Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Paving the Way for an Evolutionary Social Constructivism

  • Published:
Biological Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The idea has recently taken root that evolutionary theory and social constructivism are less antagonistic than most theorists thought, and we have even seen attempts to integrate constructivist and evolutionary approaches to human thought and behavior. We argue in this article that although the projected integration is possible, indeed valuable, attempts to date have tended to be vague or overly simplistic about the claims of social constructivists. We proceed by examining how to give more precision and substance to the research program of evolutionary social constructivism, a task we accomplish by focusing on the specific selection pressures that may have shaped the psychological and cultural mechanisms leading to social constructions. The benefit of such an integration for social constructivism is to provide a solid foundation in the natural sciences. For evolutionists, evolutionary social constructivism expands the assortment of methods used in studying the interplay between culture and human nature.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adriaens PR (2007) Evolutionary psychiatry and the schizophrenia paradox: A critique. Biology and Philosophy 22: 513–528.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adriaens PR, De Block A (2006) The evolution of a social construction: The case of male homosexuality. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine 49: 570–585.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainsworth J (1996) Categories and culture: On the “rectification of names” in comparative law. Cornell Law Review 82: 19–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atran S (1990) Cognitive Foundations of Natural History: Toward an Anthropology of Science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Axelrod R, Hammond R, Grafen A (2004) Altruism via kin-selection strategies that rely on arbitrary tags with which they coevolve. Evolution 58: 1833–1838.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkow JH (2006) Introduction: Sometimes the bus does wait. In: Missing the Revolution: Darwinism for Social Scientists (Barkow JH, ed), 3–59. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett HC (2001) On the functional origins of essentialism. Mind and Society 3: 1–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baudrillard J (1996) The System of Objects. London: Verso.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauman Z (1999) Culture as Praxis. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger PL, Luckmann T (1971) The Social Construction of Reality: A Treatise in the Sociology of Knowledge. Harmondsworth: Penguin. Orig. 1966.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1985) Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown SD, Pujol J, Curt BC (1998) “As one in a web”: Discourse, materiality and the place of ethics. In: Social Constructionism, Discourse and Realism (Parker I, ed), 75–89. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner J (1990) Acts of Meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner J (1991) The narrative construction of reality. Critical Inquiry 18: 1–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bunge M (1996) Finding Philosophy in Social Science. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burr V (1998) An Introduction to Social Constructionism. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss D (1991) Evolutionary personality psychology. Annual Review of Psychology 42: 459–491.

