Skip to main content

Recognition of others’ interoceptive states in those with and without eating disorders

Abstract

Background

The ability to recognize one’s own emotions is associated with one’s ability to recognize others’ emotions. Beyond the domain of emotion, however, the relationship between recognition of one’s own internal states (interoception) and others’ interoceptive states has not been investigated, either in the typical population or clinical groups with interoceptive difficulties (e.g. eating disorders; EDs).

Method

This study investigated recognition of one’s own and others’ internal states in adults with and without eating disorders, using a high frequency visual noise paradigm. Participants completed self-report measures of interoception, alexithymia (difficulties recognising one’s own emotional internal states) and ED symptomatology, and the Heartbeat Counting Task measure of cardiac interoceptive accuracy.

Results

Alexithymia was significantly negatively correlated with recognition of others’ interoceptive states. EDs were not associated with difficulties recognising others’ interoceptive states.

Conclusions

The ability to recognise one’s own emotional internal states is associated with the recognition of others’ interoceptive states, which may contribute to social skills and the ability to care for others.

Peer Review reports

Background

Interoception refers to the perception of the body’s internal signals, such as cardiac, respiratory and gastric signals, as well as temperature, itch, nausea, pain, and muscle fatigue, although multiple variations of this definition have been proposed [1]. Recent models of interoception distinguish between different domains of interoception, in particular interoceptive attention (the extent to which interoceptive signals are the object of one’s attention) and interoceptive accuracy (one’s ability to perceive internal signals correctly) [2,3,4]. There are substantial individual differences in interoception, with atypical interoceptive accuracy and attention being implicated in multiple clinical conditions [5, 6], such as Autism Spectrum Disorder [7,8,9,10], Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder [11], Obsessive Compulsive Disorder [12, 13], schizophrenia [14], alcohol and substance abuse [15,16,17], depression [18,19,20,21,22], anxiety and panic disorder [22,23,24], and Eating Disorders (EDs) [25, 26]. While the ability to perceive and interpret one’s own interoceptive states, particularly in clinical conditions, has been the focus of much research, very little work has focused on the ability to recognise others’ interoceptive states. The current study investigated the ability to recognise others’ interoceptive states, and its relationship with perception of one’s own internal signals, in the typical population and a clinical group where deficits in interoceptive accuracy and attention are common (those with EDs). The relationship between recognition of one’s own and others’ interoceptive states is of interest in the general population, as well as in all clinical groups characterised by interoceptive atypicality, as if a relationship is observed, training to improve one’s own interoception might lead to improved recognition of others’ interoceptive states, and in turn more successful social interactions and relationships. As an initial research step, EDs were investigated in the current study because i) they are arguably the condition most intuitively associated with interoceptive difficulties, and ii) individuals with EDs appear to experience difficulties with emotion recognition, social interactions, and relationships [27].

EDs, such as Anorexia Nervosa (AN), Bulimia Nervosa (BN) and Binge Eating Disorder (BED)Footnote 1 are characterized by atypical and inappropriate consumption of food, including restricted food intake, bingeing, and purging behaviors [35]. It is unsurprising that individuals with EDs often exhibit atypical interoception, as difficulties perceiving or attending to hunger and satiety (both interoceptive signals) may reduce one’s ability to eat according to physiological requirements (intuitive eating), as observed in EDs [36]. Indeed, intuitive eating and body mass index (BMI) are positively associated with performance on the most commonly utilised interoceptive accuracy measure [37], the Heartbeat Counting Task (HCT) [38], in which participants are asked to count the heartbeats they perceive internally in a given time period (without using external cues such as taking their pulse), and estimates are compared to the objective number of heartbeats that occurred. While empirical findings have been inconsistent, with some studies finding no differences in interoceptive accuracy between those with and without EDs (e.g. [39]), much evidence indicates atypical interoception in those with EDs, with over 90% of studies on EDs finding impairments in interoception relation to control participants, and these difficulties being observed across a range of EDs (AN, BN and BED) and recovered individuals (see [40] for a review). Individuals with, at risk of, or recovered from AN, BN and BED often self-report interoceptive difficulties, both in the domains of accuracy and attention [39, 41,42,43,44,45]. Where objective measures of interoceptive accuracy have been utilized, cardiac interoceptive accuracy has been investigated in those with AN and in those recovered from BN, with both groups exhibiting lower accuracy than typical control participants [46, 47]. Evidence also suggests elevated pain thresholds in individuals with AN and BN [48,49,50], atypicalities in a range of gastrointestinal interoceptive measures across EDs, including greater gastric capacity and reduced sensitivity to gastric distention in BN [26, 51,52,53], and atypical taste perception in EDs, such as reduced taste sensitivity in AN, higher pleasantness or intensity ratings of tastes in BN than control samples [54,55,56]. Atypical interoception may contribute to ED symptomatology, e.g. through reduced recognition of hunger and satiety [47], and reduced perception of the effects of malnutrition, such as fatigue and altered body temperature [39] (see [25] for a review). Individuals with EDs may also attempt to suppress or avoid attending to signals relating to hunger and satiety, potentially leading to reduced interoceptive attention or accuracy [25]. The nature of the relationship between interoception and EDs is therefore likely to be complicated (with interoceptive accuracy and attention both potentially involved), and may be bidirectional. The relevance of interoception to eating disorder pathology is clear, however, with suggestions that interventions to alter interoceptive accuracy or attention are incorporated in ED treatment becoming more prevalent [57, 58].

Interoceptive signals have long been understood to contribute to emotional experience with internal signals featuring prominently in all modern theories of emotion [59, 60]; emotions are thought to emerge when interoceptive signals are interpreted in combination with contextual cues. It is therefore likely that impaired interoceptive accuracy or attention gives rise to some of the emotional difficulties (e.g. difficulties identifying and regulating one’s own emotions and difficulties recognising others’ emotions) commonly experienced by those with EDs [27, 61, 62]. Indeed, alexithymia (difficulties identifying and describing one’s emotions) [63], is common in EDs [64,65,66,67], and has been associated with atypical interoceptive accuracy, such as reduced performance on tasks assessing objective interoceptive accuracy (in the cardiac, muscular effort and taste domains) [2, 68, 69] and self-report measures [70, 71]. Atypical interoceptive attention has also been observed in alexithymia, although the direction of this effect varies across studies, likely due to self-report measures being interpreted inconsistently, with some participants reporting on their interoceptive accuracy and others on their interoceptive attention [72]. Alexithymia has also been associated with atypical structure and function of the cortical structures known to support interoception, such as the anterior insula and anterior cingulate cortices e.g. [73,74,75,76,77]. Interestingly, this relationship between alexithymia and interoception has been observed in multiple clinical groups [6, 70], including those with EDs [41, 70]. While some individuals may experience alexithymia owing to non-interoceptive difficulties (e.g. language impairments), interoceptive impairment (potentially affecting both accuracy and attention) is thought to be a common cause of alexithymia (see [78] for a review). Indeed, the original definition of alexithymia describes this construct in terms of reduced accuracy distinguishing between internal signals of emotion, and differentiating emotions from other somatic sensations (i.e. interoceptive accuracy within the emotional domain) [63].

