Skip to main content

Validity across four common street-crossing distraction indicators to predict pedestrian safety

Abstract

Background

Multiple distraction indicators have been applied to measure street-crossing distraction but their validities in predicting pedestrian safety are poorly understood.

Methods

Based on a video-based observational study, we compared the validity of four commonly used distraction indicators (total duration of distraction while crossing a street, proportion of distracted time over total street-crossing time, duration of the longest distraction time, and total number of distractions) in predicting three pedestrian safety outcomes (near-crash incidence, frequency of looking left and right, and speed crossing the street) across three types of distraction (mobile phone use, talking to other pedestrians, eating/drinking/smoking). Change in Harrell’s C statistic was calculated to assess the validity of each distraction indicator based on multivariable regression models including only covariates and including both covariates and the distraction indicator.

Results

Heterogeneous capacities in predicting the three safety outcomes across the four distraction indicators were observed: 1) duration of the longest distraction time was most predictive for the occurrence of near-crashes and looks left and right among pedestrians with all three types of distraction combined and talking with other pedestrians (Harrell’s C statistic changes ranged from 0.0310 to 0.0335, P < 0.05), and for the occurrence of near-crashes for pedestrians involving mobile phone use (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0053); 2) total duration of distraction was most predictive for speed crossing the street among pedestrians with the combination and each of the three types of distraction (Harrell’s C statistic changes ranged from 0.0037 to 0.0111, P < 0.05), frequency of looking left and right among pedestrians distracted by mobile phone use (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0115), and the occurrence of near-crash among pedestrians eating, drinking, or smoking (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0119); and 3) the total number of distractions was the most predictive indicator of frequency of looking left and right among pedestrians eating, drinking, or smoking (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0013). Sensitivity analyses showed the results were robust to change in grouping criteria of the four distraction indicators.

Conclusions

Future research should consider the pedestrian safety outcomes and type of distractions to select the best distraction indicator.

Peer Review reports

Background

As mobile phone use has increased worldwide, distracted pedestrian behavior while crossing the street has also become more and more common. The reported prevalence of street-crossing distraction ranges from 6.4% to 43.2% among pedestrians globally, with rates varying across location and time period [1,2,3]. The number of injuries reported to be related to crossing streets while being distracted has also risen dramatically in many countries. For example, the number of pedestrians in the United States known to be killed by distraction from portable electronic devices, talking, or eating increased from 97 to 276 between 2010 and 2019 [4].

To develop strategies for prevention of distracted pedestrian behavior and related crashes and injuries, valid epidemiological indicators are important. These indicators should quantify the extent of street-crossing distraction and allow researchers to examine associations between pedestrian distraction and injury outcomes. However, the current literature lacks validation of pedestrian-specific distraction indicators [3].

The existing literature does include multiple indicators that are currently used in distracted driving research [5,6,7]; they are based on the type, duration, and frequency of street-crossing distraction. Several indicators have been empirically demonstrated to exhibit significant correlations with driving performance and the likelihood of crashes or near-crashes in naturalistic driving studies [6, 7]. However, the comparative validity of these indicators has not been examined through rigorous empirical study. It remains unclear if and how these distraction indicators may correlate with pedestrian safety outcomes or whether a single indicator is most predictive of risk.

We conducted a large video-based observational study to compare the validity of four common street-crossing distraction indicators for predicting pedestrian safety. Specially, we tested three hypotheses: (a) longer distraction time leads to near-crashes and failure to look at traffic and therefore is a predictive indicator of distraction for those two pedestrian safety outcomes, (b) total distraction time causes people to walk more slowly and therefore is a predictive distraction indicator for speed crossing the street, and (c) a larger number of distractions causes pedestrian to look less at traffic and is a predictive distraction indicator for frequency of looking left and right.

Methods

Study design

Data collected for a large video-based observational study were used to conduct a secondary comparative analysis assessing the validity of four common distraction indicators to predict pedestrian safety (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flowchart of the study design. Note: The definitions or classifications of collected data were described in the street-crossing distraction indicators, pedestrian safety outcomes, and covariates section

Data source

Data were collected in 2019 at 20 “╋” type road intersections in Changsha, China (Trial registration number: ChiCTR1900023791) [8]. Overall, 240 h of videotaped street-crossing behaviors were collected. Detailed information of the study design appears in Supplemental Appendix A.

