposed to its effects. (ii) Climate change perception could arise from personal experiences with the changes or from experiences acquired from other people or the media. (iii) Risk perception, i.e. the subjective judgment of climate risk, is likely related to having experienced a weather change; (iv) Attitudes and worldviews could be influenced by experience and exposure to a weather change.

Secondly, in the results, articles that addressed perception through scientific knowledge also addressed perception according to sensory stimuli. According to Cognitive Psychology, this implies a relationship between having scientific knowledge and perceiving climate change.

In general, the vast majority of articles (86%, n = 312) described perception (in the results, discussion, or conclusion of the articles) as the observation of climate change in specific environmental aspects through sensory stimuli and awareness of the effects of climate change.

The environmental aspects mentioned include descriptions of the observation of extreme events or climate variability anomalies. These descriptions can be individual reports of changes frequently or occasionally observed, including increases or reductions in factors such as precipitation, drought, temperature, and/or biophysical indicators, as well as others related to changes, such as in the flowering of plants. In these articles, the term "extreme" events or "anomalies" often refers to phenomena such as storms, landslides, tornadoes, wildfires, prolonged droughts, heatwaves and cold waves, frosts, lightning, cyclones, hurricanes, windstorms, or floods. For instance, agropastoralists [111] and pastoralists [190] in Nepal have reported a significant increase in summer temperatures over the last few decades, accompanied by a decrease in the amount of snow. Farmers have also documented unexpected occurrences like rains, droughts, floods, and violent winds [191].

Other studies have documented changes in plants, plant parts, and animals. For example, indigenous people in Zimbabwe have noted a decrease in the availability of wild berries and an increase in the insect population due to climate change. Additionally, farmers in Uganda have reported an increase in pests and a reduction in the number of native trees, besides the occurrence of extreme events, such as increased temperatures and droughts, shorter rainy seasons, and higher frequency and intensity of extreme weather events [192].

A small percentage of the articles (5%, n = 18) described climate perception, including what climate change is, its effects, and causes, based on people's traditional knowledge, sometimes incorporating weather predictors and indicators. For example, one study demonstrated that indigenous people in Bolivia rely on atmospheric, astronomic, flora, and fauna indicators when observing the environment to predict weather phenomena [104]. Among farmers in Ethiopia, climate change perception primarily relies on traditional knowledge [193]. Another study reported varying levels of traditional knowledge about climate change among indigenous people in South Africa [105]. These locals have been using indicators based on flora and fauna to make meteorological predictions and inform decisions related to planting. In Ghana, farmers utilize traditional meteorological knowledge, including flora and fauna indicators, to predict the rainy season and anticipate the occurrences of lightning [194].

An even smaller percentage (2%, n = 8) of articles contain descriptions of climate perception or climate variability perception that rely on information provided by people from outside the community and/or by the media, such as radio, television, newspapers, and magazines. Among these eight articles [42, 60, 69, 175, 195,196,197,198], agropastoralists in Burkina Faso used radios, casual discussions, and input from people outside the community to gather information about climate change [195]. A study with farmers in South Africa demonstrated that participating in a farmers' association and listening to the radio heightened their perception of climate change [175]. Indigenous people in Niger acquired information about climate change through local radio, television, farmers' associations, and phones [196]. In Bangladesh, indigenous people gathered information from television, newspapers, non-governmental organizations, and researchers [42].

The same percentage of articles described perception as the degree of concern individuals have about climate change's effects on their subsistence activities. For example, in one study by Budhathoki et al. [137], farmers in Nepal were less concerned about cold periods and heatwaves, and more concerned about floods in agriculture. Even so, they reported that all these events, together with droughts, were the main climate risks. Lone et al. [199] documented the apprehensions of Indian farmers regarding the impacts of climate change on agriculture. In Colombia, Eitzinger et al. [200] demonstrated that older agropastoralists expressed greater concern about climate change compared to their younger counterparts. Nevertheless, in Zimbabwe, agropastoralists were more concerned about the effects of climate change on agriculture and livestock production [201].

One article measured the concept of perception based on individuals' scientific knowledge of climate change. This was accomplished by employing a knowledge test created through consultation with various sources and bibliographic databases, as well as with experts in climate change [202].

Lastly, 4% of the articles (n = 14) did not present any description of respondents' perception of climate change in their results (e.g., [54, 122, 126, 130, 203,204,205,206,207,208,209,210,211]). Although these articles aimed to assess the perception of climate change, their results went in different directions. For example, based on Capital Theory, Torres et al. [122] assessed climate change perceptions by dividing respondents into five quintiles without describing each group's perceptions. Using exploratory factor analysis and regression models to analyze the data, Alam et al. [211] also did not present respondents’ perceptions in the results. Another article, by Budhathoki et al. [54], only referred to adaptation strategies.

Ng’ombe et al. [209] presented an average level of awareness of climate change and focused on perception determinants. Sarker et al. [210] calculated a climate change perception index to describe perception. Salite [208] focused on beliefs about droughts and the reasons behind these beliefs. Quandt [204] addressed resilience perception in livelihoods and the correlation between drought perception and other variables, such as ethnic group. Makondo et al. [205] focused on changes in dietary habits, migration, taboos, spirituality, and ecosystem services to examine traditional knowledge and awareness of climate change and related environmental risks. Lemahieu et al. [207] addressed changes in the environment. Ambrosio-Albala et al. [126] used the Q methodology to explore and compare worldviews according to Cultural Theory. Lastly, Khanal et al. [206] analyzed determinants and impacts on adaptation strategies.

For the complete description of all excerpts extracted from the results of the 361 articles, as provided by authors (when explicit), see Additional file 12.

Observed phenomena

Perception through the observation of physical phenomena was reported in 96% of articles (n = 346). These articles described the changes individuals observed (or did not observe), such as temperature, precipitation, wind patterns, changes in seasons, and extreme weather events or anomalies (such as droughts, hurricanes, wildfires, landslides, floods, and tornados, among others).

The articles’ second most addressed aspect of perception was the observation of human phenomena. Thus, in 85% of the articles (n = 309), changes in subsistence practices were observed (such as modifications in the timing of activities or the abandonment of certain practices; changes in human health (e.g., increased disease incidence, basic sanitation needs); alterations in food availability; human migration patterns; or the acquisition/loss of traditional knowledge.

Lastly, perception of biological phenomena was reported in 70% of the articles (n = 254). These articles presented whether changes were observed or not in plants (e.g., alternations in vegetation cover, the appearance of invasive species, and flowering patterns of specific plants), animals (e.g., the emergence or disappearance of particular species, disappearance of insect populations, the emergence of pests, and the presence of invasive species), and other living beings.

Limitations of the map

One of the limitations of this study is the English-only document review, which is justified by three reasons. First, the article's primary objective was to identify and describe the various perception concepts in use rather than attempting to analyze each individual article that adopted the term. Second, specific perception terms (e.g., awareness, attitudes) lack direct translation across languages and, consequently, including other idioms would introduce inconsistencies in the coding process. Third, our pre-tests did not reveal any significant geographical distribution in using these concepts. Our choice provides access to worldwide research while reducing selection bias that could result from focusing on a few languages. Furthermore, as the primary goal of this article is to systematize and analyze the alternative uses of the term “perception” in the context of climate change, language differences would likely pose insurmountable challenges to our review, particularly when dealing with implicit definitions.

A second limitation is the restriction of the search period to 2018–2022. Expanding the sample to include years before 2018 could be valuable for two reasons. First, it would allow the inclusion of more articles concerning small-scale populations, as indicated in our published protocol [85]. Second, it would enable comparisons between recent and older literature, facilitating the evaluation of trends.