Alexithymia is of relevance to the current study, investigating recognition of others’ interoceptive states, not only because of its strong link with one’s own interoceptive abilities, but also as it is strongly associated with recognition of others’ emotional states [79]. Indeed, alexithymia appears to explain difficulties recognising others’ emotions in a number of clinical conditions, such as autism [80, 81] and EDs [27, 67, 82,83,84]. It is therefore likely that a similar relationship exists beyond the affective domain; difficulties perceiving or identifying one’s own non-emotional interoceptive states may be associated with difficulties recognising these interoceptive states in others. While higher interoceptive accuracy appears to predict better recognition of others’ emotions [85, 86], research into the ability to recognize other’s non-emotional interoceptive states is lacking. Some studies have investigated pain recognition, and the ability to recognize others’ pain has been associated with alexithymia [83]. Typical participants also appear able to detect illness in others (whether individuals who had received either a bacterial injection triggering an immune response or a placebo injection were ‘sick’ or ‘healthy’) above chance [87]. Another study found that individuals performed above chance at identifying which of two individuals a visual representation of a heartbeat belonged to [88], and some studies have investigated recognition of babies’ cries, for example rating cries in terms of infants’ pain or sickness [89,90,91]. Beyond these studies, however, there has been no direct investigation of the ability to recognize others’ interoceptive states, beyond emotions.

Accurate recognition of others’ internal states is crucial for successful social interactions; identifying others’ interoceptive states such as hunger, fatigue, pain, and nausea allows one to empathize, respond appropriately, and provide assistance where required. More accurate recognition of others’ interoceptive states should therefore lead to more successful social interactions and relationships, and better ability to provide care for others when needed. This may be particularly relevant to caring roles, such as when parenting or in medical or care-providing professions, and when the observed individual struggles to recognize or communicate their own internal states, as may be the case in children, older adults, and some clinical groups [92, 93]. This is also especially relevant to populations where both atypical interoceptive accuracy/attention and difficulties with interpersonal relationships have been observed, as is the case for many clinical groups, including those with EDs [94,95,96,97]. The lack of research on recognition of others’ interoceptive states is therefore surprising, but may be attributable to the lack of available stimuli until now.

The current study aimed to investigate the ability to recognise others’ interoceptive states, and whether this is predicted by participants’ ability to recognise their own internal signals (both emotions and non-emotional interoceptive states). This question was investigated in a sample of typical individuals, and those with EDs, who are likely to experience interoceptive difficulties. It was hypothesized that one’s own interoceptive abilities (assessed using HCT and the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness) and alexithymia (assessed using the Toronto Alexithymia Scale) would predict one’s ability to recognize others’ interoceptive states. It was also predicted that, before controlling for interoception and alexithymia, the ED group would exhibit impaired recognition of others’ interoceptive states.

Method

Participants

Participants were 108 females 40 diagnosed with EDs by an independent clinician, 68 with no ED history). Participants were recruited through adverts in the community (including on social media), university participant pools, ED clinics, the Beat ED charity, and a database of participants who had taken part in previous research on EDs and indicated that they would like to participate in future studies. ED participants completed the Feeding and Eating Disorders section of the SCID-5-RV interview [98], administered by a member of the research team, to confirm current ED status. The ED group was heterogeneous (31 AN, 4 BN, 2 BED, 1 purging disorder, 2 with ED diagnoses unconfirmed by SCID), with illness duration ranging between 1 and 25 years. Given this heterogeneity, analyses were conducted with the entire diagnosed group, as well as in the 31 participants with confirmed AN diagnoses alone. The ED and control groups did not differ significantly in IQ, measured by the two subscale version of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence [99], meaning any group differences on the behavioural tasks would be unlikely to be driven by differences between the samples in terms of cognitive ability. The ED group was significantly older than the control group, so although the difference in means was relatively small (ED M = 29.48, SD = 9.35; control M = 24.19, SD = 8.85), age was controlled for statistically in analyses comparing the ED and control groups, as performance on behavioural tasks, and alexithymia and interoception, may change with age [100]. Eating disorder symptomatology (assessed using the Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire; EDE-Q) [101]), depression, anxiety and stress, (assessed with the Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale; DASS [102]) and BMI, were all significantly higher in the ED than control group as expected. See Table 1 for these group comparisons. Participants gave informed consent to participate.

Table 1 t-tests comparing ED and control groups on demographic variables

Interoception

Objective interoceptive accuracy

Objective cardiac interoceptive accuracy was assessed using a modified version of the HCT [38]. Participants’ right index finger was placed inside a pulse oximeter while they counted their heartbeats (felt internally, without taking their pulse) across four time intervals (either 25 s, 35 s, 45 s and 100 s or 28 s, 38 s, 48 s and 103 s), presented in a randomized order. Participants’ verbally reported number of heartbeats was compared to the objective number of heartbeats recorded by the pulse oximeter. Participants were instructed not to estimate how many heartbeats occurred [103, 104].

A time estimation task [105] was implemented as a control task [106], in order to reduce the likelihood that HCT performance reflects participants’ ability to estimate time and infer number of heartbeats from their knowledge of their heart rate. Procedure was identical to that for the HCT, except participants estimated seconds rather than heartbeats, and completed the set of intervals not completed in the HCT. Time estimation task and HCT order was counterbalanced.

Accuracy on the HCT was calculated using the formula below, with scores ranging between 0 and 100, and higher scores indicating higher cardiac interoceptive accuracyFootnote 2:

$$\left(\left(1-\left(\textrm{Absolute}\ \left(\left(\textrm{Actual}\ \textrm{number}\ \textrm{of}\ \textrm{heartbeats}-{\textrm{participant}}^{'}\textrm{s}\ \textrm{estimate}\right)/\left(\textrm{Actual}\ \textrm{number}\ \textrm{of}\ \textrm{heartbeats}\right)\right)\ast 100\right)/\textrm{Number}\ \textrm{of}\ \textrm{counting}\ \textrm{periods}\right)\right).$$

Self-reported Interoception

Self-reported interoception was measured using the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA) [107], assessing eight facets of interoception across 32 items, namely Noticing (Awareness of uncomfortable, comfortable, and neutral body sensations, e.g. ‘When I am tense I notice where the tension is located in my body’), Not Distracting (Tendency not to ignore or distract oneself from sensations of pain or discomfort, e.g. ‘I distract myself from sensations of discomfort’, reverse scored), Not Worrying (Tendency not to worry or experience emotional distress with sensations of pain or discomfort, e.g. ‘I can notice an unpleasant body sensation without worrying about it’), Attention Regulation (Ability to sustain and control attention to body sensations, e.g. ‘I can pay attention to my breath without being distracted by things happening around me’), Emotional Awareness (Awareness of the connection between body sensations and emotional states, e.g. ‘I notice how my body changes when I feel happy/joyful’), Self Regulation (Ability to regulate distress by attention to body sensations, e.g. ‘I can use my breath to reduce tension’), Body Listening (Active listening to the body for insight, e.g. ‘I listen for information from my body about my emotional state’), and Trusting (Experience of one’s body as safe and trustworthy, e.g. ‘I trust my body sensations’). The MAIA was selected as it includes subscales that explicitly assess interoceptive attention (Attention Regulation and Not Distracting subscales), and has been validated for use in EDs, with findings in an ED sample replicating the original 8 factor structure, and finding acceptable internal and external consistency [41].