    Google Scholar 

  • Callero P (2003) The sociology of the self. Annual Review of Sociology 29: 115–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll J (2004) Literary Darwinism: Evolution, Human Nature, and Literature. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers P (2006) The Architecture of the Mind: Massive Modularity and the Flexibility of Thought. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caterina R (2004) Comparative law and the cognitive revolution. Tulane Law Review 78: 1501–1547.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheah P (1996) Mattering. Diacritics 26: 108–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collier A (1998) Mind, reality and politics. Radical Philosophy 88: 38–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronk L (1999) That Complex Whole: Culture and the Evolution of Human Behavior. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronk L, Chagnon NA, Irons W, eds (2000) Adaptation and Human Behavior: An Anthropological Perspective. Hawthorne, NY: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curran V (1998) Dealing in difference: Comparative law’s potential for broadening legal perspectives. American Journal of Comparative Law 46: 657–668.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dall S, Houston A, McNamara JM (2004) The behavioural ecology of personality: Consistent individual differences from an adaptive perspective. Ecology Letters 7: 734–739.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Cruz H, De Smedt, J (2007) The role of intuitive ontologies in scientific understanding—the case of human evolution. Biology and Philosophy 22: 351–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett D (1988) Why everyone is a novelist. Times Literary Supplement 4459 (16–22 Sept.): 1028–1029.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett D (1995) Darwin’s Dangerous Idea. New York: Simon and Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett D (2003) Freedom Evolves. London: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dickins T (2004) Social constructionism as cognitive science. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 34: 333–352.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dobzhansky T (1973) Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution. American Biology Teacher 35: 125–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fireman G, McVay T, Flanagan O (2003) Introduction. In: Narrative and Consciousness: Literature, Psychology, and the Brain (Fireman G, McVay T, Flanagan O, eds), 3–13. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor J (1983) The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foucault M (2003) The Birth of the Clinic: An Archeology of Medical Perception. London: Routledge. French orig. 1963.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman S (2004) The Essential Child. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergen KJ (1985) The social constructionist movement in modern psychology. American Psychologist 40: 266–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergen KJ, Gergen M (1983) Narratives of the self. In: Studies in Social Identity (Sarbin TR, Scheibe K, eds), 254–273. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giddens A (1984) The Constitution of Society: Outline of the Theory of Structuration. Cambridge, MA: Polity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gintis H (2004) Towards the unity of the human behavioral sciences. Politics, Philosophy, and Economics 3: 37–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman E (1968) Asylums. Harmondsworth: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gross PR, Levitt N (1994) Higher Superstition: The Academic Left and Its Quarrels With Science. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guarnieri P (1998) “Dangerous girls,” family secrets, and incest law in Italy, 1861–1930. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 21: 369–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hacking I (1995) Rewriting the Soul: Multiple Personality and the Sciences of Memory. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hacking I (1998) Mad Travellers: Reflections on the Reality of Transient Mental Illnesses. London: Free Association Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hacking I (1999) The Social Construction of What? Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanson N (1958) Patterns of Discovery. London: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haraway D (1989) Primate Visions: Gender, Race, and Nature in the World of Modern Science. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harré R (2002) Public sources of the personal mind: Social constructionism in context. Theory and Psychology 12: 611–623.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haslam N, Levy S (2006) Essentialist beliefs about homosexuality: Structure and implications forprejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 32: 471–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Gil-White FJ (2001) The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred status as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior 22: 165–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hibberd F (2005) Unfolding Social Constructionism. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinchman LP, Hinchman SK (1997) Introduction. In: Memory, Identity, Community: The Idea of Narrative in the Human Sciences (Hinchman LP, Hinchman SK, eds), xiii–xxxii. Albany: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirschfeld LA (1996) Race in the Making: Cognition, Culture, and the Child’s Construction of Human Kinds. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones OD (2001) Proprioception, non-law, and biolegal history. Florida Law Review 53: 831–874.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanazawa S (2006) Can the social scientists be saved? Should they? [Review of Missing the Revolution: Darwinism for Social Scientists, Barkow JH, ed]. Evolutionary Psychology 4: 102–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller MC, Miller GF (2006) Resolving the paradox of common, harmful, heritable mental disorders: Which evolutionary genetic models work best? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29: 385–452.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitcher P (1985) Vaulting Ambition: Sociobiology and the Quest for Human Nature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruger DJ (2002) The deconstruction of constructivism. American Psychologist 57: 456–457.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn T (1970) The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland KN, Brown GR (2002) Sense and Nonsense: Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behaviour. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour B (2004) Why has critique run out of steam? From matters of fact to matters of concern. Critical Inquiry 30: 225–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour B, Woolgar S (1979) Laboratory Life: The Construction of Scientific Facts. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Legrand P (2001) The return of the repressed: Moving comparative legal studies beyond pleasure. Tulane Law Review 75: 1033–1051.