Because pedestrians younger than age 20 years accounted for less than 10% of all pedestrians and it is difficult to obtain informed consent for research from adult caregivers of young children in field observations [9], our analysis focused on adult pedestrians appearing to be aged 20 years and older.

Street-crossing distraction indicators

As defined below, four commonly-used indicators of street-crossing distraction were considered [5,6,7, 10]. Researchers flagged the videos to identify the starting and ending time of each distraction that a pedestrian displayed while crossing the street, allowing us to determine the full temporal duration of distraction.

  1. a.

    Total duration of distraction while crossing a street: Total duration of time the pedestrian was distracted while crossing the street, in seconds.

  2. b.

    Proportion of distracted time over total street-crossing time: Proportion of distracted time over total duration of time crossing the street.

  3. c.

    Duration of the longest distraction time: Duration of the longest distraction the pedestrian displays while crossing the street, in seconds.

  4. d.

    Total number of distractions: Number of distractions the pedestrian displays while crossing the street.

We also considered the type of distraction, which was coded as mobile phone use, talking to other pedestrians, eating, drinking, or smoking. The latter three categories were merged into a single category due to small sample sizes.

In total, 25,436 adult pedestrians were observed. Of those, 34.3% (n = 8,729) were undistracted while crossing, 32.8% (n = 8,347) displayed one type of distraction, and 1.5% (n = 384) displayed multiple types of distraction (Supplemental Table B1).

The indicators a-c were categorized into five groups in fitting multivariable models based on the quartiles of sample distributions (no distraction = 0; P0.1 ~ P25 = 1; P25.1 ~ P50 = 2; P50.1 ~ P75 = 3; > P75 = 4) by type of distraction; the indicator d was categorized into four groups based on distribution (no distraction = 0; distracted one time = 1; two times = 2; three or more times = 3).

Pedestrian safety outcomes

Following previous research, we considered three pedestrian safety outcome measures:

  1. a.

    Near-crash incidence [11]: A near-crash event was defined as any circumstance that required a rapid, evasive maneuver by either the pedestrian or any motor or non-motor vehicle (or both) to avoid a pedestrian-vehicle crash. Evasive maneuvers for pedestrians included physically-obvious changes in direction, stopping, or running forward [11]. The near-crash outcome was transformed into a binary variable, having a near-crash event or not. Near-crash incidence was calculated as “the number of pedestrians encountering a near-crash event / total number of observed pedestrians × 100%”. We observed no actual crashes.

  2. b.

    Frequency of looking left and right [12, 13]: Frequency of looking left and right was defined as the number times a pedestrian distinctly moved their head left or right while crossing the street. A much low frequency of looking left and right generally indicates a longer distraction period, as distracted pedestrians divert their gaze away from the road and experience diminished awareness of their surroundings. This behavior reduces safety while crossing the street [13, 14].

  3. c.

    Speed crossing the street [13]: Speed crossing the street was defined as the average distance a pedestrian walked per 10 s while crossing the street. Lower speeds while crossing the street generally lead to extended time duration on the road, increased exposure to traffic and therefore an elevated risk for crashes [14, 15].

Data for the three safety measures were manually transcribed from the videos by trained researchers (Supplemental Appendix A).

Covariates

Based on their reported relevance in previous research [16, 17], we included the following nine covariates in analyses: sex (male vs. female), estimated age group (20–39 years, 40–59 years, or ≥ 60 years), time of day (morning vs. afternoon), day of the week (weekday vs. weekend), violating red light signal (yes vs. no), road width (< 22 m, 22–31 m, and ≥ 32 m, based on the tertiles of P33.3 and P66.7), vehicle speed limit displayed prominently at intersection, median barrier present on the road, and refuge island present. Descriptive data on the covariates appear in Supplemental Tables B1 and B2.

Reliability of data transcription

Pilot research demonstrated high reliability between manually transcribed data of videos and data collected through face-to-face interviews with 300 pedestrians for sex (99.7%) and age group (93.0%). Re-transcription of 20% randomly selected videos (48 of 240 h) showed high reproducibility consistency (93.1%) for all study variables combined.

Statistical analysis

The Cochran-Armitage trend test and Spearman rank correlation analysis were used to examine correlations between the three safety outcome measures and between the four distraction indicators.