Alexithymia

At the time of data collection, a well validated self-report measure of interoceptive accuracy was not available. Therefore, alexithymia was measured with the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) [108]. The TAS-20 includes 20 items, assessing three facets (Difficulty identifying feelings, e.g. ‘I am often confused about what emotion I am feeling’, Difficulty describing feelings, e.g. ‘It is difficult for me to find the right words for my feelings’, and Externally oriented thinking, e.g. ‘I prefer to analyze problems rather than just describe them’, reverse scored). TAS-20 scores range between 20 and 80, with higher values indicating more severe alexithymia. The TAS-20 arguably assesses ‘emotional’ interoceptive accuracy, and appears to be strongly negatively associated with objective and more recently developed self-report measures explicitly assessing interoceptive accuracy [70, 71].

Recognition of others’ interoceptive states

Stimuli

Fifty-six static images depicted four trained actors posing seven interoceptive states (two exemplars of each state per actor): itch, satiety, pain, nausea, cold, breathlessness, and tiredness (Fig. 1). These stimuli were validated prior to use (Supplementary Materials 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Examples of stimuli expressing non-emotional internal states: Cold (a), nausea (b) and pain (c)

Procedure

The experimental procedure was adapted from that used by Brewer et al. [83] to investigate emotion recognition in EDs. Trials began with a fixation point (1000 ms), followed by an interoceptive state stimulus (800 ms). Participants then indicated whether the image depicted a specific internal state (e.g. ‘Cold: yes or no?’), responding with a key press. Each state was presented 50 times, yielding 350 trials. Stimuli within a state category were selected randomly on each trial.

Participants’ ability to attribute internal states accurately was estimated by their tolerance of high frequency visual noise, (Fig. 2). High frequency visual noise was used to occlude images, in order to avoid ceiling effects common in visual recognition tasks and ensure that the task was sensitive to individual differences in recognition ability. This technique has previously been found to be effective in a similar emotion recognition task in those with EDs [83]. Noise was achieved by replacing the greyscale intensity values comprising the stimulus image with the identical value to the background grey colour. These intensity values were randomly selected across the stimulus image; occluded image points varied across participants, but obscured images uniformly. Visual noise proportion was adjusted using an adaptive staircase procedure. For the first 42 trials (6 trials per internal state), noise level was 50%. For the next 28 trials, noise adjustments were made in 16% increments/decrements according to performance; two consecutive correct responses for the same attribute or one incorrect response lead to increased or decreased noise on the following trial for that attribute, respectively. Subsequently, the noise adjustments were decreased, to 8, 4, 2, and 1%, in blocks of 70 trials. The noise tolerance threshold (% noise) for each attribute at the 350th trial estimated recognition accuracy. The mean noise tolerance threshold across all states was taken as the Global Noise Tolerance Threshold (GNTT). The experimental paradigm was programmed and presented using Matlab with Psychtoolbox [109, 110].

Fig. 2
figure 2

High frequency visual noise obscuring (a) 20%, (b) 50% and (c) 80% of pixels

Results

A Kolmogorov-Smirnov test indicated that noise tolerance threshold was negatively skewed, with five participants (2 ED, 3 typical control) scoring more than twice the interquartile range below the mean. Following removal of these outliers, data were normally distributed, so analyses involving the state recognition task are reported on a final sample of 38 ED and 65 control participants. Given ED heterogeneity, analyses were repeated including only the 31 AN participants, and where this altered the pattern of significance, this is explicitly reported.

Non-normal distributions were also observed for age and a number of questionnaire variables (DASS depression, DASS anxiety, and all MAIA subscales except Not Worrying and Attention Regulation). Noise tolerance thresholds were also not normally distributed for any individual internal state. As LN, log10 and square root transformations did not normalise data, untransformed data and non-parametric tests are reported. As the ED and control groups differed in age, age was controlled for in analyses comparing these groups. Where individual participants were missing data (four missing BMI values; three missing HCT values), participants were excluded from analyses including that variable.

ED and control group comparisons

Independent samples t-tests compared the ED and control groups on measures of interoceptive abilities and alexithymia. Higher TAS-20 scores in the ED (M = 58.28, SD = 11.15) than control (M = 52.72, SD = 12.93) group, t (106) = 2.27, p = .025, indicated lower alexithymia in the ED group. This held when age was controlled for in a one-way ANCOVA, F [1, 111] = 4.79, p = .031, η2 = .044.

MAIA scores did not differ between the groups for the subscales of Noticing, Worrying, Attention Regulation, Emotional Awareness, or Listening (all p > .24), including when controlling for age in one-way ANCOVAs (all p > .19). However, the control group scored higher than the ED group on Not Distracting (ED Median = 1.33, NED = 40; Control Median = 2.67, NControl = 68; U = 677.5, p < .001), Self-Regulation (ED Median = 1.50, NED = 40; Control Median = 2.13, NControl = 68; U = 996.5, p = .020) and Trusting (ED Median = 1.00, NED = 40; Control Median = 3.00, NControl = 68; U = 515.0, p < .001) subscales, including when age was controlled for using one-way ANCOVAs (all p < .008); the ED group reported being more likely to ignore uncomfortable physical sensations, less able to regulate distress by attending to internal signals, and less likely to experience their body as safe and trustworthy. When the ED group contained only individuals with AN, significantly higher levels of Listening were also reported in the control than AN group (ED Median = 1.00, NED = 31; Control Median = 1.83, NControl = 68; U = 618.0, p = .048).

A one-way ANCOVA with HCT performance as the dependent variable, diagnostic group (ED, control) as the independent variable, and age, IQ, BMI and Time Estimation as control variables [104], indicated that the ED and control groups did not differ in cardiac interoceptive accuracy (F (1, 96) = .532, p = .468, η2 = .006).

A 7(State: Cold, Itch, Nausea, Breathlessness, Satiety, Tiredness, Pain) × 2 (Group: ED, control) ANCOVA controlling for age indicated a significant main effect of State (F(6,600) = 4.10, p = .001, η2 = .039), indicating that noise tolerance was higher for some states than others, suggesting better recognition of some states than others. Noise Tolerance was highest for stimuli depicting cold, and lowest for stimuli depicting pain (Fig. 3). A non-significant main effect of Group indicated that noise tolerance threshold did not differ between the ED (M = 65.81, SD = 12.59) and control (M = 65.25, SD = 14.08) groups (F (1, 100) = .001, p = .975, η2 < .001). The State x Group interaction, F(6,600) = 1.13, p = .343, η2 = .011, main effect of age, F (1, 100) = .098, p = .755, η2 = .001, and age x State interaction, F(6,600) = 1.46, p = .196, η2 = .014, were non-significant.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Estimated marginal means for Noise Tolerance Threshold for the seven Interoceptive States. Error bars show standard error. *Significant at α = .05 with Bonferroni correction