Third, although the review team devoted effort to the elaboration of the search string (see our protocol [85]), the string may not have captured all existing literature on the subject for reasons we are not aware of.

Fourth, owing to the considerable volume of articles on one hand and our limited time and resources on the other, we did not review the grey literature in this study. While this might be considered a limitation to be addressed by other researchers in future studies, we have no reason to believe that the patterns of concept usage differ between these two types of literature.

Lastly, because in most articles’ definitions were implicit, we may have missed a few definitions or constructs even if attempting to encompass all the spectrum of concepts.

Conclusions

This systematic mapping provided a detailed description of how the concept of climate change perception has been adopted, both explicitly or implicitly, in the scientific literature. After analyzing 361 articles (out of 5,358 screened), we observed that the definitions of climate change perception for small-scale populations varied when explicit. Explicit definitions fell into seven main categories: risk perception, perception according to psychological constructs and sensory stimuli, awareness, prior experience, observation of climate variables, beliefs and interpretations of the environment, and uncertainties and threats. However, explicit concepts were relatively scarce, comprising only one-sixth of the total articles. As a result, the literature primarily consists of implicit uses of the concept, which we have analyzed by identifying constructs within the articles.

As for explicit and implicit definitions, we initially expected that most concepts would align with Cognitive Psychology theories. However, we found that the concepts were interdisciplinary in nature. For instance, perception was defined not only based on psychological and sensory constructs but also through direct experiences with the phenomenon, indirect experiences, sets of beliefs, worldviews, and traditional knowledge. Furthermore, authors often began their texts by defining perception as a sensory stimulus, for example. However, when presenting results, authors frequently referred to perception in different ways, such as an individual's concern about climate change or a set of beliefs.

The explicit definitions were diverse, encompassing various thematic areas and perception constructs. Many articles addressed multiple subjects or perception constructs, although there were challenges in identifying the perception construct used, as they were often implicit. Consequently, other constructs may have been unintentionally overlooked by the reviewers.

This conceptual confusion is somehow expected, as climate change is not solely defined by perception and awareness. Frequently, other factors are essential for understanding it, including risk perception, individuals’ beliefs about climate change, and their direct or indirect exposure to its effects.

When it comes to a comprehensive theoretical understanding of smallholders’ perceptions of climate change, the findings suggest that the literature requires improvement. Even in cases where articles presented a theory throughout their content, the analysis was not consistently guided by that theory when assessing study results. In many other articles, theories were only mentioned sporadically. For example, theories might only appear in the methods section or the final discussion, without having guided the data collection process. As expected, when theoretical or analytical frameworks were adopted, particularly the Capital Approach Framework or Cognitive Psychology theories, it facilitated easier comparisons of results across studies.

We can speculate about why the literature has not yet reached a high level of conceptual and theoretical advancement. One possible reason is the field's interdisciplinary nature, with researchers who address the concept of perception often coming from various disciplines and, sometimes, with academic backgrounds outside of Psychology and other Social Sciences. Consequently, they may not always adhere to the rigorous protocols in these scientific fields, where clear concept presentation and consistent definitions throughout the papers are crucial. These shortcomings in current research on smallholders’ perceptions could potentially impede progress in accumulating knowledge and, consequently, hinder the development of effective public policies to mitigate the negative impacts of climate change on vulnerable populations.

Implication for policy/management

We highlight three key implications for policy development.

Firstly, it is essential to foster a comprehensive understanding of the concept of climate change perception. Its absence can lead to erroneous conclusions, limited adaptation strategies, and a failure to comprehend the full spectrum of climate change impacts. For example, suppose most studies focus on perception solely as a measure of direct observation of current natural disasters. In that case, adaptation measures may overlook more subtle changes with potentially more severe long-term consequences, such as shifts in agricultural productivity. Given that climate change can introduce new impacts over time, a lack of perception hampers the ability to adapt to evolving climate conditions.

Secondly, gaining a clearer understanding of conceptual differences in perceptions is crucial because researchers' conceptualizations of perception influence data collection methods, potentially affecting research outcomes and, in turn, policy formulation. Once again, the misuse or misunderstanding of these concepts can lead to ineffective policies.

Lastly, the absence of clear definitions for the concept of perception and its associated constructs may impede the effective engagement of small-scale communities in participatory research and the implementation of adaptation strategies. Small-scale communities may have differing interpretations of the concept and its constructs compared to researchers. For instance, alternative ways of perceiving gradual changes or perceptions that are more subjective than objective may be overlooked. In all cases, without rigorously adopting definitions suitable for smallholders, discussions about impacts and adaptation may inadvertently exclude the most vulnerable and at-risk small-scale populations.

Implication for research

There are four main implications for research.

First, the field of climate change research is inherently interdisciplinary. Therefore, maintaining a continuous dialogue among various disciplines is essential for developing one or more interdisciplinary definitions of the perception concept. This collaboration allows researchers from diverse fields to pursue their studies based on their unique perspectives on perception while also promoting integration across disciplines.

Second, defining the concepts of perception and its associated constructs is crucial, as this is essential for enabling comparisons of results across studies. Attempting to compare results from articles on climate change perceptions becomes impractical or misleading when (i) the concept of perception is left undefined, (ii) different perception definitions are employed, or (iii) comparisons are made between articles that share constructs but use different definitions for the term.

Third, it is probable that many facets of perception are being overlooked, even though implicit constructs are often utilized in defining perception. For instance, authors often allude to direct experience in their articles’ results, even when employing different terminology, such as "observing climate change through subsistence activities or engaging in specific practices.” However, the initial definition of perception is often described as sensory stimuli. Thus, it is essential to standardize the concept throughout the text.

Lastly, more research in Central and Latin America focusing on small-scale populations is needed, as our analysis has revealed a lack of investigations in these two regions. These areas are home to various small-scale populations, which are particularly vulnerable to the impacts of climate change, including indigenous and non-indigenous communities with varying levels of dependence on natural resources, residing in diverse ecosystems, both urban and remote rural. Additionally, our description of the thematic areas explored serves as a valuable evidence database for researchers seeking to identify areas requiring further scientific investigations in small-scale populations.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable.

Notes

  1. Russia is located in two continents (Europa and Asia). Here it is considered part of the European continent, as the Codebook defines it as “transcontinental—generally considered European.”.

References

  1. IPCC. Global warming of 1.5°C. An IPCC Special Report on the impacts of global warming of 1.5°C above pre-industrial levels and related global greenhouse gas emission pathways, in the context of strengthening the global response to the threat of climate change. In Press. Cambridge University Press; 2018, p. 616 https://www.ipcc.ch/site/assets/uploads/sites/2/2022/06/SR15_Full_Report_HR.pdf

  2. IPCC. Summary for Policymakers. Climate Change 2021: The Physical Science Basis. Contribution of Working Group I to the Sixth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change. Cambridge University Press; 2021, p. 3–32 https://www.ipcc.ch/report/ar6/wg1/downloads/report/IPCC_AR6_WGI_SPM.pdf

  3. World Bank T. Poverty and shared prosperity 2020. Reversals of fortune. Washington: World Bank Group; 2020. p. 178.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  4. IWGIA. The Indigenous World 2022. 36th Edition. Mamo D, Berger DN, Bulanin N, García-Alix L, Jensen MW, Leth S, et al., editors. 2022. 848 p.