Following previous research [18, 19], we adopted the change of Harrell’s C statistic in multivariate regression models to assess the predictive validity of each distraction indicator. Models were constructed including the nine covariates mentioned above versus including both the nine covariates and the street-crossing distraction indicator. The link functions of the multivariate regression models were linear functions for average speed crossing the street, quasi-Poisson functions for frequency of looking left and right (offset by total duration of time crossing the street), and logistic functions for near-crash events. Harrell’s C statistic is essentially a rank-correlation measure that represents the capacity of discrimination [20]. Harrell’s C statistic is also referred to as the estimated area under the Receiver Operating Characteristics curve quantified for binary outcomes [21]. 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) of Harrell’s C statistic change were estimated using bootstrapped standard errors with 1000 replications [22, 23]. Detailed groupings of the four distraction indicators are shown in Supplemental Table B3.

Subgroup analyses were performed by type of distraction. Due to small sample sizes, analyses for multiple types of street-crossing distractions were omitted. The variance inflation factor for all independent variables in all fitted models ranged from 1.00 to 3.17, suggesting the absence of substantial collinearity between regressors.

We performed sensitivity analysis to assess the stability of results across different groupings of the four distraction indicators. To do this, we used median, tertiles, and quintiles respectively to classify pedestrians for distraction indicators a-c and used different combination schemes to group pedestrians for distraction indicator d, as shown in Supplemental Table B4.

All statistical analyses were performed using SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute) and R 4.1.2. A p-value of less than 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

Sample characteristics

In total, we observed street-crossing by 25,436 pedestrians estimated to be aged 20 years and older, including 10,886 males (42.8%) and 14,550 females (57.2%). 15,111, 7,741, and 2,584 pedestrians were estimated to be aged 20–39 years (59.4%), aged 40–59 years (30.4%), and aged 60 years and older (10.2%), respectively (Supplemental Table B1). Of them, 2,814 (19.6%), 4,991 (11.0%), and 542 (2.1%) were distracted by mobile phone use, talking with other pedestrians, and eating, drinking, or smoking, respectively. Sample data for all four distraction indicators and two of the pedestrian safety measures demonstrated positively skewed distributions (Figs. 2 and 3).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Distribution of the four distraction indicators among 25,436 videotaped pedestrians

Fig. 3
figure 3

Distribution of the three pedestrian safety measures among 25,436 videotaped pedestrians

Correlations between distraction indicators and pedestrian safety measures

The four distraction indicators were all slightly or moderately correlated with the three pedestrian safety measures (p < 0.05, Supplemental Figs. B1-B12), except for insignificant correlations between (a) speed crossing the street for all walking distractions combined with the proportion of distracted time and the total number of distractions (rs = -0.01, p = 0.32, and rs = -0.01, p = 0.12, respectively) (Supplemental Fig. B3), (b) total number of distractions with near-crash incidence for mobile phone use (Z = 1.51, p = 0.13, Supplemental Fig. B4), and (c) all four distraction indicators with frequency of looking left and right for eating, drinking, or smoking (p > 0.05, Supplemental Fig. B11).

Predictive validity of the four distraction indicators

All distractions

For all distractions combined, the four distraction indicators were significantly predictive for all three pedestrian safety outcomes (p < 0.05, Fig. 4 and Supplemental Table B5). All four distraction indicators showed the largest predictive validity for near-crash incidence (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0274–0.0315) and the smallest predictive validity for speed crossing the street (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0033–0.0111).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Validity of the four distraction indicators for street-crossing safety among all distracted pedestrians. Abbreviations: TDD – total duration of distraction while crossing a street, PDT – proportion of distracted time over total street-crossing time, DLD – duration of the longest distraction time, and TND – total number of distractions. The TDD, PDT, and DLD were grouped by quartile; TND were grouped as no distraction, distracted one time, distracted two times, and distracted three or more times (Supplemental Table B4)

Of the four distraction indicators, duration of the longest distraction time was most predictive for near-crash incidence (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0315, 95% CI: 0.0137–0.0508) and frequency of looking left and right (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0310, 95% CI: 0.0254–0.0373), while total duration of distraction was most predictive for speed crossing the street (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0111, 95% CI: 0.0068–0.0146) (Fig. 4 and Supplemental Table B5).

Because the predictive validity of the four distraction indicators varied across the type of distraction, we considered subgroup analysis by type of distraction next.