Predictors of recognition of others’ internal states

Correlation analyses investigated the relationship between one’s own interoceptive abilities (MAIA subscales, HCT) and alexithymia (TAS-20) and recognition of others’ interoceptive states.Footnote 3 Global Noise Tolerance Threshold (GNTT; mean of the Noise Tolerance Thresholds for all individual states) was significantly negatively correlated with TAS-20 score, r (101) = − .196, p = .047, indicating poorer recognition of others’ internal states in those with greater difficulties identifying and describing their own emotional internal states (Fig. 4). This fell to a trend when individuals with EDs other than AN were excluded, likely due to reduction in power, r (92) = − .191, p = .065. Similarly, this effect fell below the significance threshold when the ED, r = −.250, p = .130, and control, r = −.186, p = .139, samples were analysed separately, again likely owing to reduced statistical power. EDE-Q scores were not significantly correlated with GNTT, r (101) = − .04, p = .657, indicating that ED symptomatology was unrelated to recognition of others’ interoceptive states. No significant relationship was observed between GNTT and any MAIA or DASS subscale. Similarly, there was no significant association between GNTT and Heartbeat Counting Task performance in a partial correlation controlling for age, IQ, BMI and Time Estimation, r (91) = − .05, p = .633. When the AN group only was analyzed, however, both DASS Anxiety, r (29) = − .372, p = .043, and DASS Depression, r (29) = − .485, p = .007, were significantly correlated with GNTT, indicating that higher depression and anxiety in those with AN were associated with lower GNTT, suggesting poorer recognition of others’ interoceptive states.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Scatter plot showing significant negative correlation between Alexithymia (measured by TAS-20 total score) and recognition of others’ internal states (measured by mean Noise Tolerance Threshold across all state stimuli

Discussion

The current study was the first to investigate the ability to recognize others’ interoceptive states, beyond the domain of emotion, and its relationship with one’s own interoceptive abilities and alexithymia. Results indicated that one’s own objective cardiac interoceptive accuracy and self-reported interoceptive attention (estimated by the two MAIA subscales most closely related to the construct of interoceptive attention: the Attention Regulation and Not Distracting subscales) were not related to recognition of others’ interoceptive states. Alexithymia, however, was significantly negatively associated with recognition of others’ interoceptive states, whereby those who struggled to interpret their own emotional internal signals were also less able to recognise visual noise-occluded images of others expressing interoceptive states (such as cold, pain, satiety and itch). ED presence and severity were not related to recognition of others’ interoceptive states, suggesting that it is the ability to recognise one’s own internal states, rather than ED pathology per se, that predicts the ability to identify these states in others.

The negative relationship between alexithymia and recognition of others’ interoceptive states extends previous findings that those with more severe alexithymia struggle to recognize others’ emotions, both within the typical population [79], and in clinical groups including EDs [83]. As alexithymia is strongly associated with poor interoceptive accuracy, and many of the items in the TAS-20 refer to ‘sensations in my body’, ‘feelings’, or ‘physical sensations’ rather than ‘emotions’, making them relevant to interoception more broadly, the current findings may be indicative of poor interoceptive accuracy in the self leading to difficulties identifying interoceptive signals in others. The current study may, therefore, have underestimated the relationship between one’s own self-reported interoceptive accuracy and ability to recognize others’ interoceptive states, due to most TAS-20 items relating more specifically to emotion. The recently developed Interoceptive Accuracy Scale (IAS; 64) was explicitly designed to assess subjective interoceptive accuracy outside of the emotional domain, allowing future work to investigate the relationship between self-reported interoceptive accuracy and recognition of others’ interoceptive states more specifically. It is likely that, as the current behavioral task involved recognition of others’ non-emotional interoceptive states, a self-report scale which explicitly refers to perception of these interoceptive states in the self (e.g. the IAS) would be an even stronger predictor of performance than the TAS-20.

The observed relationship between alexithymia and recognition of others’ states has implications for real world social functioning. If difficulties perceiving one’s own emotions and other internal states lead to difficulties recognising interoceptive states in others, this may contribute to social impairment, due to difficulties responding appropriately to others during social interactions. Similarly, these difficulties may impede one’s ability to care for others, which is particularly relevant for individuals with a caregiving role, such as parents, or those working in childcare or medical settings, for example. Indeed, attending to and responding to children’s interoceptive states has been described as an important aspect of parenting, which contributes to the development of empathy and verbal and non-verbal communication skills in children [112]. Further work is required in order to determine the causal nature of this relationship and whether training individuals’ interoceptive accuracy or attention might improve recognition of others’ interoceptive states, and in turn caring and social abilities. Future work should also aim to replicate this finding separately in typical and clinical samples separately, as the current study was underpowered to detect an effect in either sample alone.

Importantly, ED presence and symptom severity themselves were unrelated to recognition of others’ interoceptive states, despite interoceptive atypicalities (in the domain or attention and accuracy) being common in ED [40], and the current ED sample reporting higher alexithymia and lower interoceptive abilities on some MAIA subscales than the control group; those with ED reported being more likely to ignore uncomfortable physical sensations, less able to regulate distress by attending to internal signals, and less likely to experience their body as safe and trustworthy, and those with AN reported low levels of active listening to interoceptive cues for insight into their body state. This is consistent with findings in the emotional domain; while difficulties recognising others’ emotional expressions are common in EDs, these difficulties are associated with co-occurring alexithymia rather than ED symptomatology per se [83]. It is likely that there is substantial variation in the ability to recognize one’s own interoceptive states within the ED population, which may predict individual differences in the recognition of these states in others. Of course, the fact that interoceptive deficits are common in those with EDs may mean that difficulties recognising other’s interoceptive states are more prevalent in this population than in the neurotypical population, but these social perception difficulties are unlikely to be universal in EDs. Notably, when individuals with AN were analyzed separately, anxiety and depression were both negatively associated with the ability to recognize others’ interoceptive states. As both anxiety and depression have been associated with atypical interoceptive accuracy and attention (e.g. Paulus & Stein, 2010), further work with more sensitive measures of interoception is required to determine whether atypical interoceptive accuracy and/or attention mediate this relationship, whether anxiety and depression have a direct and specific influence on recognition of others’ interoceptive states in AN, or whether this effect is simply explained by a more domain-general effect of anxiety and depression on behavioral task performance.

Performance on the HCT was not associated with the ability to recognize others’ internal states, and did not differ between those with and without EDs. As the HCT assesses a single (cardiac) interoceptive signal channel and interoceptive accuracy may not be a unitary construct [111, 113, 114], it is perhaps unsurprising that performance did not predict recognition of others’ interoceptive states outside the cardiac domain, such as satiety, nausea and itch. Future work should therefore investigate objective recognition accuracy of the same interoceptive states in the self and others. This is particularly important given extensive debate in recent years concerning the utility of cardiac perception tasks, in particular the HCT, for example owing to low associations with other tasks assessing cardiac perception [115]. While the need to control for a range of confounding variables [104] has been taken into account in the current study, it remains the case that heart rate knowledge and exteroceptive cues available through the chest wall [116] or pulse oximeter [117] may have contributed to performance. HCT performance should therefore be interpreted with caution, and future work should aim to investigate the relationship with recognition of others’ internal states across a range of objective interoceptive measures.