    Google Scholar 

  • Legrand P (2003) The same and the different. In: Comparative Legal Studies: Traditions and Transitions (Legrand P, Munday R, eds), 240–311. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy N (2004) Evolutionary psychology, human universals and the Standard Social Science Model. Biology and Philosophy 19: 459–472.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman D, Tooby J, Cosmides L (2003) Does morality have a biological basis? An empirical test of the factors governing moral sentiments relating to incest. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 270: 819–826.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman D, Tooby J, Cosmides L (2007) The architecture of human kin detection. Nature 445: 727–731.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald K (1998) Evolution, culture, and the five-factor model. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 29: 119–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mallon R, Stich S (2000) The odd couple: The compatibility of social construction and evolutionary psychology. Philosophy of Science 67: 133–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mameli M (2007) Evolution and psychology in philosophical perspective. In: Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology (Dunbar RIM, Barrett L, eds), 21–34. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McAdams DP, Pals JL (2006) A new big five: Five fundamental principles for an integrative science of personality. American Psychologist 61: 204–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • McElreath R, Boyd R, Richerson PJ (2003) Shared norms can ead to the evolution of ethnic markers. Current Anthropology 44: 122–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Medin DL (1989) Concepts and conceptual structure. American Psychologist 44: 1469–1481.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mithen S (1996) The Prehistory of the Mind. London: Thames and Hudson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson K (2003) Narrative and the emergence of a consciousness of self. In: Narrative and Consciousness: Literature, Psychology, and the Brain (Fireman G, McVay T, Flanagan O, eds), 17–36. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nettle D (2005) An evolutionary approach to the extraversion continuum. Evolution and Human Behavior 26: 363–373.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nettle D (2006) The evolution of personality variation in humans and other animals. American Psychologist 61: 622–631.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker S (2002) The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature. London: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plotkin H (2002) The Imagined World Made Real: Towards a Natural Science of Culture. London: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pulliam HR, Dunford C (1980) Programmed to Learn: An Essay on the Evolution of Culture. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quine WVO (1960) Word and Object. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson PJ, Boyd R (2005) Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robins LN (1993) Vietnam veterans’ rapid recovery from heroin addiction: A fluke or normal expectation? Addiction 88: 1041–1054.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross D (2006) The economic and evolutionary basis of selves. Cognitive Systems Research 7: 246–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruse M (1999) Mystery of Mysteries: Is Evolution a Social Construction? Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell MC (2002) Why do we drink? A history and philosophy of heredity and alcoholism. Bulletin of Science, Technology and Society 22: 48–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scalise Sugiyama M (2001) Narrative theory and function: Why evolution matters. Philosophy and Literature 25: 233–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segerstråle U (2000) Defenders of the Truth: The Battle for Science in the Sociobiology Debate and Beyond. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Serrano AC, Gunzburger DW (1983) An historical perspective of incest. Contemporary Family Therapy 5: 70–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shakespeare T, Erickson M (2000) Different strokes: Beyond biological determinism and social constructionism. In: Alas, Poor Darwin: Arguments Against Evolutionary Psychology (Rose H, Rose S, eds), 190–205. London: Jonathan Cape.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer J (2004) Narrative identity and meaning making across the adult lifespan: An introduction. Journal of Personality 72: 437–459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith EA (2000) Three styles in the evolutionary analysis of human behavior. In: Adaptation and Human Behavior: An Anthropological Perspective (Cronk L, Chagnon N, Irons W, eds), 27–46. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spain D (1987) The Westermarck-Freud incest-theory debate: An evaluation and reformulation. Current Anthropology 28: 623–645.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber D (1996) Explaining Culture: A Naturalistic Approach. London: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny K (2003) Thought in a Hostile World: The Evolution of Human Cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby J, Cosmides L (1992) The psychological foundations of culture. In: The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture (Barkow J, Cosmides L, Tooby J, eds), 19–136. New York:Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Travis CB, ed (2003) Evolution, Gender and Rape. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van den Berghe P (1983) Human inbreeding avoidance: Culture in nature. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6: 91–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson DS (2003) Evolution, morality and human potential. In: Evolutionary Psychology: Alternative Approaches (Scher SJ, Rauscher F, eds), 55–70. Boston: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson DS (2005) Evolutionary social constructivism. In: The Literary Animal: Evolution and the Nature of Narrative (Gotschall J, Wilson DS, eds), 20–37. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson EO (1998) Consilience: The Unity of Knowledge. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi A (1975) Mate selection: A selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology 53: 205–214.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andreas De Block.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

De Block, A., Du Laing, B. Paving the Way for an Evolutionary Social Constructivism. Biol Theory 2, 337–348 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1162/biot.2007.2.4.337

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/biot.2007.2.4.337

Keywords

Navigation