Mobile phone use

When focused only on mobile phone use instead of all distractions, the four street-crossing distraction indicators were all significantly predictive of the three pedestrian safety outcomes (p < 0.05, Fig. 5A-C and Supplemental Table B6), except for the total number of distractions predicting incidence of near-crash and speed crossing the street, and duration of the longest distraction time for predicting speed crossing the street (p > 0.05). All four distraction indicators displayed the greatest predictive validity for frequency of looking left and right (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0103–0.0115) but the least predictive validity for speed crossing the street (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0010–0.0041).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Validity of the four distraction indicators for street-crossing safety by type of distraction. Abbreviations: TDD – total duration of distraction while crossing a street, PDT – proportion of distracted time over total street-crossing time, DLD – duration of the longest distraction time, and TND – total number of distractions. The TDD, PDT, and DLD were grouped by quartile; TND were grouped as no distraction, distracted one time, distracted two times, and distracted three or more times (Supplemental Table B4)

Of the four distraction indicators, duration of the longest distraction time by mobile phone use was most predictive of near-crashes (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0053, 95% CI: 0.0007–0.0202), while total duration of distraction by mobile phone was most predictive of frequency of looking left and right and speed crossing the street (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0115, 95% CI: 0.0081–0.0158; and 0.0041, 95% CI: 0.0013–0.0070) (Fig. 5A-C and Supplemental Table B6).

Talking with other pedestrians

When focused only on distraction talking with other pedestrians, the four street-crossing distraction indicators were all significantly predictive of the three pedestrian safety measures (p < 0.05, Fig. 5D-F and Supplemental Table B7), except for the total number of distractions for predicting speed crossing the street (p > 0.05). All four distraction indicators showed the largest predictive validity for near-crash incidence (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0306–0.0335) and the least predictive validity for predicting speed crossing the street (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0022–0.0077).

Duration of the longest distraction time was most predictive of near-crash incidence (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0335, 95% CI: 0.0140–0.0543) and frequency of looking left and right (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0334, 95% CI: 0.0266–0.0403), while total duration of distraction talking to other pedestrians offered the best predictive validity of speed crossing the street (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0077, 95% CI: 0.0045–0.0111) (Fig. 5D-F and Supplemental Table B7).

Eating, drinking, or smoking

When distraction was limited to eating, drinking, or smoking, the four street-crossing distraction indicators were all significantly predictive of the three pedestrian safety outcomes (p < 0.05, Fig. 5G-I and Supplemental Table B8). All four distraction indicators displayed the largest predictive validity for near-crash incidence (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0103–0.0119) and the least predictive validity of frequency of looking left and right (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0008–0.0013).

Of the four distraction indicators, total duration of distraction had the largest predictive validity for near-crash incidence (Harrell’s C statistic changes: 0.0119, 95% CI: 0.0029–0.0263) and speed crossing the street (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0037, 95% CI: 0.0012–0.0064), while total number of distractions had the largest predictive validity of frequency of looking left and right (Harrell’s C statistic change: 0.0013, 95% CI: 0.0002–0.0034) (Fig. 5G-I and Supplemental Table B8).

Sensitivity analyses

Sensitivity analyses were conducted by changing the groupings of the four distraction indicators and showed robust results to the changes (Supplemental Tables B5-B8).

Discussion

Primary findings

Using observational strategies with a large sample of data recorded at Chinese intersections, this study compared the predictive validity of four commonly used street-crossing distraction indicators for three street-crossing safety outcomes. All four distraction indicators – total duration of distraction, proportion of distracted time, duration of the longest distraction time, and total number of distractions – were generally predictive of the three pedestrian safety outcomes – near-crash incidence, frequency of looking left and right, and speed crossing the street. However, the indicators showed heterogeneous predictive capacities for different pedestrian safety outcomes and type of distraction (Table 1).

Table 1 Street-crossing distraction indicators most predictive for three street-crossing safety outcomes

Duration of the longest distraction time was most predictive of the occurrence of near-crashes and the frequency of looking left and right among pedestrians with all types of distraction combined and for talking with other pedestrians, and for the occurrence of near-crashes among pedestrians distracted by mobile phone use. Total duration of distraction was the best indicator for predicting (a) speed crossing the street outcomes among pedestrians with all three types of distraction, (b) frequency of looking left and right among pedestrians distracted by mobile phone use, and (c) near-crash incidence among pedestrians distracted by eating, drinking, or smoking. Finally, total number of distractions was the most predictive indicator of frequency of looking left and right among pedestrians distracted by eating, drinking, or smoking.