  5. Kronik J, Verner D. Indigenous peoples and climate change in Latin America and the Caribbean. Washington: The World Bank; 2010. p. 190.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  6. Dick-Sagoe C, Hope KN, Asare-Nuamah P. Perceived impact of climate variability and change on livelihoods of smallholder farmers in Lesotho. Afr J Sci Technol Innov Dev. 2023;15(2):175–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Akano O, Modirwa S, Oluwasemire K, Oladele O. Awareness and perception of climate change by smallholder farmers in two agroecological zones of Oyo state Southwest Nigeria. GeoJournal. 2022;30:39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Guodaar L, Kabila A, Afriyie K, Segbefia AY, Addai G. Farmers’ perceptions of severe climate risks and adaptation interventions in indigenous communities in northern Ghana. Int J Disaster Risk Reduct. 2023;95: 103891.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Nkuba MR, Chanda R, Mmopelwa G, Kato E, Najjingo Mangheni M, Lesolle D, et al. Effect of indigenous and scientific forecasts on pastoralists’ climate change perceptions in the Rwenzori region, Western Uganda. Clim Dev. 2022;15:1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Leonard L. Climate change impacts and challenges of combating food insecurity in rural Somkhele, KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa. Sustainability. 2022;14(23):16023.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Rosol R, Powell-Hellyer S, Chan LHM. Impacts of decline harvest of country food on nutrient intake among Inuit in Arctic Canada: Impact of climate change and possible adaptation plan. Int J Circumpolar Health. 2016;75:31127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Berrang-Ford L, Dingle K, Ford JD, Lee C, Lwasa S, Namanya DB, et al. Vulnerability of indigenous health to climate change: a case study of Uganda’s Batwa Pygmies. Soc Sci Med. 2012;75(6):1067–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Middleton J, Cunsolo A, Jones-Bitton A, Wright CJ, Harper SL. Indigenous mental health in a changing climate: a systematic scoping review of the global literature. Environ Res Lett. 2020;15(5):1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Middleton J, Cunsolo A, Jones-Bitton A, Shiwak I, Wood M, Pollock N, et al. “We’re people of the snow:” weather, climate change, and Inuit mental wellness. Soc Sci Med. 2020;262:1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Torres-Slimming PA, Wright CJ, Lancha G, Carcamo CP, Garcia PJ, Ford JD, et al. Climatic changes, water systems, and adaptation challenges in Shawi Communities in the Peruvian Amazon. Sustainability. 2020;12(8):3422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Lastra Landa DE, Grados Bueno CV. “Climate change might have caused our small harvest”: indigenous vulnerability, livelihoods, and environmental changes in lowland and high jungle indigenous communities in Peru. J Environ Stud Sci. 2022;12(2):216–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Sakakibara C. “No whale, no music”: Iñupiaq drumming and global warming. Polar Rec. 2009;45(4):289–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Bronen R. Forced Migration of Alaskan Indigenous Communities Due to Climate Change. In: Environment, Forced Migration and Social Vulnerability. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg; 2010. p. 87–98.

  19. Kelman I, Næss M. Climate change and migration for Scandinavian saami: a review of possible impacts. Climate. 2019;7(4):47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Ford JD, King N, Galappaththi EK, Pearce T, McDowell G, Harper SL. The resilience of indigenous peoples to environmental change. One Earth. 2020;2(6):532–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Berkes F, Berkes MK. Ecological complexity, fuzzy logic, and holism in indigenous knowledge. Futures. 2009;41(1):6–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Deressa TT, Hassan RM, Ringler C. Perception of and adaptation to climate change by farmers in the Nile basin of Ethiopia. J Agric Sci. 2011;149(1):23–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Alam GMM, Alam K, Mushtaq S. Climate change perceptions and local adaptation strategies of hazard-prone rural households in Bangladesh. Clim Risk Manag. 2017;17:52–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. IPCC. IPCC, 2022: Summary for Policymakers. Climate Change 2022: Impacts, Adaptation, and Vulnerability Contribution of Working Group II to the Sixth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change. Cambridge, UK and New York, NY, USA: Cambridge University Press; 2022. p. 3–33.

  25. Maddison D. The perception of and adaptation to climate change in Africa. Policy Research Working Paper. 2007;4308(10):1–51.

  26. Oyerinde GT, Hountondji FCC, Wisser D, Diekkrüger B, Lawin AE, Odofin AJ, et al. Hydro-climatic changes in the Niger basin and consistency of local perceptions. Reg Environ Change. 2015;15(8):1627–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Reyes-García V, Fernández-Llamazares Á, Guèze M, Garcés A, Mallo M, Vila-Gómez M, et al. Local indicators of climate change: the potential contribution of local knowledge to climate research. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Clim Change. 2016;7(1):109–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Savo V, Lepofsky D, Benner JP, Kohfeld KE, Bailey J, Lertzman K. Observations of climate change among subsistence-oriented communities around the world. Nat Clim Chang. 2016;6(5):462–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Muringai RT, Naidoo D, Mafongoya P, Sibanda M. Small-scale fishers’ perceptions of climate change and its consequences on fisheries: the case of Sanyathi fishing basin, Lake Kariba, Zimbabwe. Trans Royal Soc S Afr. 2019;74(3):248–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Waldman KB, Vergopolan N, Attari SZ, Sheffield J, Estes LD, Caylor KK, et al. Cognitive biases about climate variability in smallholder farming systems in Zambia. Weather Clim Soc. 2019;11(2):369–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Ekemini-Richard M, Ayanwale AB, Adelegan OJ. Factors influencing choice of climate change adaptation methods among underutilized indigenous vegetable farmers. Int J Veg Sci. 2020;28(2):111–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Mijiddorj TN, Alexander JS, Samelius G, Mishra C, Boldgiv B. Traditional livelihoods under a changing climate: herder perceptions of climate change and its consequences in South Gobi, Mongolia. Clim Change. 2020;162(3):1065–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Ogra M, Manral U, Platt RV, Badola R, Butcher L. Local perceptions of change in climate and agroecosystems in the Indian Himalayas: a case study of the Kedarnath Wildlife Sanctuary (KWS) landscape, India. Appl Geogr. 2020;9: 102339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Shijin W. Perception of indigenous people of climate change and its impact on the Everest National Nature Preserve. Meteorol Appl. 2021;28:1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Anyanwu IN, Nwajiuba CA. Climatic impacts on socio-cultural behavior, health and psychology of rural communities in South East Nigeria. Environ Chall. 2021;4: 100102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Braisby N, Gellatly A. Cognitive psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2012. p. 714.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Sánchez-Cortés MS, Chavero EL. Indigenous perception of changes in climate variability and its relationship with agriculture in a Zoque community of Chiapas, Mexico. Clim Change. 2011;107(3):363–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Hasan MK, Kumar L. Meteorological data and farmers’ perception of coastal climate in Bangladesh. Sci Total Environ. 2020;704: 135384.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Sraku-Lartey M, Buor D, Adjei POW, Foli EG. Perceptions and knowledge on climate change in local communities in the Offinso Municipality, Ghana. Inf Dev. 2020;36(1):16–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Ghazali S, Azadi H, Janeckova K, Sklenicka P, Kurban A, Cakir S. Indigenous knowledge about climate change and sustainability of nomadic livelihoods: understanding adaptability coping strategies. Environ Dev Sustain. 2021;23(11):16744–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Mairura FS, Musafiri CM, Kiboi MN, Macharia JM, Ng’etich OK, Shisanya CA, et al. Determinants of farmers’ perceptions of climate variability, mitigation, and adaptation strategies in the central highlands of Kenya. Weather Clim Extrem. 2021;34:1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Ahmed MNQ, Atiqul Haq SM. Indigenous people’s perceptions about climate change, forest resource management, and coping strategies: a comparative study in Bangladesh. Environ Dev Sustain. 2019;21(2):679–708.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Balama C, Augustino S, Eriksen S, Makonda FBS. Forest adjacent households’ voices on their perceptions and adaptation strategies to climate change in Kilombero District, Tanzania. Springerplus. 2016;5:792.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Herman-Mercer NM, Matkin E, Laituri MJ, Toohey RC, Massey M, Elder K, et al. Changing times, changing stories: Generational differences in climate change perspectives from four remote indigenous communities in Subarctic Alaska. Ecol Soc. 2016. https://doi.org/10.5751/ES-08463-210328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Hasan MK, Kumar L. Comparison between meteorological data and farmer perceptions of climate change and vulnerability in relation to adaptation. J Environ Manage. 2019;9:54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Wyllie de Echeverria VR, Thornton TF. Using traditional ecological knowledge to understand and adapt to climate and biodiversity change on the Pacific coast of North America. Ambio. 2019;48(12):1447–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Ayanlade A, Radeny M, Morton JF. Comparing smallholder farmers’ perception of climate change with meteorological data: a case study from southwestern Nigeria. Weather Clim Extremes. 2017;10:24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Paudel B, Zhang Y, Yan J, Rai R, Li L, Wu X, et al. Farmers’ understanding of climate change in Nepal Himalayas: important determinants and implications for developing adaptation strategies. Clim Change. 2020;158(3–4):485–502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Timité N, Kouakou ATM, Bamba I, Barima YSS, Bogaert J. Climate variability in the Sudanian Zone of Côte d’Ivoire: weather observations, perceptions, and adaptation strategies of farmers. Sustainability. 2022;14(16):10410.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Hoang HG. Vietnamese smallholders’ perspectives on causes, indicators and determinants of climate change: implication for adaptation strategies. Clim Change. 2020;162(3):1127–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Junqueira AB, Fernández-Llamazares Á, Torrents-Ticó M, Haira PL, Nasak JG, Burgas D, et al. Interactions between climate change and infrastructure projects in changing water resources: an ethnobiological perspective from the Daasanach, Kenya. J Ethnobiol. 2021;41(3):331–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Bosekeng LC, Mogotsi K, Bosekeng G. Farmers’ perception of climate change and variability in the North-East District of Botswana. Livestock Res Rural Dev. 2020;17.