Recent work has also highlighted the importance of distinguishing between interoceptive accuracy and attention [2, 3]. As a specific self-report measure of interoceptive attention was not available when data collection began, the Attention Regulation and Not Distracting subscales of the MAIA were utilized to assess interoceptive attention, and were not associated with participants’ ability to recognize others’ interoceptive states. Notably, the behavioural task assessed recognition accuracy, rather than attention to others’ states, so it is possible that attention to one’s own interoceptive states relates to attention to others’ interoceptive states, but is less relevant in predicting recognition accuracy. The recently developed Interoceptive Attention Scale [72] will enable further research into the separable roles of one’s own interoceptive accuracy and attention in recognition of others’ internal states, and future work should aim to investigate the relationship between each of these and both attention to and accuracy recognising others’ interoceptive states.

The current study is the first to utilize stimuli depicting a range of interoceptive states in others. The observed correlation between recognition of these images and alexithymia provides support for these stimuli as valid depictions of others’ interoceptive states, and the variability in performance suggests that it is possible to investigate individual differences in recognition ability. This study therefore has important implications for the research field, paving the way towards further investigation of the recognition of others’ interoceptive states, for example across typical and clinical populations, developmentally, at the neural level, and in relation to other areas of cognition. An updated stimulus set adapted from the current stimuli is now freely available for researcher use in addressing such questions [118].

While this study has important implications for the research field and real world, a number of limitations and future directions are worth considering. As discussed, measurement issues have been a key focus of contemporary interoception research, meaning replication using measures explicitly and specifically assessing subjective interoceptive accuracy and attention, and valid objective measures of interoceptive accuracy and attention across a range of interoceptive signal channels is required. Further, the current ED sample included a range of ED diagnoses, and while diagnoses often change over time [29,30,31], and both interoceptive difficulties [40] and emotion recognition difficulties [67] have been observed across a range of ED diagnostic categories, it is possible that the relationship between recognition of one’s own and others’ non-emotional interoceptive states differs across ED subtypes. Future work should therefore recruit large subsamples in order to compare across groups. Moreover, as interoceptive and social perception difficulties have been observed in multiple clinical groups [6], further work should investigate whether this relationship holds across multiple clinical populations. The current sample also comprised females exclusively, as ED prevalence (at least where AN and BN are concerned) is higher among females than males [119,120,121,122,123,124], and interoceptive difficulties have also been found to vary as a function of sex, with females reporting greater interoceptive attention, and males often exhibiting higher interoceptive accuracy than females, although this varies with task type [125,126,127]. Investigating the role of sex in the relationship between recognition of one’s own and others’ interoceptive states, both in the ED and typical population, is a priority, however, especially as recent research suggests that ED psychopathology is more prevalent in males than previously assumed [128, 129]. Finally, while the use of high frequency noise in the current recognition task is likely to have increased the sensitivity of the task to individual differences and avoided ceiling effects, it of course also reduces the ecological validity of stimuli, so replication with more naturalistic tasks is recommended. Replication is also required utilising additional interoceptive states. It is also worth noting that as stimuli could be repeated, the level of noise present during previous presentations may have affected performance on subsequent presentations if individual stimuli were memorised.

Conclusions

The current study found that alexithymia predicted the ability to recognize others’ interoceptive states. Recognition ability was not predicted, however, by self-reported interoception (including subscales assessing interoceptive attention) or objective cardiac interoceptive accuracy, or by eating disorder presence or symptom severity. Findings suggest that one’s ability to perceive and identify one’s own internal states accurately is associated with the ability to recognize these states in others, with implications for social interactions, relationships, and caring abilities. Future research should replicate these findings using recently developed measures of interoceptive accuracy and attention, across a range of clinical groups.

Availability of data and materials

Upon acceptance, all data will be made available through the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/).

Notes

  1. While distinctions between different ED categories have traditionally been made, e.g. based on divergent genetic profiles [28], diagnostic migration is common [29,30,31] encouraging transdiagnostic approaches that recognize the shared underlying psychopathology of EDs [32,33,34].

  2. Accuracy on the time estimation task was calculated using the same formula, but with number of seconds replacing number of heartbeats.

  3. Pearson correlations were used for TAS-20, EDE-Q and MAIA Not Worrying and Attention Regulation subscales, while Spearman correlations were used for DASS anxiety and depression, and all remaining MAIA subscales, due to violated normality assumptions.

Abbreviations

EDs:

Feeding and eating disorders

AN:

Anorexia nervosa

BN:

Bulimia nervosa

BED:

Binge eating disorder

HCT:

Heartbeat counting task

SCID:

Structured clinical interview for DSM-5

EDE-Q:

Eating disorder examination questionnaire

DASS:

Depression, anxiety and stress scale

MAIA:

Multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness

TAS-20:

Toronto alexithymia scale

GNTT:

Global noise tolerance threshold

References

  1. Khalsa SS, Lapidus RC. Can interoception improve the pragmatic search for biomarkers in psychiatry? Front Psychiatry. 2016;7:121.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Murphy J, Catmur C, Bird G. Alexithymia is associated with a multi-domain, multi-dimensional failure of interoception: evidence from novel tests. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2018;44:1–36.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Murphy J, Catmur C, Bird G. Classifying individual differences in interoception: implications for the measurement of interoceptive awareness. Psychon Bull Rev. 2019;26(5):1467–71.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Suksasilp C, Garfinkel SN. Towards a comprehensive assessment of interoception in a multi-dimensional framework. Biol Psychol. 2022;168:108262.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Khalsa SS, Adolphs R, Cameron OG, Critchley HD, Davenport PW, Feinstein JS, et al. Interoception and mental health: a roadmap. Biol Psychiatry. 2018;3:501–13.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Brewer R, Murphy J, Bird G. Atypical interoception as a common risk factor for psychopathology: a review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;130:470–508.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Garfinkel SN, Tiley C, O’Keeffe S, Harrison NA, Seth AK, Critchley HD. Discrepancies between dimensions of interoception in autism: implications for emotion and anxiety. Biol Psychol. 2016;114:117–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Mul C-L, Stagg SD, Herbelin B, Aspell JE. The feeling of me feeling for you: Interoception, alexithymia and empathy in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2018;48(9):2953–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Hatfield TR, Brown RF, Giummarra MJ, Lenggenhager B. Autism spectrum disorder and interoception: abnormalities in global integration? Autism. 2017;23.

  10. Nicholson T, Williams D, Carpenter K, Kallitsounaki A. Interoception is impaired in children, but not adults, with autism Spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;49(9):3625–37.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Kutscheidt K, Dresler T, Hudak J, Barth B, Blume F, Ethofer T, et al. Interoceptive awareness in patients with attention - deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). ADHD Atten Deficit Hyperact Disord. 2019;11(4):395–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Lazarov A, Dar R, Oded Y, Liberman N. Are obsessive-compulsive tendencies related to reliance on external proxies for internal states? Evidence from biofeedback-aided relaxation studies. Behav Res Ther. 2010;48(6):516–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Schultchen D, Zaudig M, Krauseneck T, Pollatos O. Interoceptive deficits in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder in the time course of cognitive-behavioral therapy. PLoS One. 2019;14.