Interpretation of findings

As distracted pedestrian behavior increases globally and leads to increasing pedestrian injury and death rates, researchers will benefit from an understanding of the predictive validity of street-crossing distraction indicators for street-crossing safety. Previous research in this domain is scarce; just one published study in the driving literature examined associations between crashes or near-crashes with two driving distraction indicators, the driver’s single longest glance off the roadway and the total duration of eyes off the roadway [6]. That study found that the risk of crash increased with the duration of the single longest glance in all secondary tasks and wireless secondary task engagement, but it focused only on newly licensed teenage drivers and relied on a single safety outcome measure.

The heterogeneous predictive validities of the four distraction indicators we discovered are logical in theory, as the indicators measure different aspects of distraction involving different cognitive and perceptual distraction processes [11]. For example, talking with other pedestrians is likely to significantly affect cognitive and aural attention but only minimally affect visual attention. Eating, drinking, and smoking may influence kinetic movements and perhaps have some cognitive impact, but their impact on visual or aural attention could be minimal [5]. Mobile phone use distracts pedestrians in multiple ways simultaneously, having cognitive, kinetic, visual and sometimes aural components that can impact pedestrian safety [11].

One could also consider our results from the perspective of pedestrian outcomes. Frequency of looking left and right is largely a visual task, though it impacts efficient cognitive processing of the perceived traffic environment. It was best predicted by the duration of the longest distraction time and by the total duration of distraction for the more visually distracting situations, including mobile phone use and talking with other pedestrians. While frequency of looking left and right was better predicted by the total number of distractions for eating, drinking, or smoking that involves fewer visual distractions. Speed crossing the street is driven primarily by cognitive distraction, which impacts gait speed [24, 25]. It was best predicted by the total number of distractions.

Our findings provide valuable evidence to support targeted selection of street-crossing distraction indicators in scientific research and prevention practice. The results will help guide researchers and practitioners to select optimal street-crossing distraction indicators that quantify the extent of street-crossing distraction in particular circumstances and for particular goals. With greater precision in the design of both observational and experimental research, researchers can better develop and evaluate effective intervention programs to reduce distracted pedestrian behavior and resultant pedestrian injuries and deaths.

Limitations

This study has several limitations. First, despite the large sample size, we did not capture any pedestrian-related crashes or injuries to use as outcome measures. Instead, we relied on common proxy measures like near-crash events [26] that may not perfectly represent prediction of crash or injury outcomes. Second, our analysis was based on videos taken at 20 “╋” type road intersections and among pedestrians appearing to be aged 20 years and older. Considering the highly similar road traffic environments and pedestrian safety culture across China, results would likely generalize to other Chinese locations but may not generalize to other countries or cultures, to mid-block crossing locations, or to pedestrian behavior by children or adolescents.

Third, we relied on observational coding strategies that can generate bias based on researcher judgment. However, such methodologies are widely used [16, 17] and we employed standard strategies to establish reliability and validity of our data. Fourth, a lower frequency of looking left and right and a slower speed crossing the street generally indicate reduced surroundings awareness, increase exposure to traffic, and consequently threaten pedestrian safety. However, there are some exceptions, like stopping in the middle of the crosswalk to let a car pass while crossing a wide street. To control for these exceptional circumstances, we included red light signal violation in multivariable models as a covariate. Last, due to absence of relevant injury data (i.e., injury morbidity and mortality), our recommendations should be used with caution when using fatal and non-fatal pedestrian injuries as safety outcomes. Further studies are needed to generate relevant evidence.