  53. Yang S, Yu L, Leng G, Qiu H. Livestock farmers’ perception and adaptation to climate change: panel evidence from pastoral areas in China. Clim Change. 2021;164(1–2):21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Budhathoki NK, Paton D, Lassa JA, Bhatta GD, Zander KK. Heat, cold, and floods: exploring farmers’ motivations to adapt to extreme weather events in the Terai region of Nepal. Nat Hazards. 2020;103(3):3213–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Waldman KB, Attari SZ, Gower DB, Giroux SA, Caylor KK, Evans TP. The salience of climate change in farmer decision-making within smallholder semi-arid agroecosystems. Clim Change. 2019;156(4):527–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Pandey VP, Sharma A, Dhaubanjar S, Bharati L, Joshi IR. Climate shocks and responses in Karnali-Mahakali Basins, Western Nepal. Climate. 2019;7(7):92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Behailu G, Ayal DY, Zeleke TT, Ture K, Bantider A. Comparative analysis of meteorological records of climate variability and farmers’ perceptions in Sekota Woreda, Ethipia. Clim Serv. 2021;23: 100239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Bom U, Tiefenbacher J, Belbase S. Individual and community perceptions of climate change in Lower Mustang, Nepal. Environ Dev Sustain. 2022;35:5997.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Habte M, Eshetu M, Maryo M, Andualem D, Legesse A. Effects of climate variability on livestock productivity and pastoralists perception: the case of drought resilience in Southeastern Ethiopia. Vet Anim Sci. 2022;16: 100240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Salvadeo C, Morzaria-Luna HN, Reyes-Bonilla H, Ivanova-Bonchera A, Ramírez DP, Juárez-León E. Fisher’s perceptions inform adaptation measures to reduce vulnerability to climate change in a Mexican natural protected area. Mar Policy. 2021;134: 104793.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Fierros-González I, López-Feldman A. Farmers’ perception of climate change: a review of the literature for Latin America. Front Environ Sci. 2021;9:1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Miara MD, Negadi M, Tabak S, Bendif H, Dahmani W, AitHammou M, et al. Climate change impacts can be differentially perceived across time scales: a study among the Tuareg of the Algerian Sahara. Geohealth. 2022;6(11): e2022GH000620.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  63. Rankoana SA. Climate change impact on indigenous food resources. Int J Soc Sci Res Rev. 2022;5(10):522–9.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Batumike R, Bulonvu F, Imani G, Akonkwa D, Gahigi A, Klein JA, et al. Climate change and hunter-gatherers in Montane eastern DR Congo. Clim Dev. 2022;14(5):431–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Saarinen J, Moswete N, Atlhopheng JR, Hambira WL. Changing socio-ecologies of Kalahari: Local perceptions towards environmental change and tourism in Kgalagadi, Botswana. Dev South Afr. 2020;37(5):855–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Koirala P, Kotani K, Managi S. How do farm size and perceptions matter for farmers’ adaptation responses to climate change in a developing country? Evid Nepal Econ Anal Policy. 2022;74:188–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Chanza N, Musakwa W. Ecological and hydrological indicators of climate change observed by dryland communities of Malipati in Chiredzi, Zimbabwe. Diversity (Basel). 2022;14(7):541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Tendonkeng F, Arnaud HTB, Noumbissi MNB, Miégoué E, Sawa C, Essie FMN, et al. Perceptions and adaptation strategies for climate change from small ruminant in North-West Cameroon. Trop Subtrop Agroecosyst. 2022;25:1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  69. Villacis AH, Alwang JR, Barrera V. Linking risk preferences and risk perceptions of climate change: a prospect theory approach. Agric Econ. 2021;52(5):863–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Maliki MA, Pauline NM. Living and responding to climatic stresses: perspectives from smallholder farmers in Hanang’ District, Tanzania. Environ Manage. 2022;14:131.

    Google Scholar 

  71. Chimi PM, Mala WA, Fobane JL, Essouma FM, Mbom JA II, Funwi FP, et al. Climate change perception and local adaptation of natural resource management in a farming community of Cameroon: a case study. Environ Chall. 2022;8: 100539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Reddy KV, Paramesh V, Arunachalam V, Das B, Ramasundaram P, Pramanik M, et al. Farmers’ perception and efficacy of adaptation decisions to climate change. Agronomy. 2022;12(5):1023.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  73. Mkonda MY, He X, Festin ES. Comparing smallholder farmers’ perception of climate change with meteorological data: experience from seven agroecological zones of Tanzania. Weather Clim Soc. 2018;10(3):435–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Benson N, Ginsburg J, Grand V, Lazyan M, Weeks M. The Psychology Book. 2012. 352 p.

  75. Sternberg RJ, Karin S, Jeff M. Cognitive Psychology. Cognitive Psychology. Wadsworth, Cengage Learning; 2012.

  76. Braisby N, Gellatly A. Foundations of cognitive psychology. In: Braisby N, Gellatly A, editors. Cognitive psychology. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  77. Pike G, Edgar G, Edgar H. Perception. In: Braisby N, Gellatly A, editors. Cognitive psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2012. p. 65–99.

    Google Scholar 

  78. Pomerantz JR. Perception: Overview. In: Encyclopedia of cognitive science. London: Nature Publishing Group; 2003. p. 1–11.

  79. Brovkin V, Brook E, Williams JW, Bathiany S, Lenton TM, Barton M, et al. Past abrupt changes, tipping points and cascading impacts in the Earth system. Nat Geosci. 2021;14(8):550–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  80. Chen X. Why do people misunderstand climate change? Heuristics, mental models and ontological assumptions. Clim Change. 2011;108(1):31–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Reisberg D. Cognition Exploring the Science of the Mind. 4th ed. Snavely SL, editor. W. W. Norton & Company; 2010. 147 p.