  14. Ardizzi M, Ambrosecchia M, Buratta L, Ferri F, Peciccia M, Donnari S, et al. Interoception and positive symptoms in schizophrenia. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016;10:379.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Jakubczyk A, Skrzeszewski J, Trucco EM, Suszek H, Zaorska J, Nowakowska M, et al. Interoceptive accuracy and interoceptive sensibility in individuals with alcohol use disorder – different phenomena with different clinical correlations? Drug Alcohol Depend. 2019;198:34–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Paulus MP, Stewart JL. Interoception and drug addiction. Neuropharmacology. 2014;76:342–50.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Verdejo-Garcia A, Clark L, Dunn BD. The role of interoception in addiction: a critical review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36(8):1857–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Aaronson ST, Sears P, Ruvuna F, Bunker M, Conway CR, Dougherty DD, et al. A 5-Year Observational Study of Patients With Treatment-Resistant Depression Treated With Vagus Nerve Stimulation or Treatment as Usual : Comparison of Response , Remission , and Suicidality. Am J Psychiatry. 2017;174(7):640–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Dunn BD, Dalgleish T, Ogilvie AD, Lawrence AD. Heartbeat perception in depression. Behav Res Ther. 2007;45(8):1921–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Furman DJ, Waugh CE, Bhattacharjee K, Thompson RJ, Gotlib IH. Interoceptive awareness, positive affect, and decision making in major depressive disorder. J Affect Disord. 2013;151(2):780–5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Harshaw C. Interoceptive dysfunction: toward and integrated frameword for understanding somatic and affective disturbance in depression. Psychol Bull. 2015;141(2):311–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Paulus MP, Stein MB. Interoception in anxiety and depression. Brain Struct Funct. 2010;214:451–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Ehlers A, Breuer P. Increased cardiac awareness in panic disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 1992;101(3):371–82.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Yoris A, Esteves S, Couto B, Melloni M, Kichic R, Cetkovich M, et al. The roles of interoceptive sensitivity and metacognitive interoception in panic. Behav Brain Funct. 2015;11(14):1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Herbert BM. Interoception and its role for eating, obesity, and eating disorders: empirical findings and conceptual conclusions. Eur J Heal Psychol. 2020;27(4):188–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Khalsa SS, Berner LA, Anderson LM. Gastrointestinal Interoception in eating disorders: charting a new path. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2022;24(1):47–60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Oldershaw A, Hambrook D, Stahl D, Tchanturia K, Treasure J, Schmidt U. The socio-emotional processing stream in anorexia nervosa. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35(3):970–88.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Kendler KS, Ohlsson H, Sundquist J, Sundquist K. The patterns of family genetic risk scores for eleven major psychiatric and substance use disorders in a Swedish national sample. Transl Psychiatry. 2021;11(1).

  29. Fichter MM, Quadflieg N. Long-term stability of eating disorder diagnoses. Int J Eat Disord. 2007;40:S61–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Milos G, Spindler A, Schnyder U, Fairburn CG. Instability of eating disorder diagnoses: prospective study. Br J Psychiatry. 2005;187:573–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Eddy KT, Dorer DJ, Franko DL, Tahilani K, Thompson-Brenner H, Herzog DB. Diagnostic crossover in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: implications for DSM-V. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165(2):245–50.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Fairburn CG, Cooper Z, Shafran R. Cognitive behaviour therapy for eating disorders: a “transdiagnostic” theory and treatment. Behav Res Ther. 2003;41(5):509–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Hoiles KJ, Egan SJ, Kane RT. The validity of the transdiagnostic cognitive behavioural model of eating disorders in predicting dietary restraint. Eat Behav. 2012;13(2):123–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Lampard AM, Tasca GA, Balfour L, Bissada H. An evaluation of the transdiagnostic cognitive-behavioural model of eating disorders. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2013;21(2):99–107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorder. 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  36. Tylka TL, Wilcox JA. Are intuitive eating and eating disorder symptomatology opposite poles of the same construct? J Couns Psychol. 2006;53(4):474–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Herbert BM, Blechert J, Hautzinger M, Matthias E, Herbert C. Intuitive eating is associated with interoceptive sensitivity. Effects Body Mass Index Appetite. 2013;70:22–30.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Schandry R. Heart Beat Perception and Emotional Experience. Psychophysiology. 1981;18(4):483–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Eshkevari E, Rieger E, Musiat P, Treasure J. An investigation of interoceptive sensitivity in eating disorders using a heartbeat detection task and a self-report measure. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2014;22:383–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Martin E, Dourish CT, Rotshtein P, Spetter MS, Higgs S. Interoception and disordered eating: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;107:166–91.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Brown TA, Berner LA, Jones MD, Reilly EE, Cusack A, Anderson LK, et al. Psychometric evaluation and norms for the multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness ( MAIA ) in a clinical eating disorders sample. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2017;25:411–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Fassino S, Pierò A, Gramaglia C, Abbate-Daga G. Clinical, psychopathological and personality correlates of interoceptive awareness in anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa and obesity. Psychopathology. 2004;37(4):168–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Jenkinson PM, Taylor L, Laws KR. Self-reported interoceptive deficits in eating disorders: a meta-analysis of studies using the eating disorder inventory. J Psychosom Res. 2018;110:38–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Maganto C, Garaigordobil M, Kortabarria L. Eating problems in adolescents and youths: explanatory variables. Span J Psychol. 2016;19(2016):1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Srinivasagam NM, Kaye WH, Plotnicov KH, Greeno C, Weltzin TE, Rao R. Persistent perfectionism, symmetry, and exactness after long-term recovery from anorexia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry. 1995;152(11):1630–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Pollatos O, Kurz AL, Albrecht J, Schreder T, Kleemann AM, Schöpf V, et al. Reduced perception of bodily signals in anorexia nervosa. Eat Behav. 2008;9(4):381–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Klabunde M, Acheson D, Boutelle K, Matthews S, Kaye W. Interoceptive sensitivity deficits in women recovered from bulimia nervosa. Eat Behav. 2013;14(4):488–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Lautenbacher S, Pauls AM, Strian F, Pirke K-M, Krieg J-C. Pain Sensitivity in Anorexia Nervosa and Bulimia Nervosa. Biol Psychiatry. 1991;29(2):1073–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Papezová H, Yamamotová A, Uher R. Elevated pain threshold in eating disorders: physiological and psychological factors. J Psychiatr Res. 2005;39(4):431–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. de Zwaan M, Biener D, Bach M, Wiesnagrotzki S, Stacher G. Pain sensitivity, alexithymia, and depression in patients with eating disorders: are they related? J Psychosom Res. 1996;41(1):65–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Geliebter A, Hashim SA. Gastric capacity in normal, obese, and bulimic women. Physiol Behav. 2001;74(4–5):743–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Kissileff HR, Wentzlaff TH, Guss JL, Walsh BT, Devlin MJ, Thornton JC. A direct measure of satiety disturbance in patients with bulimia nervosa. Physiol Behav. 1996;60(4):1077–85.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Zimmerli EJ, Walsh BT, Guss JL, Devlin MJ, Kissileff HR. Gastric compliance in bulimia nervosa. Physiol Behav. 2006;87(2):441–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Kinnaird E, Stewart C, Tchanturia K. Taste sensitivity in anorexia nervosa: a systematic review. Int J Eat Disord. 2018;51:771–84.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Sunday SR, Halmi KA. Taste perceptions and hedonics in eating disorders. Physiol Behav. 1990;48:587–94.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Rodin J, Bartoshuk L, Peterson C, Schank D. Bulimia and taste: possible interactions. J Abnorm Psychol. 1990;99(1):32–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Khoury NM, Lutz J, Schuman-Olivier Z. Interoception in psychiatric disorders: a review of randomized, controlled trials with Interoception-based interventions. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 2018;26(5):250–63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Boswell JF, Anderson LM, Anderson DA. Integration of interoceptive exposure in eating disorder treatment. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 2015;22(2):194–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Schachter S, Singer JE. Cognitive, social, and psychological determinants of emotional state. Psychol Rev. 1962;69(5):379.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Critchley HD, Garfinkel SN. Interoception and emotion. Curr Opin Psychol. 2017;17:7–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Lavender JM, Wonderlich SA, Engel SG, Gordon KH, Kaye WH, Mitchell JE. Dimensions of emotion dysregulation in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: a conceptual review of the empirical literature. Clin Psychol Rev. 2015;40:111–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Oldershaw A, Lavender T, Sallis H, Stahl D, Schmidt U. Emotion generation and regulation in anorexia nervosa: a systematic review and meta-analysis of self-report data. Clin Psychol Rev. 2015;39:83–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Sifneos PE. The prevalence of “alexithymic” characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychother Psychosom. 1973;22(2):255–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Rozenstein MH, Latzer Y, Stein D, Eviatar Z. Perception of emotion and bilateral advantage in women with eating disorders, their healthy sisters, and nonrelated healthy controls. J Affect Disord. 2011;134(1–3):386–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Carano A, De Berardis D, Gambi F, Di Paolo C, Campanella D, Pelusi L, et al. Alexithymia and body image in adult outpatients with binge eating disorder. Int J Eat Disord. 2006;39(4):332–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Nowakowski ME, McFarlane T, Cassin S. Alexithymia and eating disorders: a critical review of the literature. J Eat Disord. 2013;1(1):21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Mason TB, Lesser EL, Dolgon-Krutolow AR, Wonderlich SA, Smith KE. An updated transdiagnostic review of social cognition and eating disorder psychopathology. J Psychiatr Res. 2021;143:602–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Herbert BM, Herbert C, Pollatos O. On the relationship between interoceptive awareness and alexithymia: is interoceptive awareness related to emotional awareness? J Pers. 2011;79(5):1149–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Shah P, Hall R, Catmur C, Bird G. Alexithymia, not autism, is associated with impaired interoception. Cortex. 2016;81:215–20.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Brewer R, Cook R, Bird G. Alexithymia: a general deficit of interoception. R Soc Open Sci. 2016;3(10):150664.