Conclusion

In conclusion, four common distraction indicators showed different validity in predicting three street-crossing safety outcomes of pedestrians. To improve scientific rigor in future research and practice initiatives, we recommend selection of street-crossing distraction indicators that consider the pedestrian safety outcomes of interest and the types of walking distraction to be studied.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

TDD:

Total duration of distraction while crossing a street

PDT:

Proportion of distracted time over total street-crossing time

DLD:

Duration of the longest distraction time

TND:

Total number of distractions

References

  1. Hamann C, Dulf D, Baragan-Andrada E, Price M, Peek-Asa C. Contributors to pedestrian distraction and risky behaviours during road crossings in Romania. Inj Prev. 2017;23(6):370–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Nasar J, Hecht P, Wener R. Mobile telephones, distracted attention, and pedestrian safety. Accid Anal Prev. 2008;40(1):69–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Ning PS, Hu GQ. Progress on epidemiological characteristics and interventions of pedestrian distraction. Zhonghua Liu Xing Bing Xue Za Zhi. 2022;43(2):277–81. Chinese.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. Fatality Analysis Reporting System (FARS). https://www-fars.nhtsa.dot.gov/People/PeoplePedestrians.aspx. Accessed 24 Aug 2023.

  5. Regan MA, Lee JD, Young KL. Driver distraction: theory, effects, and mitigation. Florida: CRC Press; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Simons-Morton BG, Guo F, Klauer SG, Ehsani JP, Pradhan AK. Keep your eyes on the road: young driver crash risk increases according to duration of distraction. J Adolesc Health. 2014;54(Suppl 5):61–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Liang Y, Lee JD, Yekhshatyan L. How dangerous is looking away from the road? Algorithms predict crash risk from glance patterns in naturalistic driving. Hum Factors. 2012;54(6):1104–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Ning P, Zong H, Li L, Cheng P, Schwebel DC, Yang Y, et al. Effectiveness of a helmet promotion campaign, China. Bull World Health Organ. 2022;100(5):329–36.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Zhang W, Wang K, Wang L, Feng Z, Du Y. Exploring factors affecting pedestrians’ red-light running behaviors at intersections in China. Accid Anal Prev. 2016;96:71–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Stutts J, Feaganes J, Reinfurt D, Rodgman E, Hamlett C, Gish K, et al. Driver’s exposure to distractions in their natural driving environment. Accid Anal Prev. 2005;37(6):1093–101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Lee SE, Simons-Morton BG, Klauer SE, Ouimet MC, Dingus TA. Naturalistic assessment of novice teenage crash experience. Accid Anal Prev. 2011;43(4):1472–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Byington KW, Schwebel DC. Effects of mobile Internet use on college student pedestrian injury risk. Accid Anal Prev. 2013;51:78–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Thompson LL, Rivara FP, Ayyagari RC, Ebel BE. Impact of social and technological distraction on pedestrian crossing behaviour: an observational study. Inj Prev. 2013;19(4):232–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Zhang X, Li Q, Gao P, Zhu J, Tuo H, Lin Q, Jing F, Liu W. The effect of mobile phone task and age on gait: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Physiol. 2023;14:1163655.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Russo BJ, James E, Aguilar CY, Smaglik EJ. Pedestrian behavior at signalized intersection crosswalks: observational study of factors associated with distracted walking, pedestrian violations, and walking speed. Transp Res Rec. 2018;2672(35):1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Gao Y, Schwebel DC, Zhang L, Xiao W, Hu G. Unsafe bicycling behavior in Changsha, China: a video-based observational study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(9):3256.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Yan F, Li B, Zhang W, Hu G. Red-light running rates at five intersections by road user in Changsha, China: an observational study. Accid Anal Prev. 2016;95(Pt B):381–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Aguayo GA, Vaillant MT, Donneau AF, Schritz A, Stranges S, Malisoux L, et al. Comparative analysis of the association between 35 frailty scores and cardiovascular events, cancer, and total mortality in an elderly general population in England: an observational study. PLoS Med. 2018;15(3):e1002543.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Wu J, Zhang H, Li L, Hu M, Chen L, Xu B, et al. A nomogram for predicting overall survival in patients with low-grade endometrial stromal sarcoma: a population-based analysis. Cancer Commun (Lond). 2020;40(7):301–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Uno H, Cai T, Pencina MJ, D’Agostino RB, Wei LJ. On the C-statistics for evaluating overall adequacy of risk prediction procedures with censored survival data. Stat Med. 2011;30(10):1105–17.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Bamber D. The area above the ordinal dominance graph and the area below the receiver operating characteristic graph. J Math Psychol. 1975;12:387–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Chernick MR. Bootstrap methods; a practitioner’s guide. New York: Wiley; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Jena AB, Farid M, Blumenthal D, Bhattacharya J. Association of residency work hour reform with long term quality and costs of care of US physicians: observational study. BMJ. 2019;366:l4134.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Oh C, LaPointe LL. Changes in cognitive load and effects on parameters of gait. Cogent Psychol. 2017;4(1):1372872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Licence S, Smith R, McGuigan MP, Earnest CP. Gait pattern alterations during walking, texting and walking and texting during cognitively distractive tasks while negotiating common pedestrian obstacles. PLoS One. 2015;10(7):e0133281.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Wu KF, Jovanis PP. Defining and screening crash surrogate events using naturalistic driving data. Accid Anal Prev. 2013;61:10–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant numbers 82073672, 82103950 and 72091514); Natural Science Foundation of Hunan Province, China (grant number 2021JJ40808); China Postdoctoral Science Foundation (grant number 2021M693551); and the Postdoctoral Innovative Talents Program of Hunan Province, China (grant number 2021RC2008). The funding sources had no role in the study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, in the writing of the report, and in the decision to submit the article for publication.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