  82. Reid MG, Hamilton C, Reid SK, Trousdale W, Hill C, Turner N, et al. Indigenous climate change adaptation planning using a values-focused approach: a case study with the Gitga’at Nation. J Ethnobiol. 2014;34(3):401–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. APA. APA Dictionary of Psychology. Second Edition. VandenBos GR, editor. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association (APA); 2007. 1204 p.

  84. Khalil CA, Piero C, Ergin I, Gennari P. Defining small-scale food producers to monitor target 23 of the 2030 agenda for sustainable development. FAO Stat Stat Working Paper Ser. 2017;17(12):1–51.

    Google Scholar 

  85. Troncarelli LT, Morsello C. What evidence exists on conceptual differences in climate change perceptions of smallholders? A systematic map protocol. Environ Evid. 2022;11(1):1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. CEE. Guidelines and Standards for Evidence synthesis in Environmental Management. Version 5.0. Collaboration for Environmental Evidence. www.environmentalevidence.org/information-for-authors. Accessed 15 July 2021.

  87. Haddaway NR, Macura B, Whaley P, Pullin AS. ROSES reporting standards for systematic evidence syntheses: pro forma, flow-diagram and descriptive summary of the plan and conduct of environmental systematic reviews and systematic maps. Environ Evid. 2018;7(1):4–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  88. James KL, Randall NP, Haddaway NR. A methodology for systematic mapping in environmental sciences. Environ Evid. 2016;5(1):1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Haddaway NR; MB; WP; PAS. ROSES flow diagram for systematic maps. Version 1.0. 2017. Accessed 04 April 2023.

  90. Kazi S, Urrutia I, Ledden M van, Laboyrie JH, Verschuur J, Khan Z ul H, et al. Bangladesh enhancing coastal resilience in a changing climate. Washington, DC; 2022. 267 p.

  91. WMO WMO. State of the Global Climate 2021. World Meteorological Organization; 2022. 54 p.

  92. Eckstein D, Kunzel V, Schafer L. Global Climate Risk Index 2021: Who Suffers Most from Extreme Weather Events? Weather-related Loss Events in 2019 and 2000–2019. Chapman-Rose J, Longwitz J, editors. Berlin: Germanwatch e.V.; 2021. 48 p.

  93. WMO. State of the Climate in Africa. World Meteorological Organization; 2022. 48 p.

  94. WMO. United in Science 2022. 2022. https://public.wmo.int/en/resources/united_in_science. Accessed 5 Mar 2023.

  95. Rashid S, Yadav SS. Impact of Covid-19 pandemic on higher education and research. Indian J Hum Dev. 2020;14(2):340–3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  96. Lokhtina IA, Castelló M, Lambrechts AA, Löfström E, McGinn MK, Skakni I, et al. The impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on early career researcher activity, development, career, and well-being: the state of the art. Stud Grad Postdr Educ. 2022;13(3):245–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  97. Morgado P, Barbosa GC. Instituto Socioambiental. 2021. Wayana. https://pib.socioambiental.org/pt/Povo:Wayana. Accessed 15 Apr 2023.

  98. Grupioni DF. Instituto Socioambiental. 2021. Tiriyó. https://pib.socioambiental.org/pt/Povo:Tiriy%C3%B3. Accessed 15 Apr 2023.

  99. ONU. Cinco anos de conflito no Sudão do Sul: "o povo merece o fim do sofrimento”. 2018. https://news.un.org/pt/story/2018/12/1652091. Accessed 20 May 2023.

  100. Gallas D. BBC News Brasil. 2022. Além da Guerra na Uncrânia: 7 conflitos sangrentos que ocorrem hoje no mundo. https://www.bbc.com/portuguese/internacional-60690640. Accessed 20 May 2023.

  101. Sansoulet J, Therrien M, Delgove J, Pouxviel G, Desriac J, Sardet N, et al. An update on Inuit perceptions of their changing environment, Qikiqtaaluk (Baffin Island, Nunavut). Elementa Sci Anthropocene. 2020;8(1):025.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  102. Chisholm Hatfield S, Marino E, Whyte KP, Dello KD, Mote PW. Indian time: time, seasonality, and culture in Traditional Ecological Knowledge of climate change. Ecol Process. 2018;7(1):25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  103. World Bank T. For Up to 800 Million Rural Poor, a Strong World Bank Commitment to Agriculture. 2014. https://www.worldbank.org/en/news/feature/2014/11/12/for-up-to-800-million-rural-poor-a-strong-world-bank-commitment-to-agriculture. Accessed 14 Mar 2023.