    Article  ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Murphy J, Brewer R, Plans D, Khalsa SS, Catmur C, Bird G. Testing the independence of self-reported interoceptive accuracy and attention. Q J Exp Psychol. 2019;73.

  72. Gabriele E, Spooner R, Brewer R, Murphy J. Dissociations between self-reported interoceptive accuracy and attention: evidence from the interoceptive attention scale. Biol Psychol. 2021;168:108243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Moriguchi Y, Decety J, Ohnishi T, Maeda M, Mori T, Nemoto K, et al. Empathy and judging other’s pain: an fMRI study of alexithymia. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17(9):2223–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Van der Velde J, Servaas MN, Goerlich KS, Bruggeman R, Horton P, Costafreda SG, et al. Neural correlates of alexithymia: a meta-analysis of emotion processing studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37(8):1774–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Craig AD. How do you feel-now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(1):59–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Borsci G, Boccardi M, Rossi R, Rossi G, Perez J, Bonetti M, et al. Alexithymia in healthy women: a brain morphology study. J Affect Disord. 2009;114(1–3):208–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Goerlich-Dobre KS, Bruce L, Martens S, Aleman A, Hooker CI. Distinct associations of insula and cingulate volume with the cognitive and affective dimensions of alexithymia. Neuropsychologia. 2014;53:284–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Hobson H, Brewer R, Catmur C, Bird G. The role of language in alexithymia: moving towards a multiroute model of alexithymia. Emot Rev. 2019;11(3):247–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Grynberg D, Chang B, Corneille O, Maurage P, Vermeulen N, Berthoz S, et al. Alexithymia and the processing of emotional facial expressions (EFEs): systematic review, unanswered questions and further perspectives. PLoS One. 2012;7(8):e42429.

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  80. Bird G, Silani G, Brindley R, White S, Frith U, Singer T. Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autism. Brain. 2010;133(5):1515–25.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  81. Cook R, Brewer R, Shah P, Bird G. Alexithymia, not autism, predicts poor recognition of emotional facial expressions. Psychol Sci. 2013;24(5):723–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Bentz M, Jepsen JRM, Pedersen T, Bulik CM, Pedersen L, Pagsberg AK, et al. Impairment of social function in young females with recent-onset anorexia nervosa and recovered individuals. J Adolesc Health. 2017;60(1):23–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Brewer R, Cook R, Cardi V, Treasure J, Bird G. Emotion recognition deficits in eating disorders are explained by co-occurring alexithymia. R Soc Open Sci. 2015;2(1):140382.

    Article  ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  84. Brewer R, Cook R, Cardi V, Treasure J, Catmur C, Bird G. Alexithymia explains increased empathic personal distress in individuals with and without eating disorders. Q J Exp Psychol. 2019;72(7):1827–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  85. Chick CF, Rounds JD, Hill AB, Anderson AK. My body, your emotions: Viscerosomatic modulation of facial expression discrimination. Biol Psychol. 2019;107779.

  86. Georgiou E, Mai S, Fernandez KC, Pollatos O. I see neither your fear, nor your sadness – Interoception in adolescents. Conscious Cogn. 2018;60:52–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Axelsson J, Sundelin T, Olsson MJ, Sorjonen K, Axelsson C, Lasselin J, et al. Identification of acutely sick people and facial cues of sickness. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 1870;2018(285):3–9.

    Google Scholar 

  88. Galvez-Pol A, Antoine S, Li C, Kilner JM. People can identify the likely owner of heartbeats by looking at individuals’ faces. Cortex. 2022;151:176–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Zeskind PS, Lester BM. Acoustic features and auditory perceptions of the cries of newborns with prenatal and perinatal complications. Child Dev. 1978;49:580–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Schuetze P, Zeskind PS. Relations between women’s depressive symptoms and perceptions of infant distress signals varying in pitch. Infancy. 2001;2(4):483–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. LaGasse LL, Neal AR, Lester BM. Assessment of infant cry: acoustic cry analysis and parental perception. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2005;11(1):83–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Foley DC, McCutcheon H. Detecting pain in people with an intellectual disability. Accid Emerg Nurs. 2004;12(4):196–200.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Anand KJS, Craig KD. New perspectives on the definition of pain. Pain. 1996;67(1):3–6.