GH and PN designed the study and directed its implementation, including quality assurance and control. PN, CX, JH, and JL helped supervise the field activities. GH, PN, CX, and YY designed the study’s analytic strategy. PN and GH analyzed data and drafted the initial manuscript. CX, PC, LL, DCS, YY, JH, and JL assisted in the interpretation of data and critically reviewed the manuscript. PN and GH finalized the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Guoqing Hu.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The Ethics Committee of Xiangya School of Public Health, Central South University, China approved our study (approval no. XYGW-2021–72), exempting our research from informed consent. All our recorded data were de-identified. This report follows the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) Statement.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Appendix A.

Brief description of the video-based observational study in Changsha, China. Fig. A1. Geographic location of 20 road intersections for video-based observations in Changsha city, China. Fig. A2. Placement of cameras for video-based observation at road intersections. Appendix B. Appendix Tables and Figures. Table B1. Sample characteristics of pedestrians at 20 road intersections in Changsha, China collected between June 29 and July 21, 2019. Table B2. Basic road characteristics of the 20 included road intersections in Changsha, China. Table B3. Description of the grouping of the four pedestrian distraction indicators for primary analysis, by type of distraction. Table B4. Description of the grouping of four distraction indicators by type of distraction for sensitivity analysis. Table B5. Sensitivity analyses for discriminant validity of the four distraction indicators by alternating the classification of distraction indicators, all walking distractions combined. Table B6. Sensitivity analyses for discriminant validity of four distraction indicators by alternating the grouping of distraction indicator, mobile phone use. Table B7. Sensitivity analyses for discriminant validity of four distraction indicators by alternating the grouping of distraction indicator, talking with other pedestrians. Table B8. Sensitivity analyses for discriminant validity of four distraction indicators by changing the grouping of distraction indicator, eating, drinking, or smoking. Fig. B1. Linear graph showing the associations between the four distraction indicators and near-crash incidence, all walking distractions combined. Fig. B2. Linear graph showing the associations between the four distraction indicators and frequency of looking left and right, all walking distractions combined. Fig. B3. Linear graph showing the associations between the four distraction indicators and speed crossing the street, all walking distractions combined. Fig. B4. Linear graph showing the associations between the four distraction indicators and near-crash incidence, mobile phone use. Fig. B5. Linear graph showing the associations between the four distraction indicators and frequency of looking left and right, mobile phone use. Fig. B6. Linear graph showing the associations between the four distraction indicators and speed crossing the street, mobile phone use. Fig. B7. Linear graph showing the associations between the four distraction indicators and near-crash incidence, talking with other pedestrians. Fig. B8. Linear graph showing the associations between the four distraction indicators and frequency of looking left and right, talking with other pedestrians. Fig. B9. Linear graph showing the associations between the four distraction indicators and speed crossing the street, talking with other pedestrians. Fig. B10. Linear graph showing the associations between the four distraction indicators and speed near-crash incidence, eating, drinking, or smoking. Fig. B11. Linear graph showing the associations between the four distraction indicators and frequency of looking left and right, eating, drinking, or smoking. Fig. B12. Linear graph showing the four distraction indicators and speed crossing the street, eating, drinking, or smoking.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ning, P., Xie, C., Cheng, P. et al. Validity across four common street-crossing distraction indicators to predict pedestrian safety. BMC Public Health 24, 241 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-024-17756-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-024-17756-y

Keywords