  104. Bauer TN, de Jong W, Ingram V. Perception matters: an Indigenous perspective on climate change and its effects on forest-based livelihoods in the Amazon. Ecol Soc. 2022;27(1): art17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  105. Kom Z, Nethengwe NS, Mpandeli S, Chikoore H. Indigenous knowledge indicators employed by farmers for adaptation to climate change in rural South Africa. J Environ Plan Manag. 2022;66:2778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  106. Inman EN, Hobbs RJ, Tsvuura Z. No safety net in the face of climate change: the case of pastoralists in Kunene Region, Namibia. PLoS ONE. 2020;15(9): e0238982.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  107. Kieu TTH, Nguyen TN, Nguyen THT, Vu THA, Nguyen DHG, Nguyen QT. Indigenous knowledge in climate change adaptation: case studies of ethnic minorities in the Northern Mountain Region of Vietnam. J Vietnamese Environ. 2020;12(2):155–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  108. Mugambiwa SS, Rukema JR. Climate change and variability discourse among community members and smallholder farmers in Mutoko District, Zimbabwe. Mankind Q. 2020;61:225–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  109. Metcalfe SE, Schmook B, Boyd DS, de la Barreda-Bautista B, Endfield GE, Mardero S, et al. Community perception, adaptation and resilience to extreme weather in the Yucatan Peninsula, Mexico. Reg Environ Change. 2020;20(1):25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  110. Fitchett JM, Ebhuoma E. Phenological cues intrinsic in indigenous knowledge systems for forecasting seasonal climate in the Delta State of Nigeria. Int J Biometeorol. 2018;62(6):1115–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  111. Hamal R, Thakuri BM, Poudel KR, Gurung A, Yun SJ. Farmers’ perceptions of climate change in Lower Mustang. Nepal Environ Monit Assess. 2022;194(9):606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  112. Kimaro EG, Mor SM, Toribio JALML. Climate change perception and impacts on cattle production in pastoral communities of northern Tanzania. Pastoralism. 2018;8(1):19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  113. Saalu FN, Oriaso S, Gyampoh B. Effects of a changing climate on livelihoods of forest dependent communities. Int J Clim Chang Strateg Manag. 2020;12(1):1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  114. Funatsu BM, Dubreuil V, Racapé A, Debortoli NS, Nasuti S, le Tourneau FM. Perceptions of climate and climate change by Amazonian communities. Glob Environ Chang. 2019;57: 101923.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  115. de Batista WFM, Sparacino J, de Espindola GM, de Lucena RFP, de Barros RFM. Local trends in rain and temperatures and their perceptions by women for the surroundings of the Sete Cidades National Park in Piauí, Northeast of Brazil. Revista Brasileira de Geografia Física. 2020;13(3):1035–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  116. Kevla N, Tiwari PC, Dheeraj P, Rahul K. Changing climatic conditions and shrinking agricultural land: a community based study in betalghat development block, Kaumaun, Lesser Himalaya. Disaster Adv. 2022;15(6):1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  117. Guodaar L, Bardsley DK, Suh J. Integrating local perceptions with scientific evidence to understand climate change variability in northern Ghana: a mixed-methods approach. Appl Geogr. 2021;130: 102440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  118. Sharma R, Jagtap S, Rao P. Understanding Maharashtra coastal community’s perceptions and livelihood resilience to climate change using the community participatory approach. Int J Clim Change Impacts Responses. 2022;14(2):1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  119. Adaawen S. Understanding climate change and drought perceptions, impact and responses in the Rural Savannah, West Africa. Atmosphere (Basel). 2021;12(5):594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  120. Kannan S, Bessette DL, Abidoye B. Misalignment of perceptions with records and resources for responding to climate change risk. Front Clim. 2022;4:1038320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  121. Nguyen-Anh T, Nong D, Leu S, To-The N. Changes in the environment from perspectives of small-scale farmers in remote Vietnam. Reg Environ Change. 2021;21(4):98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  122. Torres B, Cayambe J, Paz S, Ayerve K, Heredia-R M, Torres E, et al. Livelihood capitals, income inequality, and the perception of climate change: a case study of small-scale cattle farmers in the Ecuadorian Andes. Sustainability. 2022;14(9):5028.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  123. Guodaar L, Bardsley DK, Suh J. Indigenous adaptation to climate change risks in northern Ghana. Clim Change. 2021;166(1–2):24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  124. Nef DP, Neneth D, Dini P, Abad CR, Kruetli P. How local communities attribute livelihood vulnerabilities to climate change and other causes: a case study in North Vanuatu. Clim Change. 2021;168(3–4):17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  125. Ensor JE, Abernethy KE, Hoddy ET, Aswani S, Albert S, Vaccaro I, et al. Variation in perception of environmental change in nine Solomon Islands communities: implications for securing fairness in community-based adaptation. Reg Environ Change. 2018;18(4):1131–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  126. Ambrosio-Albala P, Delgado-Serrano MM. Understanding climate change perception in community-based management contexts: perspectives of two indigenous communities. Weather, Clim Soc. 2018;10(3):471–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  127. Tiet T, To-The N, Nguyen-Anh T. Farmers’ behaviors and attitudes toward climate change adaptation: evidence from Vietnamese smallholder farmers. Environ Dev Sustain. 2022;26:14235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  128. Peng Y, Xu Z, Wei P, Cheng L. Smallholder farmers’ behavioral preferences under the impact of climate change: a comparative analysis of two agricultural areas in China. Front Earth Sci. 2022;10:1010733.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  129. Rasmus S, Wallen H, Turunen M, Landauer M, Tahkola J, Jokinen M, et al. Land-use and climate related drivers of change in the reindeer management system in Finland: Geography of perceptions. Appl Geogr. 2021;134: 102501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  130. Jellason N, Baines R, Conway J, Ogbaga C. Climate change perceptions and attitudes to smallholder adaptation in Northwestern Nigerian Drylands. Soc Sci. 2019;8(2):31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  131. Belay A, Oludhe C, Mirzabaev A, Recha JW, Berhane Z, Osano PM, et al. Knowledge of climate change and adaptation by smallholder farmers: evidence from southern Ethiopia. Heliyon. 2022;8(12): e12089.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  132. Sherpa SF, Shrestha M, Eakin H, Boone CG. Cryospheric hazards and risk perceptions in the Sagarmatha (Mt. Everest) National Park and Buffer Zone, Nepal. Nat Hazards. 2019;96(2):607–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  133. Addis Y, Abirdew S. Smallholder farmers’ perception of climate change and adaptation strategy choices in Central Ethiopia. Int J Clim Chang Strateg Manag. 2021;13(4/5):463–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  134. Singh NP, Anand B, Srivastava SK, Kumar NR, Sharma S, Bal SK, et al. Risk, perception and adaptation to climate change: evidence from arid region, India. Nat Hazards. 2022;112(2):1015–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  135. Al Mamun A, Roy S, Islam ART, Alam GMM, Alam E, Chandra Pal S, et al. Smallholder farmers’ perceived climate-related risk, impact, and their choices of sustainable adaptation strategies. Sustainability. 2021;13(21):11922.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  136. Megersa GG, Jaleta M, Tesfaye K, Getnet M, Tana T, Lakew B. Perceived climate change and determinants of adaptation responses by smallholder farmers in central Ethiopia. Sustainability. 2022;14(11):6590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  137. Budhathoki NK, Paton D, Lassa JA, Zander KK. Assessing farmers’ preparedness to cope with the impacts of multiple climate change-related hazards in the Terai lowlands of Nepal. Int J Disaster Risk Reduct. 2020;49: 101656.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  138. Poudyal NC, Joshi O, Hodges DG, Bhandari H, Bhattarai P. Climate change, risk perception, and protection motivation among high-altitude residents of the Mt. Everest region in Nepal. Ambio. 2021;50(2):505–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  139. Guáqueta-Solórzano VE, Postigo JC. Indigenous perceptions and adaptive responses to the impacts of climate variability in the Sierra Nevada de Santa Marta, Colombia. Front Clim. 2022;4: 910294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  140. Almudi T, Sinclair AJ. Extreme hydroclimatic events in rural communities of the Brazilian Amazon: local perceptions of change, impacts, and adaptation. Reg Environ Change. 2022;22(1):27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  141. Raihan ML, Basu M, Onitsuka K, Hoshino S. Determinants of farmers’ risk perceptions of hailstorms in Northern Bangladesh: is adaptive capacity the major concern? Pol J Environ Stud. 2021;31(1):257–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  142. Faisal M, Abbas A, Cai Y, Ali A, Shahzad MA, Akhtar S, et al. Perceptions, vulnerability and adaptation strategies for mitigating climate change effects among small livestock herders in Punjab, Pakistan. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(20):10771.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  143. Ahmed Z, Guha GS, Shew AM, Alam GMM. Climate change risk perceptions and agricultural adaptation strategies in vulnerable riverine char islands of Bangladesh. Land Use Policy. 2021;103: 105295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  144. Das S, Mishra AJ. Climate change and the Western Himalayan community: Exploring the local perspective through food choices. Ambio. 2022;12:534.