    Google Scholar 

  94. Arcelus J, Yates A, Whiteley R. Romantic relationships, clinical and sub-clinical eating disorders: a review of the literature. Sex Relatsh Ther. 2012;27(2):147–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  95. Broberg AG, Hjalmers I, Nevonen L. Eating Disorders, Attachment and Normal Controls. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2001;396:381–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  96. O’Shaughnessy R, Dallos R. Attachment research and eating disorders: a review of the literature. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;14(4):559–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Erriu M, Cimino S, Cerniglia L. The role of family relationships in eating disorders in adolescents: a narrative review. Behav Sci (Basel). 2020;10(4).

  98. First MB, Williams JB, Karg RS, Spitzer RL. User’s guide for the SCID-5-CV structured clinical interview for DSM-5® disorders: research version. American Psychiatric Publishing; 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  99. Wechsler D. Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence, second edition. 2nd ed. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  100. Murphy J, Brewer R, Catmur C, Bird G. Interoception and psychopathology: a developmental neuroscience perspective. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2017;23:45–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Mond JM, Hay PJ, Rodgers B, Owen C. Eating disorder examination questionnaire (EDE-Q): norms for young adult women. Behav Res Ther. 2006;44(1):53–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Lovibond S, Lovibond P. Manual for the depression anxiety stress scales. 2nd ed. Sydney: Psychology Foundation; 1995.

    Google Scholar 

  103. Desmedt O, Corneille O, Luminet O, Murphy J, Bird G, Maurage P. Contribution of time estimation and knowledge to heartbeat counting task performance under original and adapted instructions. Biol Psychol. 2020;154:107904.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. Murphy J, Brewer R, Hobson H, Catmur C. Is alexithymia characterised by impaired interoception? Further evidence, the importance of control variables, and the problems with the heartbeat counting task. Biol Psychol. 2018;136:189–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Dunn BD, Stefanovitch I, Evans D, Oliver C, Hawkins A, Dalgleish T. Can you feel the beat? Interoceptive awareness is an interactive function of anxiety- and depression-specific symptom dimensions. Behav Res Ther. 2010;48(11):1133–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  106. Ring C, Brener J. Influence of beliefs about heart rate and actual heart rate on heartbeat counting. Psychophysiology. 1996;33:541–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Mehling WE, Price C, Daubenmier JJ, Acree M, Bartmess E, Stewart A. The multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (MAIA). PLoS One. 2012;7(11):e48230.

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  108. Bagby RM, Parker JDA, Taylor GJ. The twenty-item Toronto alexithymia scale - I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. J Psychosom Res. 1994;38(1):23–32.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  109. Brainard DH. The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis. 1997;10(4):433–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Pelli DG. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis. 1997;10(4):437–42.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  111. Ferentzi E, Bogdány T, Szabolcs Z, Csala B. Multichannel investigation of Interoception: sensitivity is not a generalizable feature. Front Hum Neurosci. 2018;12:223.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  112. Rees C. Childhood attachment. Br J Gen Pract. 2007;57(544):920–2.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  113. Werner NS, Duschek S, Mattern M, Schandry R. The relationship between pain Perception and Interoception. J Psychophysiol. 2009;23(1):35–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  114. Garfinkel SN, Manassei MF, Hamilton-Fletcher G, In Den Bosch Y, Critchley HD, Engels M. Interoceptive dimensions across cardiac and respiratory axes. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2016;371:20160014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  115. Hickman L, Seyedsalehi A, Cook JL, Bird G, Murphy J. The relationship between heartbeat counting and heartbeat discrimination: a meta-analysis. Biol Psychol. 2020;156:107949.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  116. Khalsa S, Rudrauf D, Sandesara C, Olshansky B, Tranel D. Bolus isoproterenol infusions provide a reliable method for assessing interoceptive awareness. Int J Psychophysiol. 2009;72(1):34–45.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  117. Murphy J, Brewer R, Coll M, Plans D, Hall M, Shiu SS, et al. I feel it in my finger: measurement device affects cardiac interoceptive accuracy. Biol Psychol. 2019;148:107765.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  118. Biotti F, Ahmad S, Quinn R, Brewer R. Development and validation of the interoceptive states static images (ISSI) database. Behav Res Methods. 2022;54:1744–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  119. Smink FRE, Van Hoeken D, Hoek HW. Epidemiology of eating disorders: incidence, prevalence and mortality rates. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2012;14(4):406–14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  120. Bartholdy S, Allen K, Hodsoll J, O’Daly OG, Campbell IC, Banaschewski T, et al. Identifying disordered eating behaviours in adolescents: how do parent and adolescent reports differ by sex and age? Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;26(6):691–701.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  121. Striegel-Moore RH, Rosselli F, Perrin N, DeBar L, Wilson GT, May A, et al. Gender difference in the prevalence of eating disorder symptoms. Int J Eat Disord. 2009;42(5):471–4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  122. Hoek HW. Incidence, prevalence and mortality of anorexia nervosa and other eating disorders. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2006;19(4):389–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  123. Striegel-Moore RH, Bulik CM. Risk factors for eating disorders. Am Psychol. 2007;62(3):181–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  124. Hudson JI, Hiripi E, Pope HG, Kessler RC. The prevalence and correlates of eating disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61(3):348–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  125. Prentice F, Murphy J. Sex differences in interoceptive accuracy: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;132:497–518.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  126. Nusser L, Pollatos O, Zimprich D. Age-related effects on interoceptive accuracy, general interoceptive sensibility, and specific interoceptive sensibility. Eur J Heal Psychol. 2020;27(4):154–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  127. Murphy J, Viding E, Bird G. Does atypical interoception following physical change contribute to sex differences in mental illness? Psychol Rev. 2019;126(5):787–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  128. Lavender JM, Brown TA, Murray SB. Men, muscles, and eating disorders: an overview of traditional and muscularity-oriented disordered eating. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2017;19(6):1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  129. Mitchison D, Hay P, Slewa-Younan S, Mond J. The changing demographic profile of eating disorder behaviors in the community. BMC Public Health. 2014;14:943.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the actors who contributed to stimulus production and participants who took part in the study. The authors are also grateful to Beat for their help with participant recruitment and Maki Hasegawa and Naseeba Qureshi for their assistance with data collection.

Funding

RB is supported by the Medical Research Council (MR/S003509/1).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

RB, JM, VC and JT conceptualised the project and designed the study. CG, JM and RB recruited participants, collected data, and analysed results. CG and RB drafted the manuscript and visualised the data. All authors reviewed the manuscript for final publication.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rebecca Brewer.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was approved by the University of East London Research Ethics Committee. All participants gave informed consent to take part, and the study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Consent for publication

All actors depicted in the publication consented for the images to be published. All actors viewed by the participants consented for their stimuli to be used in research studies.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gajperia, C., McBride, J., Treasure, J. et al. Recognition of others’ interoceptive states in those with and without eating disorders. BMC Psychiatry 24, 169 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-024-05615-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-024-05615-4

Keywords