    Google Scholar 

  145. Baul TK, Peuly TA, Nandi R, Kar S, Karmakar S. Role of homestead forests in adaptation to climate change: a study on households’ perceptions and relevant factors in Bandarban Hill District, Bangladesh. Environ Manage. 2022;69(5):906–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  146. Moutouama FT, Tepa-Yotto GT, Agboton C, Gbaguidi B, Sekabira H, Tamò M. Farmers’ perception of climate change and climate-smart agriculture in Northern Benin, West Africa. Agronomy. 2022;12(6):1348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  147. Sen LTH, Bond J, Phuong LTH, Winkel A, Tran UC, van Le N. The importance of climate change awareness for the adaptive capacity of ethnic minority farmers in the mountainous areas of Thua Thien Hue province. Local Environ. 2021;26(2):239–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  148. Rapholo MT, Diko ML. Are smallholder farmers’ perceptions of climate variability supported by climatological evidence? Case study of a semi-arid region in South Africa. Int J Clim Chang Strateg Manag. 2020;12(5):571–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  149. Mihiretu A, Okoyo EN, Lemma T. Small holder farmers’ perception and response mechanisms to climate change: lesson from Tekeze lowland goat and sorghum livelihood zone, Ethiopia. Cogent Food Agric. 2020;6(1):1763647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  150. Tesfuhuney W, Mbeletshie E. Place-based perceptions, resilience and adaptation to climate change by smallholder farmers in rural South Africa. Int J Agric Res Innov Technol. 2021;10(2):116–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  151. Damtew A, Teferi E, Ongoma V, Mumo R, Esayas B. Spatiotemporal changes in mean and extreme climate: farmers’ perception and its agricultural implications in Awash River Basin, Ethiopia. Climate. 2022;10(6):89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  152. Landaverde R, Rodriguez MT, Niewoehner-Green J, Kitchel T, Chuquillanqui J. Climate change perceptions and adaptation strategies: a mixed methods study with subsistence farmers in Rural Peru. Sustainability. 2022;14(23):16015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  153. de Vasconcelos MA, dos Pereira SH, Lopes M, da Guimarães DFS. Impacts of climate change on the lives of riverine farmers on the Lower Rio Negro, Amazon. Atmosphere (Basel). 2022;13(11):1906.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  154. Rabbi SE, Shant R, Karmakar S, Habib A, Kropp JP. Regional mapping of climate variability index and identifying socio-economic factors influencing farmer’s perception in Bangladesh. Environ Dev Sustain. 2021;23(7):11050–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  155. Kangai R, Wemali Chitechi E, Koske J, Boaz W, Innocent N. Determinants of climate change adaptation and perceptions among small-scale farmers of Embu County, Eastern Kenya. Afr J Environ Sci Tech. 2021;15(4):167–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  156. Singh RK, Singh A, Kumar S, Sheoran P, Sharma DK, Stringer LC, et al. Perceived climate variability and compounding stressors: implications for risks to livelihoods of smallholder Indian farmers. Environ Manage. 2020;66(5):826–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  157. Etana D, Snelder DJRM, van Wesenbeeck CFA, de Cock BT. Trends of climate change and variability in three agro-ecological settings in Central Ethiopia: contrasts of meteorological data and farmers’ perceptions. Climate. 2020;8(11):121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  158. Funk C, Raghavan Sathyan A, Winker P, Breuer L. Changing climate - changing livelihood: smallholder’s perceptions and adaption strategies. J Environ Manage. 2020;259: 109702.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  159. Alfaro AA, Cortés ME. Perception of the impact of climate change on the quality of life and well-being of the inhabitants of the Cerro Blanco Agricultural Community, Limarí Province. Chile IDESIA (Chile). 2020;38(4):127–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  160. Zhang Q, Cui F, Dai L, Feng B, Lu Y, Tang H. Pastoralists’ perception of and adaptation strategies for climate change: associations with observed climate variability. Nat Hazards. 2019;96(3):1387–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  161. Scoville-Simonds M. Climate, the Earth, and God – Entangled narratives of cultural and climatic change in the Peruvian Andes. World Dev. 2018;110:345–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  162. Chepkoech W, Mungai NW, Stöber S, Bett HK, Lotze-Campen H. Farmers’ perspectives. Int J Clim Chang Strateg Manag. 2018;10(4):551–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  163. Limuwa M, Sitaula B, Njaya F, Storebakken T. Evaluation of small-scale fishers’ perceptions on climate change and their coping strategies: insights from Lake Malawi. Climate. 2018;6(2):34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  164. Röschel L, Graef F, Dietrich O, Schäfer M, Haase D. Individual local farmers’ perceptions of environmental change in Tanzania. Water (Basel). 2018;10(4):525.

    Google Scholar 

  165. Kiumbuku SK, Baaru MW, Mutinda JW. Do smallholder farmers perceive rainfall variability the same and correctly? Gendered and spatial analysis of perception versus actual trends of rainfall in three livelihood zones in Kenya. J Environ Earth Sci. 2018;8(9):1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  166. Diarra FB, Ouédraogo M, Zougmoré RB, Partey ST, Houessionon P, Mensah A. Are perception and adaptation to climate variability and change of cowpea growers in Mali gender differentiated? Environ Dev Sustain. 2021;23(9):13854–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  167. Martínez-Herrera G, Trejo I, Moreno-Calles AI, de Alba-Navarro MF, Martínez-Ballesté A. Knowing the Clouds through the Land: perceptions of changes in climate through agricultural practices in two Nahua indigenous communities. J Ethnobiol. 2021;41(3):349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  168. Sereenonchai S, Arunrat N. Fishers’ decisions to adopt adaptation strategies and expectations for their children to pursue the same profession in Chumphon Province, Thailand. Climate. 2019;7(2):34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  169. Shukla R, Agarwal A, Sachdeva K, Kurths J, Joshi PK. Climate change perception: an analysis of climate change and risk perceptions among farmer types of Indian Western Himalayas. Clim Change. 2019;152(1):103–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  170. Kahsay HT, Guta DD, Birhanu BS, Gidey TG. Farmers’ perceptions of climate change trends and adaptation strategies in semiarid highlands of Eastern Tigray, Northern Ethiopia. Adv Meteorol. 2019;2019:1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  171. Córdoba Vargas CA, Hortúa Romero S, León-Sicard T. Resilience to climate variability: the role of perceptions and traditional knowledge in the Colombian Andes. Agroecol Sustain Food Syst. 2020;44(4):419–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  172. Rankoana SA. Human perception of climate change. Weather. 2018;73(11):367–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  173. Nyangoko BP, Berg H, Mangora MM, Shalli MS, Gullström M. Community perceptions of climate change and ecosystem-based adaptation in the mangrove ecosystem of the Rufiji Delta, Tanzania. Clim Dev. 2022;14:1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  174. Pimid M, Nasir MRM, Scian J, Ahmad AG, Abdul Mutalib AH, Perijin J. Indigenous Kinabatangan perspectives on climate change impacts and adaptations: factors influencing their support and participation. Sustainability. 2022;14(11):6459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  175. Ojo TO, Baiyegunhi LJS. Climate change perception and its impact on net farm income of smallholder rice farmers in South-West, Nigeria. J Clean Prod. 2021;310:1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  176. Manh NT, Ahmad MM. Indigenous farmers’ perception of climate change and the use of local knowledge to adapt to climate variability: a case study of Vietnam. J Int Dev. 2021;33(7):1189–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  177. Manh NT, Ahmad MM. Retracted: Ethnic minority farmers’ perceptions and use of local knowledge to adapt to climate change: some insights from Vietnam. Singap J Trop Geogr. 2021;42(3):397–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  178. Nnko HJ, Gwakisa PS, Ngonyoka A, Estes A. Climate change and variability perceptions and adaptations of pastoralists’ communities in the Maasai Steppe, Tanzania. J Arid Environ. 2021;185: 104337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  179. Chaudhary BR, Acciaioli G, Erskine W, Chaudhary P. Responses of the Tharu to climate change-related hazards in the water sector: indigenous perceptions, vulnerability and adaptations in the western Tarai of Nepal. Clim Dev. 2021;13(9):816–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  180. Tume SJP, Kimengsi JN, Fogwe ZN. Indigenous knowledge and farmer perceptions of climate and ecological changes in the Bamenda Highlands of Cameroon: insights from the Bui Plateau. Climate. 2019;7(12):138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  181. Ezeh J, Madukwe E, Ezeh C. Associating farmers’ perception of climate change and variability with historical climate data. J Agric Sci Belgrade. 2022;67(3):299–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  182. Jamshidi O, Asadi A, Kalantari K, Azadi H, Scheffran J. Vulnerability to climate change of smallholder farmers in the Hamadan province, Iran. Clim Risk Manag. 2019;23:146–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  183. Ramírez KDI, Ibarra AMA. Less rain and more heat”: smallholders’ perception and climate change adaptation strategies in tropical environments. Sociedad y Ambiente. 2019;21:77–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  184. Roy D, Kowsari MS, Nath TD, Taiyebi KA, Rashid MM. Smallholder farmers’ perception to climate change impact on crop production: case from drought prone areas of Bangladesh. Int J Agric Technol. 2018;14(7):1813–28.

    Google Scholar 

  185. Escarcha JF, Lassa JA, Palacpac EP, Zander KK. Understanding climate change impacts on water buffalo production through farmers’ perceptions. Clim Risk Manag. 2018;20:50–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  186. Pandey R, Kumar P, Archie KM, Gupta AK, Joshi PK, Valente D, et al. Climate change adaptation in the western-Himalayas: household level perspectives on impacts and barriers. Ecol Indic. 2018;84:27–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  187. Consciousness AJ. In: Braisby N, Gellatly A, editors. Cognitive psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2012. p. 577–605.

    Google Scholar 

  188. Triantafyllidou E, Zabaniotou A. From theory to praxis: ‘go sustainable living’ survey for exploring individuals consciousness level of decision-making and action-taking in daily life towards a green citizenship. Circ Econ Sustain. 2022;2(1):113–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  189. Jiménez Sánchez M, Lafuente R. Defining and measuring environmental consciousness. Rev Int Sociol. 2010;68(3):731–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  190. Neupane M, Joshi R, Bhandari D, Awasthi N. Perception of transhumant herders on climate change and their adaptation strategies. Our Nature. 2022;20(1):1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  191. Paudel D, Tiwari KR, Raut N, Bajracharya RM, Bhattarai S, Sitaula BK, et al. What affects farmers in choosing better agroforestry practice as a strategy of climate change adaptation? An experience from the mid-hills of Nepal. Heliyon. 2022;8(6): e09695.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  192. Twecan D, Wang W, Xu J, Mohmmed A. Climate change vulnerability, adaptation measures, and risk perceptions at households level in Acholi sub-region, Northern Uganda. Land Use Policy. 2022;115: 106011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  193. Dawit M, Dinka MO, Halefom A. Farmers’ perception of climate change and gender sensitive perspective for optimised irrigation in a compound surface-ground water system. J Water Land Dev. 2022;52:265–71.

    Google Scholar 

  194. Baffour-Ata F, Antwi-Agyei P, Apawu GO, Nkiaka E, Amoah EA, Akorli R, et al. Using traditional agroecological knowledge to adapt to climate change and variability in the Upper East Region of Ghana. Environ Chall. 2021;4: 100205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  195. Sorgho R, Mank I, Kagoné M, Souares A, Danquah I, Sauerborn R. “We Will Always Ask Ourselves the Question of How to Feed the Family”: subsistence farmers’ perceptions on adaptation to climate change in Burkina Faso. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(19):7200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  196. Ado AM, Leshan J, Savadogo P, Bo L, Shah AA. Farmers’ awareness and perception of climate change impacts: case study of Aguie district in Niger. Environ Dev Sustain. 2019;21(6):2963–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  197. Sharif J, Rafiq MK, Rafiq MT, Aziz R, Qayyum A, Saleem AR, et al. Climate change perceptions and adaptative actions by pastoral community of the Tibetan Plateau, China. Appl Ecol Environ Res. 2019;17(4).

  198. Pondorfer A. The perception of climate change: comparative evidence from the small-island societies of Bougainville and Palawan. Environ Dev. 2019;30:21–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  199. Lone FA, Maheen M, Ul Shafiq M, Bhat MS, Rather JA. Farmer’s perception and adaptation strategies to changing climate in Kashmir Himalayas, India. GeoJournal. 2022;87(3):1743–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  200. Eitzinger A, Binder CR, Meyer MA. Risk perception and decision-making: do farmers consider risks from climate change? Clim Change. 2018;151(3–4):507–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  201. Makuvaro V, Walker S, Masere TP, Dimes J. Smallholder farmer perceived effects of climate change on agricultural productivity and adaptation strategies. J Arid Environ. 2018;152:75–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  202. Das U, Ansari MA, Ghosh S. Does climate knowledge act as a shield for farm livelihoods? Empirical analysis from the coastal and non-coastal ecosystems of India. Theor Appl Climatol. 2022;150(3–4):1627–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  203. Islam ARMT, Hasanuzzaman M, Jaman M, Alam E, Mallick J, Alam GMM, et al. Assessing farmers’ typologies of perception for adopting sustainable adaptation strategies in Bangladesh. Climate. 2021;9(12):167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  204. Quandt A. Variability in perceptions of household livelihood resilience and drought at the intersection of gender and ethnicity. Clim Change. 2019;152(1):1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  205. Makondo CC, Thomas DSG. Climate change adaptation: linking indigenous knowledge with western science for effective adaptation. Environ Sci Policy. 2018;88:83–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  206. Khanal U, Wilson C, Lee BL, Hoang VN. Climate change adaptation strategies and food productivity in Nepal: a counterfactual analysis. Clim Change. 2018;148(4):575–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  207. Lemahieu A, Scott L, Malherbe WS, Mahatante PT, Randrianarimanana JV, Aswani S. Local perceptions of environmental changes in fishing communities of southwest Madagascar. Ocean Coast Manag. 2018;163:209–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  208. Salite D. Explaining the uncertainty: understanding small-scale farmers’ cultural beliefs and reasoning of drought causes in Gaza Province, Southwest Mozambique. Agric Human Values. 2019;36(3):427–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  209. N’gombe JN, Tembo MC, Masasi B. “Are They Aware, and Why?” Bayesian analysis of predictors of smallholder farmers’ awareness of climate change and its risks to agriculture. Agronomy. 2020;10(3):376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  210. Sarker MNI, Wu M, Alam GMM, Shouse RC. RETRACTED: Life in riverine islands in Bangladesh: Local adaptation strategies of climate vulnerable riverine island dwellers for livelihood resilience. Land Use Policy. 2020;94: 104574.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  211. Alam E, Elahee Hridoy AE, Naim M. Factors affecting small-scale fishers adaptation toward the impacts of climate change: reflections from South Eastern Bangladeshi Fishers. Int Energy J. 2021;21:119–32.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank, for useful comments and suggestions on previous drafts: A.B. Junqueira; B. Funatsu; C. Adams; G. M. Di Giulio and, R. A. Sanches.

Funding

This study was partially financed by a studentship to Troncarelli from the Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior – Brazil (CAPES) (Finance Code 001) and grants to Morsello from Incline at University of São Paulo (Climate-U project) and the São Paulo Research Foundation (FAPESP Project #2016/12803-9).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

LTT and CM delineated the study and contributed to the writing and revisions of the article drafts. LTT and MTA contributed with the data tabulation, while LTT analyzed the data. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lia Taruiap Troncarelli.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

ROSES checklist of systematic map protocol.

Additional file 2.

Codebook—Description of the data that will be extracted in the systematic map.

Additional file 3.

Record of all articles.

Additional file 4.

Complete list of explicit definitions of perception.

Additional file 5.

Complete reference list of the articles selected for full reading.

Additional file 6.

Complete list of articles excluded in the title and abstract screening phase and the reasons for exclusion.

Additional file 7.

Data extracted from 361 articles after reading in full.

Additional file 8.

Percentage of thematic areas covered in the 361 articles.

Additional file 9.

Information on the theories presented in the articles.

Additional file 10.

Information about constructs (n = 361).

Additional file 11.

Constructs by geographical distribution.

Additional file 12.

Complete list of the description of how perception shows up in results.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Troncarelli, L.T., de Ataide, M.T. & Morsello, C. Existing evidence of conceptual differences in research on climate change perceptions among smallholders? A systematic map. Environ Evid 12, 28 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13750-023-00321-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13750-023-00321-2

Keywords